Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T22:54:20.668Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Processing Grammatical Differences: Perceiving versus Noticing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2016

Anna M. Babel
Affiliation:
Ohio State University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baayen, R. H. 2010. languageR: Data sets and functions with “Analyzing Linguistic Data: A practical introduction to statistics.” R package version 1.0. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=languageR.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., and Bolker, B. 2011. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-39. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.Google Scholar
Batterink, L. and Neville, H. J. 2013. The human brain processes syntax in the absence of conscious awareness. Journal of Neuroscience 33(19):8528–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowey, J. A. (1986). Syntactic awareness in relation to reading skill and ongoing comprehension monitoring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 41:282–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breadmore, H. L., Krott, A., and Olson, A. C. 2014. Agreeing to disagree: Deaf and hearing children’s awareness of subject-verb number agreement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 67(3):474–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bresnan, J. and Ford, M. 2010. Predicting syntax: Processing dative constructions in American and Australian varieties of English. Language 86(1):168213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, K. 2009. The nature of sociolinguistic perception. Language Variation and Change 21(1):135–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, K. 2010. Sociolinguistics and perception. Language and Linguistics Compass 4:377–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vicenzi, M., Job, R., Di Matteo, R., Angrilli, A., Penolazzi, B., Ciccarelli, L., and Vespignani, F. 2003. Differences in the perception and time course of syntactic and semantic violations. Brain and Language 85:280–96.Google Scholar
Docherty, G. J., Langstrof, C., and Foulkes, P. 2013. Listener evaluation of sociophonetic variability: Probing constraints and capabilities. Linguistics 51(2):355–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feature 158: Invariant don’t for all persons in the present tense. 2011. in Kortmann, B. and Lunkenheimer, K. (eds.), The Electronic World Atlas of Varieties of English [eWAVE]. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. www.ewave–atlas.org/parameters/158.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. 1993. Less skilled readers have less efficient suppression mechanisms. Psychological Science 1993(4):294–9.Google Scholar
Hanulíková, A., Van Alphen, P. M., Van Goch, M. M., and Weber, A. 2012. When one person’s mistake is another’s standard usage: The effect of foreign accent on syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24(4):878–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnstone, B. and Kiesling, S. 2008. Indexicality and experience: Exploring the meanings of /aw/-monophthongization in Pittsburgh. Journal of Sociolinguistics 12(1):533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Just, M.A., Carpenter, P.A., and Woolley, J. D. 1982. Paradigms and processes in reading comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 111(2):228–38.Google ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, M. P. 2006. What this construction needs is generalized. Memory & Cognition 34(2):368–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, M. P. and Glenberg, A. M. 2004. This construction needs learned. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133:450–67.Google ScholarPubMed
Labov, W. 1972. Sociolinguistic Patterns. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. 1973. Where do grammars stop? in Shuy, R. W. (ed.), Report of the Twenty-Third Annual Round Table Meeting on Linguistics and Language Studies, pp. 4388. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W., Ash, S., Ravindranath, M., Weldon, T., Baranowski, M., and Nagy, N. 2011. Properties of the sociolinguistic monitor. Journal of Sociolinguistics 15(4):431–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loudermilk, B. C. 2013. Cognitive mechanisms in the perception of sociolinguistic variation. PhD dissertation. University of California, Davis.Google Scholar
Mertz, E. and Yovel, J. 2003. Metalinguistic awareness, in Ostman, J.-O., Veschueren, J., Blommaert, J., and Bulcaen, C. (eds.), The Handbook of Pragmatics, pp. 126. Amsterdam and Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Nation, K. and Snowling, M. J. 2000. Factors influencing syntactic awareness skills in normal and poor comprehenders. Applied Psycholinguistics 21:229–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearlmutter, N. J., Garnsey, S. M., and Bock, K. 1999. Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 41:427–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, D. 1996. Whaddayaknow: The modes of folk linguistic awareness. Language Awareness 5:4074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, D. 2011. The power of language regard – discrimination, classification, comprehension, and production. Dialectologia (Special Issue II):9–33.Google Scholar
Schmidt, R. W. 1990. The role of consciousness in second language learning. Applied Linguistics 11:129–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silverstein, M. 1981. The limits of awareness. Texas Working Papers in Linguistics 84:130.Google Scholar
Squires, L. 2013. Limited bidirectionality in sociolinguistic perception. Journal of Sociolinguistics 17(2):200–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squires, L. 2014a. Knowledge, processing, evaluation: Testing the perception of English subject-verb agreement variation. Journal of English Linguistics 42(2):144–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Squires, L. 2014b. Talker specificity and the perception of grammatical variation. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29(7):856–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staum Casasanto, L. 2009. Experimental investigations of sociolinguistic knowledge. PhD dissertation. Stanford University, CA.Google Scholar
Wagoner, S. A. 1983. Comprehension monitoring: What it is and what we know about it. Reading Research Quarterly 18(3):328–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfram, W. 1982. Language knowledge and other dialects. American Speech 57(1):318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×