Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home
The Evolution of Language
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 230
  • Cited by
    This book has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Progovac, Ljiljana 2019. A Critical Introduction to Language Evolution. p. 67.

    Guo, Rongxing 2019. Human-Earth System Dynamics. p. 53.

    2019. Assembly of the Executive Mind. p. 124.

    Progovac, Ljiljana 2019. A Critical Introduction to Language Evolution. p. 31.

    Scalia, Jeremiah Cassar 2019. Meanings & Co.. Vol. 6, Issue. , p. 89.

    Behme, Christina 2018. Essays on Linguistic Realism. Vol. 196, Issue. , p. 61.

    Pika, Simone Wilkinson, Ray Kendrick, Kobin H. and Vernes, Sonja C. 2018. Taking turns: bridging the gap between human and animal communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, Vol. 285, Issue. 1880, p. 20180598.

    Miyagawa, Shigeru Lesure, Cora and Nóbrega, Vitor A. 2018. Cross-Modality Information Transfer: A Hypothesis about the Relationship among Prehistoric Cave Paintings, Symbolic Thinking, and the Emergence of Language. Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 9, Issue. ,

    Hasiniaina, Alida F. Scheumann, Marina Rina Evasoa, Mamy Braud, Diane Rasoloharijaona, Solofonirina Randrianambinina, Blanchard and Zimmermann, Elke 2018. High frequency/ultrasonic communication in a critically endangered nocturnal primate, Claire's mouse lemur (Microcebus mamiratra ). American Journal of Primatology, Vol. 80, Issue. 6, p. e22866.

    Belyk, Michel Johnson, Joseph F. and Kotz, Sonja A. 2018. Poor neuro-motor tuning of the human larynx: a comparison of sung and whistled pitch imitation. Royal Society Open Science, Vol. 5, Issue. 4, p. 171544.

    Frey, Roland Reby, David Fritsch, Guido and Charlton, Benjamin D. 2018. The remarkable vocal anatomy of the koala (Phascolarctos cinereus ): insights into low-frequency sound production in a marsupial species. Journal of Anatomy, Vol. 232, Issue. 4, p. 575.

    Larsson, Matz and Abbott, Benjamin W. 2018. Is the Capacity for Vocal Learning in Vertebrates Rooted in Fish Schooling Behavior?. Evolutionary Biology, Vol. 45, Issue. 4, p. 359.

    Suzuki, Toshitaka N. 2018. Alarm calls evoke a visual search image of a predator in birds. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Vol. 115, Issue. 7, p. 1541.

    Ferretti, Francesco Adornetti, Ines Chiera, Alessandra Cosentino, Erica and Nicchiarelli, Serena 2018. Introduction: Origin and Evolution of Language—An Interdisciplinary Perspective. Topoi, Vol. 37, Issue. 2, p. 219.

    Pohl, Peter Douglas Greer, R. Du, Lin and Lee Moschella, Jennifer 2018. Verbal Development, Behavioral Metamorphosis, and the Evolution of Language. Perspectives on Behavior Science,

    Ellard, Donna Beth 2018. Communicating Between Species and Between Disciplines—Lessons from the Old English Seafarer. Exemplaria, Vol. 30, Issue. 4, p. 293.

    Moore, Richard 2018. Gricean communication, language development, and animal minds. Philosophy Compass, Vol. 13, Issue. 12, p. e12550.

    Suzuki, Toshitaka N. Wheatcroft, David and Griesser, Michael 2018. Call combinations in birds and the evolution of compositional syntax. PLOS Biology, Vol. 16, Issue. 8, p. e2006532.

    DUNN, JACOB C. 2018. Sexual selection and the loss of laryngeal air sacs during the evolution of speech. Anthropological Science, Vol. 126, Issue. 1, p. 29.

