Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-26T02:36:45.522Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Functionalism, behaviorism, and mental testing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2015

John D. Greenwood
Affiliation:
City University of New York
Get access

Summary

In the early decades of the twentieth century, Wundt's students and the indigenous pioneers of American psychology continued to promote and develop their varied conceptions of scientific psychology. Cattell claimed in his 1895 presidential address to the APA that the “wide range of individual interests” of American psychologists demonstrated their “adjustment in a complex environment” (1896, p. 134). Yet these different interests also proved to be forces of division in the following decades, sometimes characterized as the period of the competing “schools” of psychology (Murchison, 1930; Woodworth, 1931).

Titchener's form of structural psychology remained an intellectual force in the first two decades of the twentieth century, but became increasingly isolated at Cornell and was eventually displaced by functionalist and behaviorist psychology. As the twentieth century progressed, psychology in America distanced itself from its philosophical roots, including the notion that scientific psychology should be grounded in the introspective analysis of consciousness. Many psychologists had abandoned introspective methods by the time John B. Watson issued his behaviorist “manifesto” in 1913 (Watson, 1913a). This was partly because of the “imageless thought” debate, but also because they had little use for introspection in their applied educational, industrial, and clinical work. In addition, psychology in America distanced itself from its roots in German physiological psychology. Although courses and demonstration practicals in laboratory methods continued to be employed in the PhD certification of American psychologists as bona fide practitioners of scientific psychology, the “brass instruments” of the new psychology were increasingly appropriated and adapted by educational, industrial, and clinical psychologists as part of their battery of mental and physical aptitude tests.

Many psychologists who came to embrace Watson's (1913a) call for a behavioral science of prediction and control did so because it suited their already well developed applied interests, not because they were convinced by Watson's arguments or rhetoric. These interests were frequently a product of the various institutional and social pressures and opportunities that promoted the development of applied psychology in the first two decades of the twentieth century. Indeed, Watson's own goal of developing a behaviorist psychology had as much to do with his own professional career interests and institutional realities as his avowed arguments against introspection and in favor of a positivistic science of behavior.

Type
Chapter
Information
A Conceptual History of Psychology
Exploring the Tangled Web
, pp. 344 - 409
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, F. H. (1924). Social Psychology. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Allport, F. H. (1933). Institutional Behavior. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press.Google Scholar
Angell, J. R. (1904). Psychology: An Introduction to the Structure and Function of Consciousness. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Angell, J. R. (1907). The province of functional psychology. Psychological Review, 14, 61–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angell, J. R. (1909). The influence of Darwin on psychology. Psychological Review, 16, 152–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angell, J. R. (1913). Behavior as a category of psychology. Psychological Review, 20, 255–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angell, J. R. (1936). James Roland Angell. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. New York, NY: Russell and Russell.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1889). Handbook of Psychology: Vol. 1. Senses and Intellect. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1891). Handbook of Psychology: Vol. 2. Feeling and Will. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1895a). Types of reaction. Psychological Review, 2, 259–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1895b). Mental Development in the Child and the Race. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1896). The “type-theory” of reaction. Mind, 5, 81–90.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1897). Social and Ethical Interpretations in Mental Development. New York, NY: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1901). Dictionary of Pphilosophy and Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and Evolution. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1909). Darwin and the Humanities. Baltimore, MD: Review Publishing.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1911). The Individual and Society. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1913). History of Psychology. New York, NY: Putnam.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1926). Between Two Wars, 1861–1921. London: Stratford.Google Scholar
Bartlett, T. (2014). The search for psychology's lost boy: in 2009 the decades-old mystery of “Little Albert” was finally solved. Or was it? Chronicle of Higher Education, June 2.
