from Part IV - Models in Psychology
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 February 2018
The ability to learn is a fundamental one. Many models of cognition incorporate some form of learning, as it is rarely the case that performance on a task is unaffected by experience. This chapter presents a family of models that provide a simple and profound architecture for understanding memory and how we learn about regularities in our world.
Neural network models, or connectionist models, model human behavior using structures and representations that loosely capture our understanding of how the brain functions. Although some models do attempt to capture fine-grained details of the operation of neurons, the framework we discuss here abstracts some general operating principles from the brain and uses these to account for performance on learning and memory tasks. Most connectionist models have the following features in common:
The modeling architecture is composed of units (“neurons”) connected by weights;
Each unit has its own firing rate;
The pattern of activation (i.e., firing rates) across an ensemble of units can be treated as a representation;
The activity of a unit is determined by the incoming weights, and the activity of the units that are passing activation through those weights;
Learning is accomplished by modifying the connection strengths – the weights – between units.
Don't worry if you found that description very abstract – it is! Like many things, neural networks are better understood by example than by description. We next go through a simple and popular example of a connectionist model to put some flesh on this abstract description.
Hebbian Models
The Hebbian Associator
Associative memory refers to our ability to remember pairings between items or objects. One example of an associative memory task is the cued recall task, where a person is shown pairings of stimuli and responses, and must produce the appropriate response on seeing a stimulus. A real-world example of a cued recall task is remembering someone's name when you see their face at a conference. Here, we will assume that the stimuli and responses are both words; so, for example, if the word trench has been paired with the word scissors, then the goal is to say scissors when the word trench is later seen again.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.