Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-22T11:01:23.493Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Neobehaviorism, radical behaviorism, and problems of behaviorism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2015

John D. Greenwood
Affiliation:
City University of New York
Get access

Summary

While many psychologists accepted Watson's rhetoric of prediction and control, few accepted the theoretical details of his behaviorist system, and in the 1930s and 1940s, the neobehaviorism of Clark L. Hull (1884–1952) and Edward C. Tolman (1866–1959) superseded Watson's positivist brand of behaviorism. Hull and Tolman followed Watson in maintaining that scientific psychology should be directed to the explanation, prediction, and control of observable behavior rather than introspected mental states, and they rejected the form of structural psychology championed by Titchener and his followers. Yet in contrast to Watson, they recognized the legitimacy of theoretical explanations of observable behavior in terms of the internal states of organisms, including their mental states, on a par with theoretical explanations of the observable properties of physical elements in terms of their internal composition and structure (such as the explanation of the properties of carbon in terms of its molecular composition and structure).

The neobehaviorist attempt to approximate the theoretical orientation of the natural sciences more closely marked an advance over Watson's restriction of behaviorist psychology to the description of observable stimulus–response sequences. However, in their attempt to model behaviorist psychology upon the natural sciences, neobehaviorists did not look to the actual practice of natural sciences such as physics and chemistry, but adopted the equally restrictive logical positivist account of theory advanced by philosophers of science in the early decades of the twentieth century. As Sigmund Koch put it:

In pursuit of these ends, psychology did not go directly to physics, but turned instead for its directives to middlemen. These were, for the most part, philosophers of science (especially logical positivists) and a number of physical scientists who had been codifying a synoptic view of the nature of science and who, by the early thirties, were actively exporting that view from their specialties in the scholarly community at large.(Koch, 1964, p. 10)

The logical positivist account of theory exercised a debilitating influence on the development of neobehaviorist theory. Burrhus F. Skinner 411(1904–1990) exploited the limitations of this account of theory and developed a radical behaviorism that eschewed theories about the internal states of organisms.

