Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T05:04:20.942Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Camouflage in colour-changing animals

Trade-offs and constraints

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Devi Stuart-Fox
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Adnan Moussalli
Affiliation:
Museum Victoria, Australia
Martin Stevens
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Sami Merilaita
Affiliation:
Åbo Akademi University, Finland
Get access

Summary

Colour change is widespread in ectotherm animals including crustaceans, insects, cephalopods, amphibians, reptiles and fish (Bagnara & Hadley 1973). There are two types of colour change, morphological and physiological, which differ in their mechanism and speed. Morphological colour change occurs due to changes in the density and quality of pigment-containing cells (chromatophores) in the dermis (a layer of the skin) and usually takes place over a timescale of days or months. For instance, a common form of morphological colour change is long-term background or chromatic adaptation, in which the animal's colour changes to more closely resemble that of the background. Long-term background adaptation involves an increase in both the density of melanophores (melanin-containing chromatophores) and melanin pigment within the melanophores (Bagnara & Hadley 1973; Sugimoto 2002). By contrast, physiological colour change occurs due to movement (dispersion or concentration) of pigment within chromatophores and is much more rapid, taking milliseconds to hours (Bagnara & Hadley 1973; Thurman 1988). For example, short-term background adaptation generally involves movement of melanosomes (organelles containing melanin pigment) within melanophores, either becoming concentrated in the middle, resulting in lightening, or becoming dispersed throughout, resulting in darkening. The exception is cephalopods (squid, cuttlefish, octopuses and their relatives), in which colour change occurs due to contraction of the muscle fibres of specialised ‘chromatophore organs’, which comprise the chromatophore itself surrounded by radial muscle fibres and sheath cells (Bagnara & Hadley 1973; Messenger 2001; Hanlon 2007). In cephalopods, colour change is effected by neural control of muscle contraction within chromatophore organs whereas in other animals, movement of pigment-containing organelles within chromatophores is under neural and/or endocrine control (Bagnara & Hadley 1973; Nery & Castrucci 1997). The majority of early experiments using colour-changing animals focussed on the physiological basis of colour change in fish and amphibians maintained on (i.e. adapted to) black-and-white backgrounds (reviewed in Bagnara & Hadley 1973). However, many animals capable of physiological colour change show much more complex responses to changes in their visual environments than simple adaptation to dark and light backgrounds (Figure 13.1). Consequently, recent studies have manipulated specific visual features, habitat characteristics, perceived predation risk and social environment to assess dynamic, adaptive colour responses. These studies have highlighted the diversity of visual cues and interacting selective pressures influencing the colour patterns and camouflage strategies adopted by colour-changing animals.

Many colour-changing taxa appear to rely primarily on background matching (e.g. amphibians, reptiles, flatfish, insects, crustaceans). One possible reason for this is that in species with more limited capacity for colour change, changes in coloration are often primarily restricted to changes in overall dermal reflectance, with little change in pattern (e.g. most crustaceans, amphibians and reptiles). Nevertheless, physiological colour change potentially enables animals to employ more than one camouflage or anti-predator strategy (e.g. background matching, disruptive coloration, countershading, dazzle markings, warning coloration; see Stevens & Merilaita 2009a, b for definitions and discussion). Although the range of colour patterns exhibited is unknown for most colour-changing taxa, there are a few exceptions. For example, juvenile bullethead parrotfish show three principal physiological colour patterns: stripes, a distinct ‘eyespot’ at the base of the tail fin and a ‘uniformly dark’ pattern (Crook 1997). These three patterns may exploit different camouflage or anti-predator mechanisms. Specifically, the stripes may be associated with disruptive camouflage or motion dazzle (stripes: Stevens 2007), the ‘eyespot’ pattern may intimidate predators (‘eyespot’ pattern: Stevens et al. 2008a) and the ‘uniformly dark’ pattern appears to match the background (‘uniformly dark’ pattern: Stevens 2007). However, these different camouflage strategies have not been confirmed experimentally.