Bechterev, V. M. (1913). La psychologie objective [Objective Psychology]. Paris: F. Alcan. (Original work published 1907–1912.)Google Scholar
Bechterev, V. M. (1932). General Principles of Human Reflexology (Murphy, E. and Murphy, W., Trans.) New York, NY: International. (Original work published 1917.)Google Scholar
Beck, H. P. (2011). Finding Little Albert: a seven-year search for psychology's lost boy. The Psychologist, 24, 392–395.Google Scholar
Beck, H. P., Levinson, S., and Irons, G. (2009). Finding Little Albert: a journey to John B. Watson's infant laboratory. American Psychologist, 64, 605–614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, H. P., Standish, C., Vaughen, R., et al. (2014). Pioneering Science: An Early Attempt to Obtain Measures of Female Sexual Response. Paper presented at 106th Annual Meeting of the Southern Society of Philosophy and Psychology, Francis Marion Hotel, Charleston, SC, February 6–8.
Benjamin, L. T. Jr. and Bryant, W. H. M. (1997). A history of popular psychology magazines in America. In Bringmann, W. G., Lück, H. E., Miller, R., and Early, C. E. (Eds.), A Pictorial History of Psychology. Chicago, IL: Quintessence.Google Scholar
Benjamin, L. T. Jr., DeLeon, P. H., Freedheim, D. K., and Vandenbos, G. R. (2003). Psychology as a profession. In Freedheim, D. K. (Ed.), Handbook of Psychology: Vol. 1. History of Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Benjamin, L. T. Jr., Whitaker, J. L., Ramsey, R. M., and Zeve, D.R. (2007). John B. Watson's alleged sex research: an appraisal of the evidence. American Psychologist, 62, 131–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernard, L. L. (1919). The objective method in sociology. American Journal of Sociology, 25, 298–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernard, L. L. (1921). The use of instinct in the social sciences. Psychological Review, 28, 96–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A. (1903). L'étude expérimentale de l'intelligence [Experimental Studies of Intelligence]. Paris: Schleichter.Google Scholar
Binet, A. and Simon, T. (1905a). Sur la necessité d'etablir un diagnostic scientifique des états inférieurs de l'intelligence [On the necessity of establishing a scientific diagnosis of inferior states of intelligence]. L'Année Psychologique, 11, 163–190.Google Scholar
Binet, A. and Simon, T. (1905b). Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux [New methods for the diagnosis of the intellectual level of the abnormal]. L'Année Psychologique, 11, 191–244.Google Scholar
Binet, A. and Simon, T. (1905c). Applications des méthodes nouvelles au diagnostic du niveau intellectuel chez des enfants normaux et abnormaux d'hospice et d'école primaire [Applications of new methods of the diagnosis of intellectual level with normal and abnormal infants in the hospital and primary school]. L'Année Psychologique, 11, 245–336.Google Scholar
Blowers, G. (1998). Was Z. Y. Kuo (Guo Renyuan) a radical behaviorist? Proceedings of the 30th Annual Meeting of Cheiron, University of San Diego, June 18–21.
Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviorism: Psychology and the Minds of Animals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1993). The Story of Psychology: A Thematic History. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1923). Facts and fancies of immigration. New Republic, 245–246.
Brigham, C. C. (1923). A Study of American Intelligence. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. and Allport, G. W. (1940). Fifty years of change in American psychology. Psychological Review, 37, 757–776.Google Scholar
Bryan, W. L. and Harter, N. (1897). Studies in the physiology and psychology of the telegraphic language. Psychological Review, 4, 27–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchner, E. F. (1911). Psychological progress in 1910. Psychological Bulletin, 8, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckley, K. W. (1989). Mechanical Man: John Broadus Watson and the Beginnings of Behaviorism. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Burnham, W. H. (1924). The Normal Mind. New York, NY: Harper.Google Scholar
Carr, H. C. (1925). Psychology: A Study of Mental Activity. New York, NY: Longmans Green.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carr, H. C. (1935). An Introduction to Visual Space Perception. New York, NY: Longmans Green.Google Scholar
Carr, H. C. and Watson, J. B. (1908). Orientation in the white rat. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 27–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1896). Address of the president before the American Psychological Association, 1895. Psychological Review, 3, 134–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1904). The conceptions and methods of psychology. Popular Science Monthly, 66, 176–186.Google Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1922). The First Year of the Psychological Corporation. Report at Annual Meeting of the Psychological Corporation, December 1. Lewis M. Terman Papers, Stanford University Archives.