Type
Chapter
Information
A Conceptual History of Psychology
Exploring the Tangled Web
, pp. 410 - 453
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amundson, R. (1985). Psychology and epistemology: The place versus response controversy. Cognition, 20, 127–153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
APA Lifetime Award. (1990). Citation for Outstanding Lifetime Contribution to Psychology: Presented to B. F. Skinner. American Psychologist, 45, 1205.CrossRef
Beach, F. A. (1950). The snark was a boojum. American Psychologist, 5, 115–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beach, F. A. (1960). Experimental investigations of species-specific behavior. American Psychologist, 15, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechtel, W. (1988). Connectionism and rules and representation systems: are they compatible?Philosophical Psychology, 1, 5–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamin, L. T. (1986). Why don't they understand us? A history of psychology's public image. American Psychologist, 41, 941–946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjamin, L. T. Jr., DeLean, P. H., Freedheim, D. K., and Vandenbos, G. R. (2003). Psychology as a profession. In Freedheim, D. K. (Ed.), Handbook of Psychology: Vol. 1. History of Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bergmann, G. and Spence, K. W. (1941). Operationism and theory in psychology. Psychological Review, 48, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviorism: Psychology and the Minds of Animals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1993). The Story of Psychology: A Thematic History. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks-Cole.Google Scholar
Breland, K. and Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 16, 681–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridgman, P. (1927). The Logic of Modern Physics. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1980). Jerome S. Bruner. In Lindzey, G. (Ed.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. (Vol. 7.) San Francisco, CA: Freeman.Google Scholar
Capshew, J. H. (1999). Psychologists on the March: Science, Practice, and Professional Identity in America, 1929–1969. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carnap, R. (1936). Testability and meaning. Philosophy of Science, 3, 419–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carnap, R. (1937). Testability and meaning. Philosophy of Science, 4, 1–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, D. (1949). Some principles of mass persuasion: selected findings of research on the sale of U. S. War bonds. Human Relations, 2, 253–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of a Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1966). Cartesian Linguistics. New York, NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1972). Language and Mind. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and the Problem of Knowledge. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dashiell, J. F. (1928). Fundamentals of Objective Psychology. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollard, J. and Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and Psychotherapy. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Dulany, D. E. (1968). Awareness, rules, and propositional control: a confrontation with S–R behavior theory. In Dixon, T. R. and Horton, D. C. (Eds.), Verbal Behavior and General Behavior Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Estes, W. K., Koch, S., MacCorquodale, K., et al. (Eds.) (1954). Modern Learning Theory. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Ferster, C. S. and Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of Reinforcement. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1965). Could meaning be an rm?Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 4, 73–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J. and Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J., McGowan, B. K., and Green, K. F. (1972). Constraints on conditioning. In Seligman, M. E. P. and Hager, J. L. (Eds.), Biological Boundaries of Learning. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1985). The Mind's New Science: A History of the Cognitive Revolution. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Green, C. D. (1992). Of immortal mythological beasts: operationism in psychology. Theory and Psychology, 2, 291–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenspoon, J. (1955). The reinforcing effect of two spoken sounds on the frequency of two behaviors. American Journal of Psychology, 68, 409–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hempel, C. G. (1965). Aspects of Scientific Explanation. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
Hilgard, E. R. (1987). Psychology in America: A Historical Survey. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Hovland, C. I., Lumsdaine, A. A., and Sheffield, F. D. (Eds.) (1949). Experiments on Mass Communication. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1920). Quantitative aspects of the evolution of concepts: an experimental approach. Psychological Monographs, 28 (Whole No. 123).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1928). Aptitude Testing. Yonkers-on Hudson, NY: World Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1930). Knowledge and purpose as habit mechanisms. Psychological Review, 36, 511–525.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1933). Hypnosis and Suggestibility: An Experimental Approach. New York, NY: Appleton–Century.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1937). Mind, mechanism and adaptive behavior. Psychological Review, 44, 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943a). Principles of Behavior. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943b). The problem of intervening variables in molar behavior theory. Psychological Review, 50, 273–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1952). Clark Leonard Hull. In Boring, E. G. (Ed.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. (Vol. 4.) New York, NY: Russell and Russell.Google Scholar
Huxley, A. (1932). Brave New World. London: Chatto and Windus.Google Scholar
Kamin, L. J. (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In Campbell, B. A. and Church, R. M. (Eds.), Punishment and Aversive Behavior. New York, NY: Appleton.Google Scholar
Kendler, H. H. (1952). What is learned? – a theoretical blind alley. Psychological Review, 59, 269–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, H. H. (1981). Psychology: A Science in Conflict. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (1962). Behaviorism. Encyclopedia Britannica.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (1964). Psychology and emerging conceptions of science as unitary. In Wann, T. W. (Ed.), Behaviorism and Phenomenology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Koch, S. (1992). Psychology's Bridgman vs. Bridgman's Bridgman. Theory and Psychology, 2, 261–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krantz, D. L. (1972). The mutual isolation of operant and non-operant psychology as a case study. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 8, 86–102.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krechevsky, I. (1932). Hypotheses in rats. Psychological Review, 39, 516–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1923). The behaviorist interpretation of consciousness. Psychological Review, 30, 232–272, 329–353.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In Jeffress, L. A. (Ed.), Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior: The Hixon Symposium. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Leahey, T. H. (1992). A History of Psychology. (edn.) Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Lewin, K. (1947). Group decision and social change. In Newcomb, T. M. and Hartley, E. L. (Eds.), Readings in Social Psychology. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1950). Innate behaviour patterns. In Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology: No. 4. Physiological Mechanisms in Animal Behaviour. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Lubek, I. and Apfelbaum, E. (1987). Neo-behaviorism and the Garcia effect: a social psychology of science approach to the history of a paradigm clash. In Ash, M. G. and Woodward, W. R. (Eds.), Psychology in Twentieth-Century Thought and Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Luongo, A.F. (1976). Stimulus selection in discriminative taste-aversion learning in the rat. Animal Learning Behavior, 4, 225–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacCorquodale, K. and Meehl, P. E. (1948). On a distinction between hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. Psychological Review, 55, 95–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. J. (1978). Conditioning. In Foss, B. M. (Ed.), Psychology Survey No 1. London: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Marx, M. H. (1951). Intervening variable or hypothetical construct?Psychological Review, 58, 235–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCulloch, W. S. (1951). Why the mind is in the head. In Jeffress, L. A. (Ed.), Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior: The Hixon Symposium. