Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T08:14:08.480Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - The concealment of body parts through coincident disruptive coloration

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Innes C. Cuthill
Affiliation:
University of Bristol
Aron Székely
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Martin Stevens
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Sami Merilaita
Affiliation:
Åbo Akademi University, Finland
Get access

Summary

Most recent tests of the theory of disruptive coloration have focussed on the disguise of the body's outline (e.g. Merilaita 1998; Cuthill et al. 2005; Schaefer & Stobbe 2006; Stevens et al. 2006b; Fraser et al. 2007). When placed at the body's edge, the high-contrast colour boundaries that are characteristic of disruptive patterning create false contours of higher stimulus intensity than those of the real outline (Stevens & Cuthill 2006; Stevens et al. 2006a). In this way, the probability of object recognition through boundary shape is diminished. However, the pioneers of the theory of disruptive coloration, Abbott Thayer (1909) and Hugh Cott (1940), also emphasised the importance of concealing other characteristic, and thus potentially revealing, body parts, such as eyes and limbs. Cott (1940) devoted a whole chapter of his influential textbook to this topic, arguing that the successful disguise of such features could be achieved through what he termed ‘coincident disruptive coloration’ (Figure 3.1).

Type
Chapter
Information
Animal Camouflage
Mechanisms and Function
, pp. 34 - 52
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Chiao, C. C. & Hanlon, R. T. 2001. Cuttlefish camouflage: visual perception of size, contrast and number of white squares on artificial checkerboard substrata initiates disruptive coloration. Journal of Experimental Biology, 204, 2119–2125.Google ScholarPubMed
Chiao, C. C., Kelman, E. J. & Hanlon, R. T. 2005. Disruptive body patterning of cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) requires visual information regarding edges and contrast of objects in natural substrate backgrounds. Biological Bulletin, 208, 7–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cott, H. B. 1940. Adaptive Coloration in Animals. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Cox, D. R. 1972. Regression models and life-tables. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 34, 187–220.
Cuthill, I. C. & Szekely, A. 2009. Coincident disruptive coloration. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, 364, 489–496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthill, I. C. & Troscianko, T. S. 2009. Animal camouflage: biology meets psychology, computer science and art. International Journal of Design & Nature and Ecodynamics, 4, 183–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuthill, I. C., Stevens, M., Sheppard, J.et al. 2005. Disruptive coloration and background pattern matching. Nature, 434, 72–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, S., Callahan, A., Klassen, D.et al. 2007. Empirical tests of the role of disruptive coloration in reducing detectability. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 274, 1325–1331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghim, M. M. & Hodos, W. 2006. Spatial contrast sensitivity of birds. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 192, 523–534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, N. S. 2001. The visual ecology of avian photoreceptors. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research, 20, 675–703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, N. S., Partridge, J. C., Cuthill, I. C.et al. 2000. Visual pigments, oil droplets, ocular media and cone photoreceptor distribution in two species of passerine: the blue tit (Parus caeruleus L.) and the blackbird (Turdus merula L.). Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 186, 375–387.CrossRef
Kelman, E. J., Baddeley, R. J., Shohet, A. J.et al. 2007. Perception of visual texture and the expression of disruptive camouflage by the cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 274, 1369–1375.CrossRef
Klein, J. P. & Moeschberger, M. L. 2003. Survival Analysis: Techniques for Censored and Truncated Data. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Mathger, L. M., Chiao, C. C., Barbosa, A.et al. 2007. Disruptive coloration elicited on controlled natural substrates in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Journal of Experimental Biology, 210, 2657–2666.CrossRef
Merilaita, S. 1998. Crypsis through disruptive coloration in an isopod. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 265, 1059–1064.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merilaita, S. 2003. Visual background complexity facilitates the evolution of camouflage. Evolution, 57, 1248–1254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagawa, S. & Cuthill, I. C. 2007. Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: a practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews, 82, 591–605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R., Rosnow, R. L. & Rubin, D. B. 2000. Contrasts and Effect Sizes in Behavioral Research. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ruxton, G. D., Sherratt, T. N. & Speed, M. P. 2005. Avoiding Attack: The Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals and Mimicry. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Schaefer, H. M. & Stobbe, N. 2006. Disruptive coloration provides camouflage independent of background matching. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 273, 2427–2432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shohet, A. J., Baddeley, R. J., Anderson, J. C.et al. 2006. Cuttlefish responses to visual orientation of substrates, water flow and a model of motion camouflage. Journal of Experimental Biology, 209, 4717–4723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, M. 2005. The role of eyespots as anti-predator mechanisms, principally demonstrated in the Lepidoptera. Biological Reviews, 80, 573–588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M. 2007. Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 274, 1457–1464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M. & Cuthill, I. C. 2006. Disruptive coloration, crypsis and edge detection in early visual processing. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 273, 2141–2147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M., Cuthill, I. C., Parraga, C. A.et al. 2006a. The effectiveness of disruptive coloration as a concealment strategy. In Progress in Brain Research, vol. 115, eds. Alonso, J.-M., Macknik, S., Martinez, L., Tse, P. & Martinez-Conde, S.Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 49–65.Google Scholar
Stevens, M., Cuthill, I. C., Windsor, A. M. M.et al. 2006b. Disruptive contrast in animal camouflage. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 273, 2433–2438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, M., Hopkins, E., Hinde, W.et al. 2007. Field experiments on the effectiveness of ‘eyespots' as predator deterrents. Animal Behaviour, 74, 1215–1227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, M., Hardman, C. J. & Stubbins, C. L. 2008a. Conspicuousness, not eye mimicry, makes “eyespots” effective antipredator signals. Behavioral Ecology, 19, 525–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, M., Stubbins, C. L. & Hardman, C. J. 2008b. The anti-predator function of ‘eyespots’ on camouflaged and conspicuous prey. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 62, 1787–1793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thayer, G. H. 1909. Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom: An Exposition of the Laws of Disguise through Color and Pattern: Being a Summary of Abbott H. Thayer's Discoveries. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Vorobyev, M. & Osorio, D. 1998. Receptor noise as a determinant of colour thresholds. Proceedings of the Royal Society, Series B, 265, 351–358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vorobyev, M., Osorio, D., Bennett, A. T. D.et al. 1998. Tetrachromacy, oil droplets and bird plumage colours. Journal of Comparative Physiology A, 183, 621–633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zar, J. H. 1999. Biostatistical Analysis, 4th edn. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×