Cohen, J. (1979). J. B. Watson: The Founder of Behaviorism. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Coon, Deborah J. (1994) “Not a creature of reason”: The alleged impact of Watsonian behaviorism on advertising in the 1920s. In Todd, J.T. and Morris, E.K. (Eds.), Modern Perspectives on John B. Watson and Classical Behaviorism. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
Creelan, P. G. (1974). Watsonian behaviorism and the Calvinist conscience. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 10, 183–189.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1886a). Psychology. New York, NY: Harper and Brothers.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1886b). Soul and body. Bibliotheca Sacra, 43, 239–263.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3, 357–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, J. (1900). Psychology and social practice. Psychological Review, 7, 105–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, J. (1917). The need for social psychology. Psychological Review, 24, 266–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Digdon, N., Powell, R. A., and Harris, B. (2014). Little Albert's alleged neurological impairment: Watson, Rayner and historical revision. History of Psychology, 17, 312–324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dugdale, R. (1877). The Jukes: A Study in Crime, Pauperism, Disease and Heredity. New York, NY: Putnam.Google Scholar
Dunlap, K. (1912). The case against introspection. Psychological Review, 19, 404–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunlap, K. (1919). Are there any instincts?Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 14, 307–311.Google Scholar
Dunlap, K. (1922). Elements of Scientific Psychology. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esper, E. A. (1966). Max Meyer: the making of a scientific isolate. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 2, 341–356.3.0.CO;2-D>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, F. N. (1922). The mental age of adults. Journal of Educational Research, 6, 441–444.Google Scholar
Fridlund, A. J., Beck, H. P., Goldie, W. D., and Irons, G. (2012). Little Albert: a neurologically impaired child. History of Psychology, 15, 302–327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, A. H. (2000). Contributions of American mental philosophers to psychology in the United States. History of Psychology, 3, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galton, F. (1889). Natural Inheritance. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, H. H. (1911). The Binet–Simon Measuring Scale for Intelligence. Vineland, NJ: Training School.Google Scholar
Goddard, H. H. (1912). The Kallikak Family: A Study in the Heredity of Feeblemindedness. New York, NY: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, H. H. (1919). Psychology of the Normal and Subnormal. New York, NY: Dodd, Mead.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, H. H. (1920). Human Efficiency and Levels of Intelligence. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1985). Carrie Buck's daughter. Natural History, 93, 14–18.Google Scholar
Grant, M. (1916). The Passing of the Great Race. New York, NY: Scribner.Google Scholar
Hamel, I. A. (1919). A study and analysis of the conditioned reflex. Psychological Monographs, 27 (Whole No. 118).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hannush, M. J. (1987). John B. Watson remembered: an interview with James B. Watson. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 23, 137–152.3.0.CO;2-Y>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to little Albert?American Psychologist, 34, 151–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. and Murray, C. (1994). The Bell Curve. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Holmes, O. W. (1927). Buck vs. Bell. 274 US 200.