Type
Chapter
Information
Animal Camouflage
Mechanisms and Function
, pp. 237 - 253
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aspengren, S., Hedberg, D., Skold, H. N. & Wallin, M. 2009a. New insights into melanosome transport in vertebrate pigement cells. International Review of Cell and Molecular Biology, 272, 246–288.Google Scholar
Aspengren, S., Skold, H. N. & Wallin, M. 2009b. Different strategies for color change. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 66, 187–191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bagnara, J. T. & Hadley, M. E. 1973. Chromatophores and Color Change: The Comparative Physiology of Animal Pigmentation. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Barbosa, A., Mathger, L. M., Chubb, C.et al. 2007. Disruptive coloration in cuttlefish: a visual perception mechanism that regulates ontogenetic adjustment of skin patterning. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 1139–1147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbosa, A., Mathger, L. M., Buresch, K. C.et al. 2008. Cuttlefish camouflage: the effects of substrate contrast and size in evoking uniform, mottle or disruptive body patterns. Vision Research, 48, 1242–1253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohlin, T., Tullberg, B. S. & Merilaita, S. 2008. The effect of signal appearance and distance on detection risk in an aposematic butterfly larva (Parnassius apollo). Animal Behaviour, 76, 577–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camargo, C. R., Visconti, M. A. & Castrucci, A. M. L. 1999. Physiological color change in the bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana. Journal of Experimental Zoology, 283, 160–169.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castrucci, A. M. D., Sherbrooke, W. C. & Zucker, N. 1997. Regulation of physiological color change in dorsal skin of male tree lizards, Urosaurus ornatus. Herpetologica, 53, 405–410.Google Scholar
Chiao, C. C. & Hanlon, R. T. 2001. Cuttlefish camouflage: visual perception of size, contrast and number of white squares on artificial checkerboard substrata initiates disruptive coloration. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204, 2119–2125.Google ScholarPubMed
Chiao, C. C., Kelman, E. J. & Hanlon, R. T. 2005. Disruptive body patterning of cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) requires visual information regarding edges and contrast of objects in natural substrate backgrounds. Biological Bulletin, 208, 7–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, W. E. & Greenberg, N. 1992. Reptilian coloration and behavior. In Biology of the Reptilia, eds. Gans, C. & Crews, D.Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press, pp. 298–422.Google Scholar
Cott, H. B. 1940. Adaptive Coloration in Animals. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Cox, S., Chandler, S., Barron, C. & Work, K. 2009. Benthic fish exhibit more plastic crypsis than non-benthic species in a freshwater spring. Journal of Ethology, 27, 497–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crook, A. C. 1997 Colour patterns in a coral reef fish: is background complexity important? Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 217, 237–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demski, L. S. 1992. Chromatophore systems in teleosts and cephalopods: a levels oriented analysis of convergent systems. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 40, 141–156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dimitrova, M., Stobbe, N., Schaefer, H. M. & Merilaita, S. 2009. Concealed by conspicuousness: distractive prey markings and backgrounds. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 276, 1905–1910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edmunds, M. 1974. Defence in Animals: A Survey of Antipredator Defences. Harlow, UK: Longman.Google Scholar
Endler, J. A. 1978. A predator's view of animal color patterns. In Evolutionary Biology, eds. Hecht, M. K., Steere, W. C. & Wallace, B.New York: Plenum Press, pp. 319–364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Filshie, B. K., Day, M. F. & Mercer, E. H. 1975. Color and color change in the grasshopper, Kosciuscola tristis. Journal of Insect Physiology, 21, 1763–1770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forsman, A. & Appelqvist, S. 1998. Visual predators imposes correlated selection on prey color pattern and behavior. Behavioral Ecology, 9, 409–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, T. S. & Sih, A. 2003. Color change and color-dependent behavior in response to predation risk in the salamander sister species Ambystoma barbouri and Ambystoma texanum. Oecologia, 137, 131–139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadley, M. E. & Goldman, J. M. 1969. Physiological color changes in reptiles. American Zoologist, 9, 489–504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanlon, R. T. 1996. Cephalopod Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hanlon, R. T. 2007. Cephalopod dynamic camouflage. Current Biology, 17, R400–R404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanlon, R. T. & Messenger, J. B. 1988. Adaptive coloration in young cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis): the morphology and development of body patterns and their relation to behavior. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 320, 437–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanlon, R. T., Forsythe, J. W. & Joneschild, D. E. 1999. Crypsis, conspicuousness, mimicry and polyphenism as antipredator defences of foraging octopuses on Indo-Pacific coral reefs, with a method of quantifying crypsis from video tapes. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 66, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hastad, O., Victorsson, J. & Odeen, A. 2005. Differences in color vision make passerines less conspicuous in the eyes of their predators. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 102, 6391–6394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Healey, E. G. 1999. The skin pattern of young plaice and its rapid modification in response to graded changes in background tint and pattern. Journal of Fish Biology, 55, 937–971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemmi, J. M., Marshall, J., Pix, W., Vorobyev, M. & Zeil, J. 2006. The variable colours of the fiddler crab Uca vomeris and their relation to background and predation. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209, 4140–4153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoglund, E., Balm, P. H. M. & Winberg, S. 2002. Behavioural and neuroendocrine effects of environmental background colour and social interaction in Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus). Journal of Experimental Biology, 205, 2535–2543.Google Scholar
Insausti, T. C. & Casas, J. 2008. The functional morphology of color changing in a spider: development of ommochrome pigment granules. Journal of Experimental Biology, 211, 780–789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelman, E. J., Tiptus, P. & Osorio, D. 2006. Juvenile plaice (Pleuronectes platessa) produce camouflage by flexibly combining two separate patterns. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209, 3288–3292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelman, E. J., Baddeley, R. J., Shohet, A. J. & Osorio, D. 2007. Perception of visual texture and the expression of disruptive camouflage by the cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 274, 1369–1375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelman, E. J., Osorio, D. & Baddeley, R. J. 2008. A review of cuttlefish camouflage and object recognition and evidence for depth perception. Journal of Experimental Biology, 211, 1757–1763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Key, K. H. L. & Day, M. F. 1954a. The physiological mechanism of colour change in the grasshopper Kosciuscola tristis Sjost (Orthoptera, Acrididae). Australian Journal of Zoology, 2, 340–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Key, K. H. L. & Day, M. F. 1954b. A temperature-controlled physiological colour change in the grasshopper Kosciuscola tristis Sjost (Orthoptera: Acrididae). Australian Journal of Zoology, 2, 309–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, R. B., Hauff, S. & Phillips, J. B. 1994. Physiological color change in the green treefrog: responses to background brightness and temperature. Copeia, 1994, 422–432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kodric-Brown, A. 1998. Sexual dichromatism and temporary color changes in the reproduction of fishes. American Zoologist, 38, 70–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langridge, K. V. 2006. Symmetrical crypsis and asymmetrical signalling in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 273, 959–967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langridge, K. V., Broom, M. & Osorio, D. 2007. Selective signalling by cuttlefish to predators. Current Biology, 17, R1044–R1045.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, N. J. 2000. Communication and camouflage with the same ‘bright’ colours in reef fishes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 355, 1243–1248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, N. J., Jennings, K., Mcfarland, W. N., Loew, E. R. & Losey, G. S. 2003a. Visual biology of Hawaiian coral reef fishes. II. Colors of Hawaiian coral reef fish. Copeia, 2003, 455–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, N. J., Jennings, K., Mcfarland, W. N., Loew, E. R. & Losey, G. S. 2003b. Visual biology of Hawaiian coral reef fishes. III. Environmental light and an integrated approach to the ecology of reef fish vision. Copeia, 2003, 467–480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mäthger, L. M., Chiao, C. C., Barbosa, A.et al. 2007. Disruptive coloration elicited on controlled natural substrates in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 2657–2666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mäthger, L. M., Chiao, C. C., Barbosa, A. & Hanlon, R. T. 2008. Color matching on natural substrates in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 194, 577–585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merila, J., Laurila, A. & Lindgren, B. 2004. Variation in the degree and costs of adaptive phenotypic plasticity among Rana temporaria populations. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 17, 1132–1140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merilaita, S., Tuomi, J. & Jormalainen, V. 1999. Optimization of cryptic coloration in heterogeneous habitats. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 67, 151–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messenger, J. B. 2001. Cephalopod chromatophores: neurobiology and natural history. Biological Reviews, 76, 473–528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nečas, P. 2001. Chameleons: Nature's Hidden Jewels. Malabar, FL: Krieger Publishing.