    Koda, Hiroki Kunieda, Takumi and Nishimura, Takeshi 2018. From hand to mouth: monkeys require greater effort in motor preparation for voluntary control of vocalization than for manual actions. Royal Society Open Science, Vol. 5, Issue. 11, p. 180879.

    ×

Book description

Language, more than anything else, is what makes us human. It appears that no communication system of equivalent power exists elsewhere in the animal kingdom. Any normal human child will learn a language based on rather sparse data in the surrounding world, while even the brightest chimpanzee, exposed to the same environment, will not. Why not? How, and why, did language evolve in our species and not in others? Since Darwin's theory of evolution, questions about the origin of language have generated a rapidly-growing scientific literature, stretched across a number of disciplines, much of it directed at specialist audiences. The diversity of perspectives - from linguistics, anthropology, speech science, genetics, neuroscience and evolutionary biology - can be bewildering. Tecumseh Fitch cuts through this vast literature, bringing together its most important insights to explore one of the biggest unsolved puzzles of human history.

Reviews

'The evolution of language has been described as the hardest problem in science, fraught with conflict, entrenched views, and misunderstandings between the multifarious disciplines involved. Fitch guides us through this tangled and often treacherous domain with clarity, equanimity, and encyclopedic reach. No other book so completely, fairly, and eloquently presents contemporary notions as to how language evolved.'

Michael Corballis - University of Auckland

'Reconstructing the evolution of language is a daunting task but Tecumseh Fitch brings it off with style. An impressive synthesis.'

Robert Seyfarth - Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania

'Tecumseh Fitch's book reads like a novel, telling the story of the long and complex search for a theory of language evolution very clearly and engagingly … comprehensive, analytic, and fair.'

Merlin Donald - Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario

Refine List
Actions for selected content:
Select all | Deselect all
  • View selected items
  • Export citations
  • Download PDF (zip)
  • Send to Kindle
  • Send to Dropbox
  • Send to Google Drive
  • Send content to

    To send content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about sending content to .

    To send content items to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

    Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

    Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

    Please be advised that item(s) you selected are not available.
    You are about to send
    ×

Save Search

You can save your searches here and later view and run them again in "My saved searches".