Hothersall, D. (1995). History of Psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Hunter, W. S. (1922). An open letter to the anti-behaviorists. Journal of Philosophy, 19, 307–308.Google Scholar
Hunter, W. S. (1925). General anthroponomy and its systematic problems. American Journal of Psychology, 36, 286–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunter, W. S. (1928). Human Behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. (Vols. 1–2.) New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
James, W. (1892). A plea for psychology as a “natural science.”Philosophical Review, 1, 146–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jastrow, J. (1928). Keeping Mentally Fit. New York, NY: Garden City Publishing.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and educational achievement?Harvard Educational Review, 39, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, M. C. (1924). A laboratory study of fear: the case of Peter. Pedagogical Seminary, 31, 308–315.Google Scholar
Kamin, L. J. (1974). The Science and Politics of IQ. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Karier, C. J. (1986). Scientists of the Mind. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1925). The Mentality of Apes. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1917.)Google Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1921). Giving up instincts in psychology. Journal of Philosophy, 18, 645–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1924). A psychology without heredity. Psychological Review, 31, 427–448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1930). The genesis of the cat's responses to the rat. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 11, 1–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, C. A. and Sullivan, J. J. (1965). Watson's relation to Titchener. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 1, 338–354.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1916). The human salivary reflex and its use in psychology. Psychological Review, 23, 446–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1923). The behaviorist interpretation of consciousness. Psychological Review, 30, 232–272, 329–353.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lawrence, J. (2000). The Indian Health Service and the sterilization of Native American women. American Indian Quarterly, 24, 400–419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leahey, T. H. (1992). A History of Psychology: Main Currents in Psychological Thought. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Lippmann, W. (1922). The mental age of Americans. New Republic, 32, 213–215.Google Scholar
Logan, C. (1999). The altered rationale for the choice of a standard animal in experimental psychology: Henry H. Donaldson, Adolf Meyer, and “the” albino rat. History of Psychology, 2, 3–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal of Ornithology, 83, 137–213, 289–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackenzie, B. D. (1977). Behaviourism and the Limits of Scientific Method. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Manicas, P. T. (1987). A History and Philosophy of the Social Sciences. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mateer, F. (1918). Child Behavior: A Critical and Experimental Study of Children by the Method of Conditioned Reflexes. Boston, MA: Badger.Google Scholar
McConnell, J. V. (1974). Understanding Human Behavior: An Introduction to Psychology. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1908). Introduction to Social Psychology. London: Methuen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDougall, W. (1912). The Science of Human Behavior. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1920). The Group Mind. New York, NY: Putnam.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1921). Is America Safe for Democracy?New York, NY: Scribner.Google Scholar
Merriam, C. E. (1921). The present state of the study of politics. American Political Science Review, 15, 183–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, M. (1911). The Fundamental Laws of Human Behavior. Boston, MA: Badger.Google Scholar
Meyer, M. (1921). The Psychology of the Other One. Columbia, MO: Missouri Book Company.Google Scholar
Mills, J. A. (1998). Control: A History of Behavioral Psychology. New York, NY: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Mills, W. (1899). The nature of animal intelligence. Psychological Review, 6, 262–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, W. C. (1925). Quantitative analysis in economic theory. American Economic Review, 15, 1–12.Google Scholar
Morgan, C. L. (1898). Review of Animal Intelligence by E. L. Thorndike. Nature, 58, 249–250.Google Scholar
Morgan, C. L. (1977). Introduction to comparative psychology. In Robinson, D. N. (Ed.), Significant Contributions to the History of Psychology, 1750–1920: Series D. Comparative Psychology. (Vol. 2.) Washington, DC: University Publications of America. (Original work published 1894.)Google Scholar
Murchison, C. (1930). Psychologies of 1930. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Napoli, D. S. (1981). Architects of Adjustment: The History of the Psychological Profession in the United States. Port Washington, NY: Kennikat Press.Google Scholar
National Research Council. (1920). Fourth Annual Report. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
O'Donnell, J. M. (1985). The Origins of Behaviorism, American Psychology, 1870–1920. New York, NY: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Passmore, N. (1978). The army intelligence tests and Walter Lippmann. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 14, 316–327.Google Scholar
Pauly, P. J. (1979). Psychology at Johns Hopkins: its rise and fall and rise and fall and…Johns Hopkins Magazine, 30, 36–41.Google Scholar
Pavlov, I. (1928). Conditioned Reflexes. (Vol. 1.) (Gantt, W. H. and Volborth, G., Trans.) New York, NY: International.Google Scholar
Pillsbury, W. B. (1911). Essentials of Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Powell, R. A., Digdon, N., Harris, B., and Smithson, C. (2014). Correcting the record on Watson, Rayner and Little Albert: Albert Barger as “Psychology's lost boy.”American Psychologist, 69, 600–611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Report of Committee of Clinical Section of the APA. (1935). Psychological Clinic, 23, 1–140.
