from Section A - Cognitive Development
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 August 2016
The term “real” is extremely ambiguous. A trip to an art museum brings out one interpretation. For example, the French Impressionist Edgar Degas (1834–1917) painted ballet dancers practicing in mirrored rooms. Some dancers in Degas’ paintings show dancers in front of, and reflected in a mirror; some show mirror images of dancers off to the side. Which, if any, is a reflection of a real person? Many other artists’ paintings challenge viewers to ponder reality. Sometimes the work shows reflections in windows, outside open windows, or even reproductions within a painting inside the painting.
Our research provides a rather simple way to determine whether an artist assumed that the reflections or reproductions in their paintings were “real” or not. This is to ask observers to count the number of separate objects in a particular painting. If a mirror captures the reflection of an object in front of it, it cannot be counted, but an object shown outside an open window, can. Why? Our implicit counting knowledge prohibits the double counting of an item. So, if you do not count the reflection in the mirror, you do not think it is a real item. To explain, I need to cover a bit about your counting knowledge.
When we count, we must honor the one–one principle and, therefore, uniquely tag every object once and only once. If we double tag an item, we will use too many tags and get the wrong answer to a “how many?” question. All normal adults and many young children are good counters. Even four-year-olds will correct a puppet that counts an item twice. If one counts an exact mirror copy of a foregrounded item, one commits a principled violation. In our experiments with adults we took advantage of this fact.
Dana Chesney and I showed college students line drawings of frames that we dubbed a mirror, picture frame, or an open window. Within each there was a reproduction of the objects drawn in the foreground (e.g., two chairs). So, two chairs were shown in the mirror, two were outside the open window, and two smaller ones were in a painting on the wall. People simply were asked how many were present in each kind.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.