Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T22:03:04.562Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Chimpanzees – Investigating Cognition in the Wild

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2018

Nereida Bueno-Guerra
Affiliation:
Comillas Pontifical University
Federica Amici
Affiliation:
Universität Leipzig
Get access

Summary

Chimpanzees are one of our closest living relative. Like humans, they live in multi-male multi-female communities following fission-fusion dynamics. Their evolutionary and social closeness makes them a perfect target to investigate the evolutionary roots of human cognition. Although chimpanzee cognition has been studied extensively, most research has been conducted in captivity. Since cognition is supposed to be shaped by the ecological and social environment, here we mainly review cognitive studies on wild chimpanzees. Chimpanzees in the wild use a wide range of social and physical cognition to gain knowledge about others, cooperate with conspecifics, and use their spatial knowledge and knowledge about tool properties. We then review different methods to investigate cognitive abilities in wild chimpanzees, in order to introduce readers to the different possible experimental set-ups. We finally discuss the advantages and problems of play-back experiments, object presentations and combinations of the two approaches. We want to provide readers with the knowledge that the investigation of many ecological relevant question in cognition needs a natural setup, allowing chimpanzees to draw from experience shaped by natural pressure.
Type
Chapter
Information
Field and Laboratory Methods in Animal Cognition
A Comparative Guide
, pp. 119 - 145
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Balda, R. P., Kamil, A. C., and Bednekoff, P. A. (1996). Predicting cognitive capacity from natural history: examples from four species of corvids. Current Ornithology, 13, 3366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, C. D., and Emery, N. J. (2009). Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 1037010375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, N. J., and Clayton, N. S. (2004). The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science, 306, 19031907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greggor, A. L., McIvor, G. E., Clayton, N. S., and Thornton, A. (2016). Contagious risk taking: social information and context influence wild jackdaws’ responses to novelty and risk. Scientific Reports, 6, 27764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, G. R. (1996). Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows. Nature, 379, 249251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, G. R. (2016). Social and spatial reintegration success of New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) released after aviary confinement. Wilson Journal of Ornithology, 128, 168173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kabadayi, C., and Osvath, M. (2017). Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science, 357, 202204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klump, B. C., Sugasawa, S., St Clair, J. J. H., and Rutz, C. (2015). Hook tool manufacture in New Caledonian crows: behavioural variation and the influence of raw materials. BMC Biology, 13, 97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loretto, M.-C., Schuster, R., Itty, C., Marchand, P., Genero, F., and Bugnyar, T. (2017). Fission–fusion dynamics over large distances in raven non-breeders. Scientific Reports, 7, 380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massen, J. J. M., Pašukonis, A., Schmidt, J., and Bugnyar, T. (2014). Ravens notice dominance reversals among conspecifics within and outside their social group. Nature Communications, 5, 3679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raby, C. R., Alexis, D. M., Dickinson, A., and Clayton, N. S. (2007). Planning for the future by western scrub-jays. Nature, 445, 919921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutz, C., Bluff, L. A., Weir, A. A. S., and Kacelnik, A. (2007). Video cameras on wild birds. Science, 318, 765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutz, C., Klump, B. C., Komarczyk, L., et al. (2016a). Discovery of species-wide tool use in the Hawaiian crow. Nature, 537, 403407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutz, C., Sugasawa, S., van der Wal, J. E. M., Klump, B. C., and St Clair, J. J. H. (2016b). Tool bending in New Caledonian crows. Royal Society Open Science, 3, 160439.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St Clair, J. J. H., Burns, Z. T., Bettaney, E., et al. (2015). Experimental resource pulses influence social-network dynamics and the potential for information flow in tool-using crows. Nature Communications, 6, 7197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swift, K. N., and Marzluff, J. M. (2015). Wild American crows gather around their dead to learn about danger. Animal Behaviour, 109, 187197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., Holzhaider, J. C., and Gray, R. D. (2007). Spontaneous metatool use by New Caledonian crows. Current Biology, 17, 15041507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wascher, C. A. F., Hillemann, F., Canestrari, D., and Baglione, V. (2015). Carrion crows learn to discriminate between calls of reliable and unreliable conspecifics. Animal Cognition, 18, 11811185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Fischer, J. (2017). Monkeytalk: inside the worlds and minds of primates. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

References