Please provide a title, maximum of 40 characters.
×
References
Aarsleff, H. (1976). “An outline of language-origins theory since the Renaissance,” Annals of the New York Academy of Science 280, pp. 4–17.
Abe, H., Hasegawa, Y., and Wada, K. (1977). “A note on the air-sac of the ribbon seal,” Scientific Reports of the Whales Research Institute 29, pp. 129–135.
Abeillé, A. and Rambow, O. (eds) (2000). Tree Adjoining Grammars: Formalisms, Linguistic Analysis and Processing (Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications).
Abler, W. (1989). “On the particulate principle of self-diversifying system,” Journal of Social & Biological Structures 12, pp. 1–13.
Adret, P. (1993). “Vocal learning induced with operant techniques: An overview,” Netherlands Journal of Zoology 43, pp. 125–142.
Agusti, J. and Antón, M. (2002). Mammoths, Sabertooths, and Hominids: 65 million years of mammalian evolution in Europe (New York, NY: Columbia University Press).
Aiello, L. C. (1996). “Terrestriality, bipedalism and the origin of language,” Proceedings of the British Academy 88, pp. 269–289.
Aiello, L. C. and Dean, M. C. (1990). An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy (London: Academic Press).
Aiello, L. C. and Key, C. (2002). “Energetic consequences of being a Homo erectus female,” American Journal of Human Biology 14, pp. 551–565.
Aiello, L. C. and Wheeler, P. (1995). “The expensive tissue hypothesis: The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution,” Current Anthropology 36, pp. 199–221.
Aitchison, J. (2000). The Seeds of Speech: Language origin and evolution (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Aitken, P. G. and Wilson, W. A. (1979). “Discriminative vocal conditioning in Rhesus monkeys: Evidence for volitional control?,” Brain and Language 8, pp. 227–240.
Alberts, B., Johnson, A., Walter, P., Raff, M., and Roberts, K. (2008). Molecular Biology of the Cell (New York, NY: Garland).
Alemseged, Z., Spoor, F., Kimbel, W. H., Bobe, R., Geraads, D., Reed, D., and Wynn, J. G. (2006). “A juvenile early hominin skeleton from Dikika, Ethiopia,” Nature 443, pp. 296–301.
Allin, E. F. (1975). “Evolution of the mammalian middle ear,” Journal of Morphology 147, pp. 403–438.
Allin, E. F. and Hopson, J. A. (1992). “Evolution of the auditory system in Synapsida (‘mammal-like retiles’ and primitive mammals) as seen in the fossil record,” in The Evolutionary Biology of Hearing, ed. Webster, D. B., Fay, R. F., and Popper, A. N. (New York, NY: Springer-Verlag, pp. 587–614).
Allman, J. M. (1999). Evolving Brains (New York, NY: Scientific American Library; distributed by W. H. Freeman and Co.).
Allott, R. (1989). The Motor Theory of Language Origin(Sussex: The Book Guild).
Andersson, M. B. (1994). Sexual Selection (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Arbib, M. A. (2002). “The mirror system, imitation, and the evolution of language,” in Imitation in Animals and Artifacts, ed. Nehaniv, C. and Dautenhahn, K. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 229–280).
Arbib, M. A. (2004). “How far is language beyond our grasp: A response to Hurford,” in The Evolution of Communication Systems: A comparative approach, ed. Oller, D. K. and Griebel, U. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 315–321).
Arbib, M. A. (2005). “From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28, pp. 105–167.
Arcadi, A. C. (1996). “Phrase structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: Patterns of production and interpopulation variability,” American Journal of Primatology 39, pp. 159–178.
Arcadi, A. C., Robert, D., and Mugurusi, F. (2004). “A comparison of buttress drumming by male chimpanzees from two populations,” Primates 45, pp. 135–139.
Arensburg, B. (1994). “Middle Paleolithic speech capabilities: A response to Dr. Lieberman,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 94, pp. 279–280.
Arensburg, B., Schepartz, L. A., Tillier, A. M., Vandermeersch, B., and Rak, Y. (1990). “A reappraisal of the anatomical basis for speech in middle Paleolithic hominids,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 83, pp. 137–146.
Arensburg, B., Tillier, A. M., Vandermeersch, B., Duday, H., Schepartz, L. A., and Rak, Y. (1989). “A middle paleolithic human hyoid bone,” Nature 338, pp. 758–760.
