Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-nr4z6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-26T13:25:54.829Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - History, science, and psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2015

John D. Greenwood
Affiliation:
City University of New York
Get access

Summary

In 1877, James Ward and John Venn petitioned the University of Cambridge in England to have experimental psychology introduced as an academic discipline. The University Senate refused to do so on the grounds that it would “insult religion by putting the soul on a pair of scales” (Hearnshaw, 1989, p. 125). In a 1907 paper published in American Medicine, Dr. Duncan Macdougall of Haverhill, Massachusetts, described his attempt to put the soul on a scale (Macdougall, 1907). He persuaded six dying patients to spend their last hours in a special bed that rested on a platform beam scale. By comparing the weight of the individual (plus bed) before and immediately after death, Macdougall estimated the weight of the human soul to be about “three-fourths of an ounce.” He repeated this experiment with fifteen dying dogs, who manifested no weight loss upon expiration, confirming the popular belief that animals have no soul (Roach, 2003).

From the dawn of recorded civilization, humans have not only speculated about the nature and causes of mind and behavior, but have also employed their ingenuity to test these speculations. In the seventh century BCE, the Egyptian king Psamtik I supposed that children with no opportunity to learn a language from other people would spontaneously develop the natural and universal language of humankind, which he presumed to be Egyptian (Hunt, 1994). He tested this hypothesis by having one of his subjects seclude a number of infants and observe which language they first spoke; he was disappointed to learn that they did not speak Egyptian. As the centuries progressed, critical thinkers continued to speculate about the nature and causes of mind and behavior and to subject their theories to empirical test. The process was accelerated by the scientific revolution in Europe in the sixteenth and seventeenth centuries and by the development of experimental physiology and evolutionary theory in the nineteenth century, which promoted the growth of the institutional science of psychology in the late nineteenth and twentieth centuries. The story of this progression, development, and growth is the history of psychology.