Arnold, K., and Whiten, A. (2001). Post-conflict behaviour of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo forest, Uganda. Behaviour, 138, 649690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ban, S. D., Boesch, C., and Janmaat, K. R. L. (2014). Taï chimpanzees anticipate revisiting high-valued fruit trees from further distances. Animal Cognition, 17, 13531364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., and Baayen, H. (2015). Parsimonious mixed models. ArXiv:1506.04967 [Stat]. Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/abs/1506.04967Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (1996). Hunting strategies of Gombe and Tai chimpanzees. In Chimpanzee cultures (pp. 7791). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (2002). Cooperative hunting roles among Taï chimpanzees. Human Nature, 13, 2746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. (2007). What makes us human (Homo sapiens): the challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 227240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1982). Optimisation of nut-cracking with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 83, 265286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78, 547573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., and Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The chimpanzees of the Taï forest: behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C., Kohou, G., Néné, H., and Vigilant, L. (2006). Male competition and paternity in wild chimpanzees of the Taï forest. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 130, 103115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Wittig, R., Moebius, Y., and Normand, E. (2008). Intergroup conflicts among chimpanzees in Taï National Park: lethal violence and the female perspective. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 519532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., Head, J., and Robbins, M. M. (2009). Complex tool sets for honey extraction among chimpanzees in Loango National Park, Gabon. Journal of Human Evolution, 56, 560569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., Bolé, C., Eckhardt, N., and Boesch, H. (2010). Altruism in forest chimpanzees: the case of adoption. PLoS ONE, 5(1), e8901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brüne, M., and Brüne-Cohrs, U. (2006). Theory of mind – evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 437455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R., and Whiten, A. (1989). Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Call, J. (2009). Contrasting the social cognition of humans and nonhuman apes: the shared intentionality hypothesis. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 368379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, G., Kuehl, H., Diarrassouba, A., N’Goran, P. K., and Boesch, C. (2011). Long-term research sites as refugia for threatened and over-harvested species. Biology Letters, 7, 723726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, C. S., Harris, J., and Porges, S. W. (2011). Neural and evolutionary perspectives on empathy. In The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 169182). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (2008). Baboon metaphysics: the evolution of a social mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Christel, M. (1993). Grasping techniques and hand preferences in Hominoidea. In Hands of primates (pp. 91108). Vienna: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coolidge, H. J., and Shea, B. T. (1982). External body dimensions of Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes chimpanzees. Primates, 23, 245251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., and Boesch, C. (2003). Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Animal Behaviour, 66, 115125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., and Boesch, C. (2005). Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 142, 397421.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L., and Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology, 110, 221243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2007). Baboons eavesdrop to deduce mating opportunities. Animal Behaviour, 73, 885890.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., and Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 142146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Langergraber, K., Ziegler, T. E., Zuberbühler, K., and Deschner, T. (2013). Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 280, 20122765.Google ScholarPubMed
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., and Zuberbühler, K. (2015). An intentional vocalization draws others’ attention: a playback experiment with wild chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 18, 581591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., and Zuberbuehler, K. (2017). Vocalizing in chimpanzees is influenced by social cognitive processes. Science Advances, 3(11), e1701742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronin, K. A., Pieper, B., van Leeuwen, E. J., Crockford, C., and Haun, D. B. (2015). Cooperating to compete: evaluating behavioral coordination in response to simulated territorial intrusion in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology, 77(S1), 64.Google Scholar
Duffy, K. G., Wrangham, R. W., and Silk, J. B. (2007). Male chimpanzees exchange political support for mating opportunities. Current Biology, 17, 586587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Estienne, V., Stephens, C., and Boesch, C. (2017). Extraction of honey from underground bee nests by central African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in Loango National Park, Gabon: techniques and individual differences. American Journal of Primatology, 79, 101109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, J., Noser, R., and Hammerschmidt, K. (2013). Bioacoustic field research: a primer to acoustic analyses and playback experiments with primates. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 643663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraser, O. N., Koski, S. E., Wittig, R. M., and Aureli, F. (2009). Why are bystanders friendly to recipients of aggression? Communicative and Integrative Biology, 2, 285291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilby, I. C. (2006). Meat sharing among the Gombe chimpanzees: harassment and reciprocal exchange. Animal Behaviour, 71, 953963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilby, I. C., and Connor, R. C. (2010). The role of intelligence in group hunting: are chimpanzees different from other social predators? In The mind of the chimpanzee: ecological and experimental perspectives (pp. 220232). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C., Thompson, M. E., Ruane, J. D., and Wrangham, R. (2010). No evidence of short-term exchange of meat for sex among chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 59, 4453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomes, C. M., and Boesch, C. (2009). Wild chimpanzees exchange meat for sex on a long-term basis. PLoS ONE, 4(4), e5116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomes, C. M., Mundry, R., and Boesch, C. (2009). Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 276, 699706.Google ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gruber, T., Muller, M. N., Strimling, P., Wrangham, R., and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Wild chimpanzees rely on cultural knowledge to solve an experimental honey acquisition task. Current Biology, 19, 18061810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grützmacher, K., Keil, V., Leinert, V., et al. (2017). Human quarantine: toward reducing infectious pressure on chimpanzees at the Taï Chimpanzee Project, Côte d’Ivoire. American Journal of Primatology, 80, e22619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61, 139151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernàndez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317, 13601366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, E., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Call, J., Hare, B., and Tomasello, M. (2010). The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychological Science, 21, 102110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heymann, E. W. (2006). The neglected sense – olfaction in primate behavior, ecology, and evolution. American Journal of Primatology, 68, 519524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobaiter, C., Schel, A. M., Langergraber, K., and Zuberbühler, K. (2014). ‘Adoption’ by maternal siblings in wild chimpanzees. PLoS ONE, 9(8), e103777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockings, K. J., Humle, T., Anderson, J. R., et al. (2007). Chimpanzees share forbidden fruit. PLoS ONE, 2(9), e886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horner, V., and Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humle, T., and Matsuzawa, T. (2002). Ant-dipping among the chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, and some comparisons with other sites. American Journal of Primatology, 58, 133148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inoue, S., and Matsuzawa, T. (2007). Working memory of numerals in chimpanzees. Current Biology, 17, 10041005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inoue-Nakamura, N., and Matsuzawa, T. (1997). Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 159173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janmaat, K. R. L., Ban, S. D., and Boesch, C. (2013). Taï chimpanzees use botanical skills to discover fruit: what we can learn from their mistakes. Animal Cognition, 16, 851860.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janmaat, K. R. L., Polansky, L., Ban, S. D., and Boesch, C. (2014). Wild chimpanzees plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 1634316348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahlenberg, S. M., Thompson, M. E., Muller, M. N., and Wrangham, R. W. (2008a). Immigration costs for female chimpanzees and male protection as an immigrant counterstrategy to intrasexual aggression. Animal Behaviour, 76, 14971509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M., Thompson, M. E., and Wrangham, R. W. (2008b). Female competition over core areas in Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology, 29, 931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köhler, W. (2013). Intelligenzprüfungen an Menschenaffen: mit einem Anhang zur Psychologie des Schimpansen (Vol. 134). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Kojima, S. (1990). Comparison of auditory functions in the chimpanzee and human. Folia Primatologica, 55, 6272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köndgen, S., Kühl, H., N’Goran, P. K., et al. (2008). Pandemic human viruses cause decline of endangered great apes. Current Biology, 18, 260264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354, 110114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kühl, H. S., Kalan, A. K., Arandjelovic, M., et al. (2016). Chimpanzee accumulative stone throwing. Scientific Reports, 6, 22219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kutsukake, N., and Castles, D. L. (2004). Reconciliation and post-conflict third-party affiliation among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates, 45, 157165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langergraber, K. E., Mitani, J. C., and Vigilant, L. (2007). The limited impact of kinship on cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 77867790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langergraber, K. E., Boesch, C., Inoue, E., et al. (2010). Genetic and ‘cultural’ similarity in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 278, 20101112.Google ScholarPubMed