Ariew, A. (1999). “Innateness is Canalization: In defense of a developmental account of innateness,” in Where Biology Meets Psychology: Philosophical essays, ed. Hardcastle, V. G. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 117–138).
,Aristotle (350 bc). The History of Animals (London: Heinemann).
Armstrong, D. F. (1983). “Iconicity, arbitrariness, and duality of patterning in signed and spoken languages: Perspectives on language evolution,” Sign Language Studies 38, pp. 51–69.
Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe, W. C., and Wilcox, S. E. (1984). “Signs of the origin of syntax,” Current Anthropology 35, pp. 349–368.
Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe, W. C., and Wilcox, S. E. (1995). Gesture and the Nature of Language (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006). “Semantic combinations in primate calls,” Nature 441, p. 303.
Arom, S. (2000). “Prologomena to a biomusicology,” in The Origins of Music, ed. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B., and Brown, S. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 27–29).
Arthur, W. (2002). “The emerging conceptual framework of evolutionary developmental biology,” Nature 415, pp. 757–764.
Arvola, A. (1974). “Vocalization in the guinea-pig, C. porcellus L.,” Annales Zoologici Fennici 11, pp. 1–96.
Asfaw, B., Beyene, Y., Suwa, G., Walter, R. C., White, T. D., WoldeGabriel, G., and Yemane, T. (1992). “The earliest Acheulean from Konso-Gardula,” Nature 360, pp. 732–735.
Auel, J. M. (1984). The Clan of the Cave Bear (New York: Bantam).
Avanzini, G., Faienza, C., Minciacchi, D., Lopez, L., and Majno, M. (eds) (2003). The Neurosciences and Music (New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences).
Avanzini, G., Lopez, L., Koelsch, S., and Majno, M. (eds) (2005). The Neurosciences and Music II (New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences).
Avital, E. and Jablonka, E. (2000). Animal Traditions: Behavioural inheritance in evolution (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Axelrod, R. (1997). The Complexity of Cooperation: Agent-based models of competition and collaboration (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Axelrod, R. and Dion, D. (1988). “The further evolution of cooperation,” Science 242, pp. 1385–1390.
Axelrod, R. and Hamilton, W. D. (1981). “The evolution of cooperation,” Science 211, pp. 1390–1396.
Bachorowski, J.-A. and Owren, M. J. (2001). “Not all laughs are alike: Voiced but not unvoiced laughter readily elicits positive affect,” Psychological Science 12, pp. 252–257.
Baker, M. C. and Cunningham, M. A. (1985a). “The biology of bird song dialects,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, pp. 85–133.
Baker, M. C. and Cunningham, M. A. (1985b). “The biology of bird-song dialects,” Behavioural Processes 8, pp. 85–133.
Baker, M. C. and Mewaldt, L. R. (1978). “Song dialects as barriers to dispersal in White-crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttali,” Evolution 32, pp. 712–722.
Bakewell, M. A., Shi, P., and Zhang, J. (2007). “More genes underwent positive selection in chimpanzee evolution than in human evolution,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104, pp. 7489–7494.
Balaban, E. (1988). “Bird song syntax: Learned intraspecific variation is meaningful,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 85, pp. 3657–3660.
Balda, R. P., Pepperberg, I. M., and Kamil, A. C. (1998). Animal Cognition in Nature: The convergence of psychology and biology in laboratory and field (London: Academic Press).
Balzano, G. J. (1980). “The group-theoretic description of 12-fold and microtonal pitch systems,” Computer Music Journal 4, pp. 66–84.
Bangert, M., Peschel, T., Schlaug, G., Rotte, M., Drescher, D., Hinrichs, H., Heinze, H. J., and Altenmüller, E. (2006). “Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction,” Neuroimage 30, pp. 917–926.
Barash, D. P. (1974). “Neighbor recognition in two ‘solitary’ carnivores: The raccoon (Procyon lotor) and the Red Fox (Vulpes fulva),” Science 185, pp. 794–796.
Barfield, R. J., Auerbach, P. A., Geyer, L. A., and McKintosh, T. K. (1979). “Ultrasonic vocalisation in rat sexual behaviour,” American Zoologist 19, pp. 469–480.
Barfield, R. J. and Geyer, L. A. (1972). “Sexual behaviour: Ultrasonic post-ejaculatory song of the male rat,” Science 176, pp. 1349–1350.
Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (eds) (1992). The Adapted Mind (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., and Frith, U. (1985). “Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’?,” Cognition 21, pp. 37–46.
Barrett, M. D. (1978). “Lexical development and overextension in child language,” Journal of Child Language 5, pp. 209–219.
Barton, N. and Partridge, L. (2000). “Limits to natural selection,” Bioessays 22, pp. 1075–1084.
Bass, A. H. and Baker, R. (1997). “Phenotypic specification of hindbrain rhombomeres and the origins of rhythmic circuits in vertebrates,” Brain, Behavior and Evolution 50, pp. 3–16.
Bates, E. (1999). “Plasticity, localization and language development,” in The Changing Nervous System: Neurobiological consequences of early brain disorders, ed. Bronan, S. and Fletcher, J. M. (New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 214–253).
Bateson, P. P. G. (1966). “The characteristics and context of imprinting,” Biological Reviews 41, pp. 177–220.
Bateson, W. (1894). Materials for the Study of Variation Treated with Especial Regard to Discontinuity in the Origin of Species (London: Macmillan).
Bauer, R. H. (1993). “Lateralization of neural control for vocalization by the frog (Rana pipiens),” Psychobiology 21, pp. 243–248.
Bear, M. F., Connors, B. W., and Paradiso, M. A. (2001). Neuroscience: Exploring the brain (Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins).
Beck, B. B. (1980). Animal Tool Behavior: The use and manufacture of tools by animals (New York, NY: Garland STPM Press).
Beck, C. B. (1976). Origin and Early Evolution of Angiosperms (New York, NY: Columbia University Press).
Bednekoff, P. A. and Balda, R. P. (1996). “Social caching and observational spatial memory in Pinyon Jays,” Behaviour 133, pp. 807–826.
Bekoff, M., Allen, C., and Burghardt, G. M. (eds) (2002). The Cognitive Animal: Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/London: Bradford Books).
Bellugi, U. and Klima, E. S. (1978). “Two faces of sign: Iconic and abstract,” Annals of the New York Academy of Science 280, pp. 514–538.
Belyaev, D. K. (1969). “Domestication of animals,” Science Journal, January, pp. 47–52.
Bercovitch, F. B. (1988). “Coalitions, cooperation and reproductive tactics among adult male baboons,” Animal Behavavior 36, pp. 1198–1209.
Bergman, T. J., Beehner, J. C., Cheney, D. L., and Seyfarth, R. M. (2003). “Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons,” Science 302, pp. 1234–1236.
Bergstrom, C. T. and Lachmann, M. (1998a). “Signalling among relatives I: Is costly signalling too costly?,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B (London) 352, pp. 609–617.
Bergstrom, C. T. and Lachmann, M. (1998b). “Signalling among relatives III: Talk is cheap,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95, pp. 5100–5105.
Berlinski, D. (2001). The Advent of the Algorithm: The 300-year journey from an idea to the computer (San Diego, CA: Harcourt).
Bermúdez de Castro, J. M., Arsuaga, J. L., Carbonell, E., Rosas, A., Martínez, I., and Mosquera, M. (1997). “A Hominid from the Lower Pleistocene of Atapuerca, Spain: Possible ancestor to Neandertals and modern humans,” Science 276, pp. 1392–1395.
Bernstein, L. (1981). The Unanswered Question: Six talks at Harvard (Charles Eliot Norton lectures) (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Berntson, G. G., Boysen, S. T., Bauer, H. R., and Torello, M. S. (1990). “Conspecific screams and laughter: Cardiac and behavioral reactions of infant chimpanzees,” Developmental Psychobiology 22, pp. 771–787.
Berthier, M. (1999). Transcortical Aphasias (London: Psychology Press).
Berwick, R. C. (1997). “Syntax facit saltum: Computation and the genotype and phenotype of language,” Journal of Neurolinguistics 10, pp. 231–249.
Berwick, R. C. (1998). “Language evolution and the Minimalist Program: The origins of syntax,” in Approaches to the Evolution of Language, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 320–340).
Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of Language (Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma Press).
Bickerton, D. (1984). “The language bioprogram hypothesis,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7, pp. 173–221.
Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and Species (Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press).
Bickerton, D. (1995). Language and Human Behavior (Seattle: University of Washington Press).
Bickerton, D. (1998). “Catastrophic evolution: The case for a single step from protolanguage to full human language,” in Approaches to the Evolution of Language, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 341–358).
Bickerton, D. (2000). “How protolanguage became language,” in The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form, ed. Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Hurford, J. R. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 264–284).
Bickerton, D. (2003). “Symbol and structure: A comprehensive framework for language evolution,” in Language Evolution, ed. Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 77–94).
Bickerton, D. (2007). “Language evolution: A brief guide for linguists,” Lingua 117, pp. 510–526.
Bienenstock, E. (1995). “A model of neocortex,” Network: Computation inNeural Systems 6, pp. 179–224.
Bisazza, A., Rogers, L. J., and Vallortigara, G. (1998). “The origins of cerebral asymmetry: A review of evidence of behavioural and brain lateralization in fishes, reptiles and amphibians,” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 22, pp. 411–426.
Bisazza, A., Rogers, L. J., and Vallortigara, G. (1999). “Possible evolutionary origins of cognitive brain lateralization,” Brain Research Reviews 30, pp. 164–175.
Blackmore, S. J. (2000). The Meme Machine (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Blevins, J. (2004). Evolutionary Phonology (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Blevins, J. (2006). “A theoretical synopsis of evolutionary phonology,” Theoretical Linguistics 32, pp. 117–166.
Bloom, P. (2000). How Children Learn the Meanings of Words (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Bloomfield, L. (1933). Language (New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston).
Blumenthal, P. J. (2003). Kaspar Hausers Geschwister (Munich: Piper Verlag).
Boë, L.-J., Heim, J.-L., Honda, K., and Maeda, S. (2002). “The potential Neandertal vowel space was as large as that of modern humans,” Journal of Phonetics 30, pp. 465–484.
Boesch, C. (1991). “Teaching among wild chimpanzees,” Animal Behavavior 41, pp. 530–532.
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1983). “Optimization of nut-cracking in wild chimpanzees,” Behaviour 83, pp. 265–286.
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Boetius, A. (2005). “Microfauna–macrofauna interaction in the seafloor: Lessons from a tubeworm,” PLos Biology 3, pp. 375–378.
Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). “Mechanisms of avian imprinting: A review,” Biological Reviews 66, pp. 303–345.
Bonner, J. T. (1983). The Evolution of Culture in Animals (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Bookheimer, S. (2002). “Functional MRI of language: New approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing,” Annual Review of Neuroscience 25, pp. 151–188.
Boroditsky, L. (2003). “Linguistic relativity,” in Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, ed. Nadel, L. (London: MacMillan, pp. 917–921).
Borsley, R. D. (1996). Modern Phrase Structure Grammar (Oxford: Blackwell).
Borst, A. (1957). Der Turmbau von Babel: Geschicte der Meinungen über Urspring und Vielfalt der Sprachen und Völker (Stuttgart: A. Hiersemann).
Bosma, J. and Lind, J. (1965). “Cry motions of the newborn infant,” Acta Paediatrica Scandanavica Suppl 163, pp. 61–92.
Botha, R. P. (2008). “On modelling prelinguistic evolution in early hominins,” Language & Communication 28, pp. 258–275.
Botha, R. P. (2009). “On musilanguage/‘Hmmmmm’ as an evolutionary precursor to language,” Language & Communication 29, pp. 61–76.
Botha, R. P. (2003). Unravelling the Evolution of Language (New York, NY: Elsevier).
Bowden, D., Winter, P., and Ploog, D. W. (1967). “Pregnancy and delivery behavior in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus) and other primates,” Folia Primatologica 5, pp. 1–42.
Bowler, P. J. (2003). Evolution: The history of an idea (Berkely, CA: University of California Press).
Bowles, A. E., Young, W. G., and Asper, E. D. (1988). “Ontogeny of stereotyped calling of a killer whale calf, Orcinus orca, during her first year,” Rit Fiskideildar 11, pp. 251–275.