Nery, L. E. M. & Castrucci, A. M. D. 1997. Pigment cell signalling for physiological color change. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology A, 118, 1135–1144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, M. D., Finn, J. & Tregenza, T. 2001. Dynamic mimicry in an Indo-Malayan octopus. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 268, 1755–1758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, K. S. 1967. Color adaptation in desert reptiles and its thermal relationships. In Lizard Ecology: A Symposium, ed. Milstead, W. W.Columbia, MO: University of Missouri Press, pp. 162–229.Google Scholar
Olson, V. A. & Owens, I. P. F. 1998. Costly sexual signals: are carotenoids rare, risky or required? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 13, 510–514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulton, E. B. 1890. The Colours of Animals: Their Meaning and Use, Especially Considered in the Case of Insects. London: Kegan Paul, Trench.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., Tyler, C. W., Gregory, R. L.et al. 1996. Rapid adaptive camouflage in tropical flounders. Nature, 379, 815–818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Relyea, R. A. 2002. Costs of phenotypic plasticity. American Naturalist, 159, 272–282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruxton, G. D., Sherratt, T. N. & Speed, M. P. 2004. Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saidel, W. 1978 Analysis of flatfish camouflage. American Zoologist, 18, 579–597.Google Scholar
Silbiger, N. & Munguia, P. 2008. Carapace color change in Uca pugilator as a response to temperature. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 355, 41–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stegen, J. C., Gienger, C. M. & Sun, L. X. 2004. The control of color change in the Pacific tree frog, Hyla regilla. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 82, 889–896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiner, U. K. & Van Buskirk, J. 2008. Environmental stress and the costs of whole-organism phenotypic plasticity in tadpoles. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21, 97–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M. 2007. Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 274, 1457–1464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M. & Merilaita, S. 2009a. Animal camouflage: current issues and new perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 423–427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M. & Merilaita, S. 2009b. Defining disruptive coloration and distinguishing its functions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 481–488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M., Hardman, C. J. & Stubbins, C. L. 2008a. Conspicuousness, not eye mimicry, makes ‘eyespots’ effective antipredator signals. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 525–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, M., Yule, D. H. & Ruxton, G. D. 2008b. Dazzle coloration and prey movement. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 275, 2639–2643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuart-Fox, D. & Moussalli, A. 2008. Selection for social signalling drives the evolution of chameleon colour change. PLoS Biology, 6(1), e25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuart-Fox, D. & Moussalli, A. 2009. Camouflage, communication and thermoregulation: lessons from colour changing organisms. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 463–470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stuart-Fox, D., Moussalli, A., Marshall, J. & Owens, I. P. F. 2003. Conspicuous males suffer higher predation risk: visual modelling and experimental evidence from lizards. Animal Behaviour, 66, 541–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stuart-Fox, D., Moussalli, A. & Whiting, M. J. 2008. Predator-specific camouflage in chameleons. Biology Letters, 4, 326–329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugimoto, M. 2002. Morphological color changes in fish: regulation of pigment cell density and morphology. Microscopy Research and Technique, 58, 496–503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thery, M. & Casas, J. 2009. The multiple disguises of spiders: web colour and decorations, body colour and movement. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 471–480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thurman, C. L. 1988. Rhythmic physiological colour change in crustacean: a review. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology C, 91, 171–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troscianko, T., Benton, C. P., Lovell, P. G., Tolhurst, D. J. & Pizlo, Z. 2009. Camouflage and visual perception. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 449–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tullberg, B. S., Merilaita, S. & Wiklund, C. 2005. Aposematism and crypsis combined as a result of distance dependence: functional versatility of the colour pattern in the swallowtail butterfly larva. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 272, 1315–1321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zylinski, S., Osorio, D. & Shohet, A. J. 2009a. Cuttlefish camouflage: context-dependent body pattern use during motion. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 276, 3963–3969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zylinski, S., Osorio, D. & Shohet, A. J. 2009b. Perception of edges and visual texture in the camouflage of the common cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 439–448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×