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1908). Introduction to Social Psychology. London: Methuen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1989). George A. Miller. In Lindzey, G. (Ed.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. (Vol. 8.) Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1959). Liberalization of basic S–R concepts. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: Study of a Science. (Vol. 2.) New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Napoli, D. S. (1981). Architects of Adjustment: The History of the Psychological Profession in the United States. Port Washington, NY: Kennikat Press.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. E. (1957). A behaviorist analysis of perception and language as cognitive phenomena. In Gruber, H. E. (Ed.), Contemporary Approaches to Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pratt, C. C. (1939). The Logic of Modern Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1973). Effect of US habituation following conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 137–143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. (1978). Some implications of a cognitive perspective in Pavlovian conditioning. In Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., and Honig, W. K. (Eds.), Cognitive Processes in Animal Behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. and Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. and Prokasy, W. F. (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Revuski, S. (1971). The role of interference in association over a delay. In Honig, W. K. and James, P. H. R. (Eds.), Animal Memory. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Riley, J. W. Jr. (1947). Opinion research in liberated Normandy. American Sociological Review, 12, 698–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B. and Whitehead, A. N. (1910). Principia Mathematica. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rutherford, A. (2009). Beyond the Box: B. F. Skinner's Technology of Behavior from Laboratory to Life, 1950s–1970s. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Schlick, M. (1936). Meaning and verification. Philosophical Review, 45, 339–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, D. P. and Schultz, S. E. (1992). A History of Modern Psychology. (edn.) New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Sears, R. R. (1943). Survey of Objective Studies of Psychoanalytic Concepts. (Bulletin 51.) New York, NY: Social Science Research Council.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 77, 406–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1945). Baby in a box. Ladies’ Home Journal. October.
Skinner, B. F. (1948). Walden Two. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary?Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1951). How to teach animals. Scientific American, 185, 26–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and Human Behavior. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1960). Pigeons in a pelican. American Psychologist, 15, 28–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1961). Cumulative Record. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1967). B. F. Skinner. In Boring, Edwin G. and Lindzey, Gardner (Eds.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. (Vol. 5.) New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond Freedom and Dignity. New York, NY: Knopf.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1974). About Behaviorism. New York, NY: Knopf.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1985). Cognitive science and behaviorism. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 291–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1987). Upon Further Reflection. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1990). Can psychology be a science of the mind?American Psychologist, 45, 1206–1210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. and Vaughan, M. E. (1983). Enjoy Old Age: Living Fully in Your Later Years. New York, NY: Warner.Google Scholar
Smith, L. D. (1986). Behaviorism and Logical Positivism: A Revised Account of the Alliance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1936). The nature of discrimination learning in animals. Psychological Review, 43, 427–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1952). Clark Leonard Hull: 1884–1952. American Journal of Psychology, 65, 639–646.Google ScholarPubMed
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior Theory and Conditioning. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, K. W. (1960). Behavior Theory and Learning: Selected Papers. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1935). The operational definition of psychological concepts. Psychological Review, 42, 517–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stouffer, S. A., Lumsdane, A. A., Lumsdane, M. H., et al. (Eds.) (1949). The American Soldier: Combat and its Aftermath. (Studies in Social Psychology in World War II, No 2.) Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Stouffer, S. A., Suchman, E. A., De Vinney, L. C., Star, S. A., and Williams, R. B. Jr. (Eds.) (1949). The American Soldier: Adjustment during Army Life. (Studies in Social Psychology in World War II, No 1.) Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Thoreau, H. D. (1854). Walden. Boston, MA: Ticknor and Fields.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1951). The Study of Instinct. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1917). Retroactive inhibition as affected by conditions of learning. Psychological Monographs, 25 (Whole No. 107).Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1922). A new formula for behaviorism. Psychological Review, 29, 44–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1923). A behaviorist account of the emotions. Psychological Review, 30, 217–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1925). Behaviorism and purpose. Journal of Philosophy, 22, 36–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1926). A behaviorist theory of ideas. Psychological Review, 33, 352–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1927). A behaviorist's definition of consciousness. Psychological Review, 34, 433–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1928). Purposive behavior. Psychological Review, 35, 524–530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men. New York, NY: Century.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1938). The determiners of behavior at a choice-point. Psychological Review, 45, 1–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1942). Drives Towards War. New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1945). A stimulus-expectancy need-cathexis psychology. Science, 101, 160–166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189–209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolman, E. C. (1949). Discussion. Journal of Personality, 18, 48–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1951). Operational behaviorism and current trends in psychology. In Collected Papers in Psychology. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. (Original work published 1936.)Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1959). Principles of purposive behaviorism. In Koch, S. (Ed.), Psychology: A Study of a Science. (Vol. 2.) New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. and Honzik, C. H. (1930). Introduction and removal of reward, and maze performance in rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 257–273.Google Scholar
U. S. Strategic Bombing Survey. (1946). The Effects of Bombing upon German Morale. Washington, DC: U. S. Government Printing Office.
von Neumann, J. (1951). The general and logical theory of automata. In Jeffress, L. A. (Ed.), Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior: The Hixon Symposium. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Watson, G. (Ed.) (1942). Civilian Morale. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1914). Behavior: An Introduction to Comparative Psychology. New York, NY: HoltCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism. (edn.) New York, NY: Norton. (Original work published 1924.)Google Scholar
Wolfle, D. (1997). The reorganized American Psychological Association. American Psychologist, 52, 721–724. (Original work published 1946)CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×