Langergraber, K. E., Prüfer, K., Rowney, C., et al. (2012). Generation times in wild chimpanzees and gorillas suggest earlier divergence times in great ape and human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1571615721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2006). What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? Animal Cognition, 9, 3646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luncz, L. V., Mundry, R., and Boesch, C. (2012). Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities. Current Biology, 22, 922926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luncz, L. V., Wittig, R. M., and Boesch, C. (2015). Primate archaeology reveals cultural transmission in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370, 20140348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Ordas, G., Haun, D., Colmenares, F., and Call, J. (2010). Keeping track of time: evidence for episodic-like memory in great apes. Animal Cognition, 13, 331340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto-Oda, A., Kutsukake, N., Hosaka, K., and Matsusaka, T. (2007). Sniffing behaviors in Mahale chimpanzees. Primates, 48, 8185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsuno, T., Kawai, N., and Matsuzawa, T. (2004). Color classification by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a matching-to-sample task. Behavioural Brain Research, 148, 157165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsuzawa, T., Biro, D., Humle, T., Inoue-Nakamura, N., Tonooka, R., and Yamakoshi, G. (2008). Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master-apprenticeship. In Primate origins of human cognition and behavior (pp. 557574). Japan: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mennill, D. J., Ratcliffe, L. M., and Boag, P. T. (2002). Female eavesdropping on male song contests in songbirds. Science, 296, 873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menzel, C. R. (1999). Unprompted recall and reporting of hidden objects by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) after extended delays. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 426434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mielke, A., Samuni, L., Preis, A., Gogarten, J. F., Crockford, C., and Wittig, R. M. (2017). Bystanders intervene to impede grooming in Western chimpanzees and sooty mangabeys. Royal Society Open Science, 4(11), 171296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitani, J. C., and Watts, D. P. (2001). Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour, 61, 915924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Merriwether, D. A., and Zhang, C. (2000). Male affiliation, cooperation and kinship in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 59, 885893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitani, J. C., Watts, D. P., and Amsler, S. J. (2010). Lethal intergroup aggression leads to territorial expansion in wild chimpanzees. Current Biology, 20, 507508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulcahy, N. J., and Call, J. (2006). Apes save tools for future use. Science, 312, 10381040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundry, R. (2017). From nonparametric tests to mixed models: a brief overview of statistical tools frequently used in comparative psychology. In APA Handbook of comparative psychology, Vol. 1 (pp. 157177). Philadelphia, PA: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E., and Lee, P. C. (2011). Grooming reciprocity in wild male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 81, 439446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates, 9, 167224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., Hasegawa, T., Hayaki, H., Takahata, Y., and Uehara, S. (1992). Meat-sharing as a coalitionary strategy by an alpha male chimpanzee. In Topics in primatology (pp. 159174). Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.Google Scholar
Normand, E., and Boesch, C. (2009). Sophisticated Euclidean maps in forest chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 77, 11951201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Normand, E., Ban, S. D., and Boesch, C. (2009). Forest chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) remember the location of numerous fruit trees. Animal Cognition, 12, 797807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Povinelli, D. J., and Vonk, J. (2003). Chimpanzee minds: suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 157160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruetz, J. D., and Bertolani, P. (2007). Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biology, 17, 412417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radford, A. N., Majolo, B., and Aureli, F. (2016). Within-group behavioural consequences of between-group conflict: a prospective review. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 283, 1843.Google ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, G., Bastien, M. L., and van Schaik, C. (2007). Animal innovation defined and operationalized. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 30, 393437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rilling, J. K., and Young, L. J. (2014). The biology of mammalian parenting and its effect on offspring social development. Science, 345, 771776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuni, L., Preis, A., Mundry, R., Deschner, T., Crockford, C., and Wittig, R. M. (2017). Oxytocin reactivity during intergroup conflict in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 268273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanz, C. M., and Morgan, D. B. (2007). Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 420433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schel, A. M., Machanda, Z., Townsend, S. W., Zuberbühler, K., and Slocombe, K. E. (2013a). Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Animal Behaviour, 86, 955965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schel, A. M., Townsend, S. W., Machanda, Z., Zuberbuehler, K., and Slocombe, K. E. (2013b). Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE, 8(10), e76674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schino, G., and Aureli, F. (2010). Primate reciprocity and its cognitive requirements. Evolutionary Anthropology, 19, 130135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2012). The evolutionary origins of friendship. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 153177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2013). Affiliation, empathy, and the origins of theory of mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 1034910356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sirianni, G., Mundry, R., and Boesch, C. (2015a). When to choose which tool: multidimensional and conditional selection of nut-cracking hammers in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 100, 152165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sirianni, G., Wittig, R. M., and Boesch, C. (2015b). Do chimpanzees anticipate an object’s weight? A field experiment on the kinematics of hammer-lifting movements in the nut-cracking Tai chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 86, 360361.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1722817233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Vocal communication in chimpanzees. In The mind of the chimpanzee: ecological and experimental perspectives (pp. 192207). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., Kaller, T., Turman, L., et al. (2010). Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64, 19591966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Surbeck, M., Boesch, C., Girard-Buttoz, C., Crockford, C., Hohmann, G., and Wittig, R. M. (2017a). Comparison of male conflict behavior in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus), with specific regard to coalition and post-conflict behavior. American Journal of Primatology, 79, e22641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surbeck, M., Girard-Buttoz, C., Boesch, C., et al. (2017b). Sex-specific association patterns in bonobos and chimpanzees reflect species differences in cooperation. Royal Society Open Science, 4, 161081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2009a). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009b). Why we cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uehara, S., and Nishida, T. (1987). Body weights of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 72, 315321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs, 1, 161-IN12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2000). Grooming between male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. I. Partner number and diversity and grooming reciprocity. International Journal of Primatology, 21, 189210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, D. P., and Mitani, J. C. (2001). Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 299327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M. L., and Wrangham, R. W. (2003). Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 363392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Kahlenberg, S. M., Wells, M., and Wrangham, R. W. (2012). Ecological and social factors affect the occurrence and outcomes of intergroup encounters in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 83, 277291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Boesch, C., Fruth, B., et al. (2014). Lethal aggression in Pan is better explained by adaptive strategies than human impacts. Nature, 513, 414417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittig, R. M., and Boesch, C. (2003a). Food competition and linear dominance hierarchy among female chimpanzees of the Taï National Park. International Journal of Primatology, 24, 847867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittig, R. M., and Boesch, C. (2003b). The choice of post-conflict interactions in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behaviour, 140, 15271559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittig, R. M., and Boesch, C. (2010). Receiving post-conflict affiliation from the enemy’s friend reconciles former opponents. PLoS ONE, 5(11), e13995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2007a). Vocal alliances in Chacma baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61, 899909.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Wikberg, E., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2007b). Kin-mediated reconciliation substitutes for direct reconciliation in female baboons. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 274, 11091115.Google ScholarPubMed
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Deschner, T., Langergraber, K. E., Ziegler, T. E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2014a). Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20133096.Google ScholarPubMed
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Langergraber, K. E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2014b). Triadic social interactions operate across time: a field experiment with wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20133155.Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Weltring, A., Langergraber, K. E., Deschner, T., and Zuberbühler, K. (2016). Social support reduces stress hormone levels in wild chimpanzees across stressful events and everyday affiliations. Nature Communications, 7, 13361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wroblewski, E. E. (2008). An unusual incident of adoption in a wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) population at Gombe National Park. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 995998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wroblewski, E. E., Murray, C. M., Keele, B. F., Schumacher-Stankey, J. C., Hahn, B. H., and Pusey, A. E. (2009). Male dominance rank and reproductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Animal Behaviour, 77, 873885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuberbühler, K., and Byrne, R. W. (2006). Social cognition. Current Biology, 16, 786790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuberbühler, K., and Wittig, R. M. (2011). Field experiments with nonhuman primates: a tutorial. In Field and laboratory methods in primatology: a practical guide (pp. 207224). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×