Bowles, R. L. (1889). “Observations upon the mammalian pharynx, with especial reference to the epiglottis,” Journal of Anatomy and Physiology, London 23, pp. 606–615.
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1983). “The cultural transmission of acquired variation: Effects on genetic fitness,” Journal of Theoretical Biology 58, pp. 567–596.
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press).
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1988). “The evolution of reciprocity in sizeable groups,” Journal of Theoretical Biology 132, pp. 337–356.
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1992). “Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups,” Ethology and Sociobiology 13, pp. 171–195.
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1996). “Why culture is common but cultural evolution is rare,” Proceedings of the British Academy 88, pp. 77–93.
Boysen, S. T. (1997). “Representation of quantities by apes,” Advances in the Study of Behavior 26, pp. 435–462.
Bradbury, J. W. (2001). “Vocal communication of wild parrots,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 115, p. 2373.
Bradbury, J. W. and Andersson, M. B. (eds) (1987). Sexual Selection: Testing the alternatives (Berlin: Springer-Verlag).
Bradbury, J. W. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (1998). Principles of Animal Communication (Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates).
Bradshaw, J. L. and Rodgers, L. J. (1993). The Evolution of Lateral Asymmetries: Language, Tool Use, and Intellect (San Diego, CA: Academic Press).
Braitenberg, V. (1977). On the Texture of Brains (New York, NY: Springer-Verlag).
Bramble, D. M. and Carrier, D. R. (1983). “Running and breathing in mammals,” Science 219, pp. 251–256.
Bramble, D. M. and Lieberman, D. E. (2004). “Endurance running and the evolution of Homo,” Nature 432, pp. 345–352.
Brandon, R. N. (1990). Adaptation and Natural Selection (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Brentari, D. (1996). “Sign language phonology,” in The Handbook of Phonological Theory, ed. Goldsmith, J. A. (Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 615–639).
Brentari, D. (1998). A Prosodic Model of Sign Language Phonology (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Bresnan, J. (2001). Lexical–Functional Syntax (Oxford: Blackwell).
Breuer, T., Ndoundou-Hockemba, M., and Fishlock, V. (2006). “First observation of tool use in wild gorillas,” PLOS Biology 3, p. e380.
Briscoe, T. (ed.) (2002). Linguistic Evolution through Language Acquisition: Formal and computational models (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Briscoe, T. (2003). “Grammatical assimilation,” in Language Evolution, ed. Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 295–316).
Brockelman, W. Y. and Schilling, D. (1984). “Inheritance of stereotyped gibbon calls,” Nature 312, pp. 634–636.
Brosch, M., Selezneva, E., Bucks, C., and Scheich, H. (2004). “Macaque monkeys discriminate pitch relationships,” Cognition 91, pp. 259–272.
Brotherton, P. N. M. and Komers, P. E. (2003). “Mate guarding and the evolution of social monogamy in mammals,” in Monogamy: Mating strategies and partnerships in birds, humans and other mammals, ed. Reichard, U. H. and Boesch, C. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 42–58).
Browman, C. and Goldstein, L. (1986). “Towards an articulatory phonology,” Phonology Yearbook 3, pp. 219–252.
Browman, C. and Goldstein, L. (1989). “Articulatory gestures as phonological units,” Phonology 6, pp. 201–251.
Browman, C. and Goldstein, L. (1992). “Articulatory phonology: An overview,” Phonetica 49, pp. 155–180.
Brown, J. L. (1978). “Avian communal breeding systems,” Annual Review of Ecology & Systematics 9, pp. 123–155.
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language: The early stages (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Brown, S. (2000). “The ‘Musilanguage’ model of music evolution,” in The Origins of Music, ed. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B., and Brown, S. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 271–300.
Brunet, M., Guy, F., Pilbeam, D., Lieberman, D. E., Likius, A., Leon, M. P. D., Zollikofer, C., and Vignaud, P. (2005). “New material of the earliest hominid from the Upper Miocene of Chad,” Nature 434, pp. 752–755.