Type
Chapter
Information
A Conceptual History of Psychology
Exploring the Tangled Web
, pp. 1 - 19
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alberghina, L. and Westerhoff, H. V. (2008). Systems Biology: Definitions and Perspectives. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Altschule, M. D. (1965). Acedia: its evolution from deadly sin to psychiatric syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 111, 117–119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, J. M. (1913). History of Psychology. New York, NY: Putnam.Google Scholar
Beckerman, A. P. and Petchey, O. (2012). Getting Started with R: An Introduction for Biologists. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, L. and Le Page, A. (1968). Weapons as aggression-eliciting stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 202–207.Google Scholar
Bhaskar, R. (1975). A Realist Theory of Science. London: Leeds Books.Google Scholar
Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviorism: Psychology and the Minds of Animals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1929). A History of Experimental Psychology. New York, NY: Century.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1933). The Physical Dimensions of Consciousness. New York, NY: Century.Google Scholar
Brandt, L. W. (1970). American psychology. American Psychologist, 25, 1091–1093.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brett, G. S. (1912–1921). A History of Psychology. (Vols. 1–3.) London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Buss, A. R. (1975). The emerging field of the sociology of psychological knowledge. American Psychologist, 30, 988–1002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapanis, A. (1967). The relevance of laboratory studies to practical situations. Ergonomics, 10, 557–577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural Psychology: The Once and Future Discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danziger, K. (2013). Psychology and its history. Theory and Psychology, 23, 829–839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doi, T. (1973). The Anatomy of Dependence. Tokyo: Kodansha International.Google Scholar
Findley-Jones, R. (1986). Accidie and melancholia in a clinical context. In Harré, R. (Ed.), The Social Construction of Emotions. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1955). Project for a scientific psychology. In Strachey, J. (Ed. and Trans.), The Standard Edition of the Works of Sigmund Freud. (Vol. 1.) London: Hogarth. (Original work published 1895.)Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1957). The unconscious. In Strachey, J. (Ed. and Trans.), The Standard Edition of the Works of Sigmund Freud. (Vol. 14.) London: Hogarth. (Original work published 1915.)Google Scholar
Furumoto, L. (1989). The new history of psychology. In Cohen, I. S. (Ed.), The G. Stanley Hall Lecture Series. (Vol. 9.) Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Gomez-Lavin, J. (2014). The End of Working Memory. Paper presented at the 106th Meeting of the Southern Society for Philosophy and Psychology, Charleston, SC, February 6–8.
Greenwood, J. D. (1994). The Disappearance of the Social in American Social Psychology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hatfield, G. (1992). Descartes's physiology and its relation to his psychology. In Cottingham, J. (Ed.), The Cambridge Companion to Descartes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hearnshaw, L. S. (1989). The Shaping of Modern Psychology: An Historical Introduction. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Heelas, P. and Lock, A. (Eds.) (1981). Indigenous Psychologies. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, R. W. (2003). Historical origins of schizophrenia: two early madmen and their illness. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 39, 349–363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, M. (1994). The Story of Psychology. New York, NY: Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Jansz, J. and van Drunen, P. (Eds.) (2004). A Social History of Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kimble, G. A. (1989). Psychology from the point of view of a generalist. American Psychologist, 44, 491–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimble, G. A. (1995). Discussant's remarks: from chaos to coherence in psychology. International Newsletter of Uninomic Psychology, 15, 34–38.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (1992). Postscript. In Koch, S. and Leary, D. E. (Eds.), A Century of Psychology as a Science. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, K. M. (1983). Hwa-byung: a Korean culture-bound syndrome?American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 105–107.Google ScholarPubMed
Logan, C. (2002). When scientific knowledge becomes scientific discovery: classical conditioning before Pavlov. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 38, 393–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macdougall, D. (1907). Hypothesis concerning soul substance together with experimental evidence of the existence of such substance. American Medicine, 2, 240–243.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R. and Hamedani, M. G. (2007). Sociocultural psychology: the dynamic interdependence among self systems and social systems. In Kitayama, S. and Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to Authority. New York, NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Min, S. K., Suh, S. Y., and Song, K. J. (2009). Symptoms to use for diagnostic criteria of hwa-byung, an anger syndrome. Psychiatry Investigation, 6, 7–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moghaddam, F. M. (1987). Psychology in three worlds as reflected by the crisis in social psychology and the move toward indigenous third-world psychology. American Psychologist, 42, 912–920.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. and Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 11163–11175.CrossRef
Pawlik, K. and d'Ydewalle, G. (1996). Psychology and the global commons: perspectives of international psychology. American Psychologist, 51, 488–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickren, W.E. and Ruthford, A. (2010). A History of Modern Psychology in Context. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Reed, E. S. (1997). From Soul to Mind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Roach, M. (2003). Stiff: The Curious Lives of Human Cadavers. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Robinson, D. N. (2013). Historiography in psychology: a note on ignorance. Theory and Psychology, 23, 819–828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, M. R. (1984). U.S. psychology and world psychology. American Psychologist, 39, 877–884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenzweig, M. (1992). Psychological science around the world. American Psychologist, 47, 718–722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenzweig, M. (1999). Continuity and change in the development of psychology around the world. American Psychologist, 54, 252–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sexton, V. S. and Hogan, J. D. (Eds.) (1992). International Psychology: Views from Around the World. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1987). Toward a universal law of generalization for psychological science. Science, 237, 1317–1323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1995). Mental universals: towards a twenty-first century science of mind. In Solso, R. L. and Massaro, D. W. (Eds.), The Science of the Mind: 2001 and Beyond. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, R. (1997). The Human Sciences. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Smith, R. (2013). Between Mind and Nature: A History of Psychology. London: Reaktion Books.Google Scholar
Spence, J. T. (1987). Centrifugal verus centripetal trends in psychology: will the center hold?American Psychologist, 42, 1052–1054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staats, A. W. (1983). Psychology's Crisis of Disunity: Philosophy and Method for a Unified Science. New York, NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
Stocking, G. W. (1965). Editorial: On the limits of “presentism” and “historicism” in the historiography of the behavioral sciences. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 1, 211–218.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Strein, P. J. (1997). The American “colonization” of northwest European social psychology after World War II. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 33, 349–363.Google Scholar
Winston, A. S. (1990). Robert Sessions Woodworth and the “Columbia Bible”: how the psychological experiment was redefined. American Journal of Psychology, 103, 391–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winston, A. S. and Blais, D. J. (1996). What counts as an experiment? A transdisciplinary analysis of textbooks, 1930–1970. American Journal of Psychology, 109, 599–616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, W. R. (1987). Professionalization, rationality, and political linkages in twentieth-century psychology. In Ash, M. G. and Woodward, W. R. (Eds.), Psychology in Twentieth Century Thought and Society. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1938). Experimental Psychology. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×