Rodgers, J. E. (2001). Sex: A Natural History. New York, NY: Times Books.Google Scholar
Rosenweig, M. R. (1959). Salivary conditioning before Pavlov. American Journal of Psychology, 72, 628–633.Google Scholar
Routh, D. K. and Reisman, J. M. (2003). Clinical psychology. In Freedheim, D. K. (Ed.), Handbook of Psychology: Vol. 1. History of Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rudolph, F. (1962). The American College and University. New York, NY: Knopf.Google Scholar
Samelson, F. (1977). World War I intelligence testing and the development of psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 13, 274–282.3.0.CO;2-K>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samelson, F. (1981). Struggle for scientific authority: the reception of Watson's behaviorism, 1913–1920. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 17, 399–425.3.0.CO;2-2>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samelson, F. (1985). Organizing for the kingdom of behavior: academic battles and organizational policies in the twenties. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 21, 33–47.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samelson, F. (1992). Rescuing the reputation of Sir Cyril. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 28, 221–233.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shook, J. R. (1995). Wilhelm Wundt's contribution to John Dewey's functional psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 31, 347–369.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, B. (1957). Psychology in the Soviet Union. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1963). Behaviorism at fifty. Science, 140, 951–958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, M. B. (1989). Comment on the case of William McDougall. American Psychologist, 44, 13–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. and Guthrie, E. (1921). Chapters in General Psychology. Seattle, WA: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Sohn, D. (1976). Two concepts of adaptation: Darwin's and psychology's. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 12, 367–375.3.0.CO;2-E>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokal, M. M. (1982). James McKeen Cattell and the failure of anthropometric mental testing, 1890–1901. In Woodward, W. R. and Ash, M. G. (Eds.), The Problematic Science: Psychology in Nineteenth-Century Thought. New York, NY: Praeger.Google Scholar
Sokal, M. M. (1984). James McKeen Cattell and American psychology in the 1920s. In Brozek, J. (Ed.), Explorations in the History of Psychology in the United States. Lewisburg, PA: Bucknell University Press.Google Scholar
Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stam, H. and Kalmanovitch, T. (1988). E. L. Thorndike and the origins of animal psychology. American Psychologist, 53, 1135–1144.Google Scholar
Stern, W. (1914). Die psychologischen Methoden der Intelligenzprüfung: und deren Anwendung an Schulkindren [The psychological methods of testing intelligence]. Educational Psychology Monographs, No. 13. (Whipple, G. M., Trans.) (Original work published in German in 1912.)
Terman, L. M. (1906). Genius and stupidity. Pedagogical Seminary, 13, 307–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terman, L. M. (1916). The Measurement of Intelligence. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terman, L. M. (1924). The mental test as a psychological method. Psychological Review, 31, 93–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: an experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 2(8).Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1903a). Educational Psychology. New York, NY: Teachers College.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1903b). Heredity, Correlation and Sex Differences in School Abilities. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1904). An Introduction to the Theory of Mental and Social Measurements. New York, NY: Science Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal Intelligence. New York, NY: Hafner.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1913–1914). Educational Psychology. (Vols. 1–3.). New York, NY: Teachers College.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1929). Human Learning. New York, NY: Appleton.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1936). Edward Lee Thorndike. In Murchison, C. A. (Ed.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. (Vol. 3.) Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1895a). Simple reactions. Mind, 4, 74–81.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1895b). The “type-theory” of simple reaction. Mind, 4, 506–514.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1896). The “type-theory” of simple reaction. Mind, 5, 236–241.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1898). The postulates of a structural psychology. Philosophical Review, 7, 449–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1899). Structural and functional psychology. Philosophical Review, 8, 290–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1905). Review of E. L. Thorndike, Elements of Psychology. Mind, 20, 552–554.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1914). On “Psychology as the behaviorist views it.”Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 53, 1–17.Google Scholar
Todd, J. (1994). What psychology has to say about John B. Watson. In Todd, J. and Morris, E. K. (Eds.), Modern Perspectives on John B. Watson and Classical Behaviorism. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
Todes, D. P. (1997). From the machine to the ghost within: Pavlov's transition from digestive physiology to conditional reflexes. American Psychologist, 52, 947–955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1951). Operational behaviorism and current trends in psychology. In Collected Papers in Psychology. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. (Original work published 1936.)Google Scholar
Wallace, A. R. (1864). The origin of human races and the antiquity of man deduced from “the theory of natural selection.”Anthropological Review, 2, 158–187.Google Scholar
Ward, M. (1987). Poor Women, Powerful Men: America's Great Experiment in Family Planning. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
Warren, H. C. (1914). The mental and the physical. Psychological Review, 21, 79–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1903). Animal Education. PhD dissertation, University of Chicago, IL.
Watson, J. B. (1907). Kinaesthetic and organic sensations: their role in the reactions of the white rat to the maze. Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 8(33).Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1908). The behavior of noddy and sooty terns. Publications of the Carnegie Institution, 2, 187–255.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1909). A point of view in comparative psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 6, 57–58.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913a). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913b). Image and affection in behavior. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 10, 421–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1914). Behavior: An Introduction to Comparative Psychology. New York, NY: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1916). The place of the conditioned reflex in psychology. Psychological Review, 23, 89–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the Standpoint of a Behaviorist. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1920). Is thinking merely the action of language mechanisms?British Journal of Psychology, 11, 87–104.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism. (edn.) New York, NY: Norton. (Original work published 1924.)Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1936). John Broadus Watson. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. New York, NY: Russell and Russell.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. and Lashley, K. S. (1915). Homing and Related Activities of Birds. (Vol. 7.) Washington, DC: Carnegie Institution, Department of Marine Biology.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. and McDougall, W. (1929). The Battle of Behaviorism. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. and Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. and Watson, R. R. (1928). The Psychological Care of Infant and Child. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Watson, R. R. (1930). I am the mother of a behaviorist's sons. Parents Magazine, Dec., 16–18.
Weiss, A. P. (1917). Relation between functional and behavior psychology. Psychological Review, 24, 353–368.Google Scholar
Weiss, A. P. (1918). Conscious behavior. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 15, 631–641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, A. P. (1925). A Theoretical Basis of Human Behavior. Columbus, OH: Adams.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiggam, A. E. (1928). Exploring Your Mind with the Psychologists. New York, NY: Bobbs- Merrill.Google Scholar
Wissler, C. (1901). The correlation of mental and physical tests. Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements, 3(6).Google Scholar
Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by Reciprocal Inhibition. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Wolpe, J. (1973). The Practice of Behavior Therapy. (edn.) New York, NY: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1931). Contemporary Schools of Psychology. New York, NY: Ronald Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wozniak, R.H. and Santiago-Blay, J. A. (2013). Trouble at Tyson Alley: James Mark Baldwin's arrest in a Baltimore bordello. History of Psychology, 16, 227–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1905a). Animal psychology and the criterion of the psychic. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 2, 141–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1905b). Review of E. Claparede, La psychologie comparé est-ille légitime? [Is comparative psychology legitimate?]. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 2, 527–528.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1911). Introduction to Psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (Ed.) (1921). Psychological examining in the United States Army. Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences, 15, 1–890.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (1923). Introduction to C. C. Brigham'sA Study of American Intelligence. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Yerkes, R. M., Bridges, J. W., and Hardwick, R. S. (1915). A Point Scale for Measuring Mental Ability. Baltimore, MD: Warwick and York.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yerkes, R., M. and Morgulis, S. (1909). The method of Pavlov in animal psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 6, 257–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. and Yerkes, A. W. (1929). The Great Apes: A Study of Anthropoid Life. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Zenderland, L. (1998). Measuring Minds: Henry Herbert Goddard and the Origins of American Intelligence Testing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×