Bshary, R., Wickler, W., and Fricke, H. (2002). “Fish cognition: A primate's eye view,” Animal Cognition 5, pp. 1–13.
Bugnyar, T. (2007). “An integrative approach to the study of ‘theory-of-mind’-like abilities in ravens,” The Japanese Journal of Animal Psychology 57, pp. 15–27.
Bugnyar, T. and Heinrich, B. (2005). “Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledgable and ignorant competitors,” Proceedings of the Royal Society B 272, pp. 1641–1646.
Bugnyar, T., Stöwe, M., and Heinrich, B. (2004). “Ravens, Corvus corax, follow gaze direction of humans around obstacles,” Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 271, pp. 1331–1336.
Burling, R. (2005). The Talking Ape: How language evolved (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Burnet, J. (1967 [1773]). Of the Origin and Progress of Language (Menston: Scholar Press).
Burnstein, D. D. and Wolff, P. C. (1967). “Vocal conditioning in the guinea pig,” Psychonomic Science 8, pp. 39–40.
Burt, A. and Trivers, R. L. (2006). Genes in Conflict: The biology of selfish genetic elements (Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press).
Burton, D. and Ettlinger, G. (1960). “Cross-modal transfer of training in monkeys,” Nature 186, pp. 1071–1072.
Buss, D. M. (1994). The Evolution of Desire (New York: Basic Books).
Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., and Wakefield, J. C. (1998). “Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels,” American Psychologist 53, pp. 533–548.
Bybee, J. L. (1998). “A functionalist approach to grammar and its evolution,” Evolution of Communication 2, pp. 249–278.
Bybee, J. L. and Hopper, P. (eds) (2001). Frequency and the Emergence of Linguistic Structure (Amsterdam: John Benjamins).
Byrne, R. W. (1997). “Machiavellian intelligence,” Evolutionary Anthropology 5, pp. 172–180.
Byrne, R. W. and Bates, L. A. (2006). “Why are animals cognitive,” Current Biology 16, pp. 445–448.
Byrne, R. W. and Russon, A. E. (1998). “Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, pp. 667–684.
Byrne, R. W. and Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans (Oxford: Clarendon Press).
Call, J., Braueur, J., Kaminski, J., and Tomasello, M. (2003). “Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans,” Journal of Comparative Psychology 117, pp. 257–263.
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2007). The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys (London: Lawrence Erlbaum).
Calvin, W. H. (2003). A Brain for All Seasons (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press).
Calvin, W. H. and Bickerton, D. (2000). Lingua Ex Machina: Reconciling Darwin with the human brain (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Cameron, D. W. (2004). Hominid Adaptations and Extinctions (Sydney: University of New South Wales Press).
Camper, P. (1779). “Account of the organs of speech of the Orang Outang,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 69, pp. 139–159.
Cangelosi, A. and Parisi, D. (eds) (2002). Simulating the Evolution of Language (New York, NY: Springer).
Cann, R. L., Stoneking, M., and Wilson, A. C. (1987). “Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution,” Nature 325, pp. 31–36.
Caplan, D. (1987). Neurolinguistics and Linguistic Aphasiology (New York, NY: McGraw Hill).
Caramazza, A. and Zurif, E. B. (1976). “Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasi,” Brain and Language 3, pp. 572–582.
Carey, S. (1978). “The child as word learner,” in Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality, ed. Halle, M., Bresnan, J., and Miller, G. A. (Cambridge., MA: MIT Press, pp. 264–293).
Carlsson, P. and Mahlapuu, M. (2002). “Forkhead transcription factors: Key players in development and metabolism,” Developmental Biology 250, pp. 1–23.
Carnie, A. (2002). Syntax: A generative introduction (Oxford: Blackwell).
Carrano, M. T. (2000). “Homoplasy and the evolution of dinosaur locomotion,” Paleobiology 26, pp. 489–512.
Carré, R., Lindblom, B., and MacNeilage, P. (1995). “Acoustic factors in the evolution of the human vocal tract,” Compte Rendu Academie des Sciences, Paris, IIb 320, pp. 471–476.
Carrier, D. R. (1984). “The energetic paradox of human running and hominid evolution,” Current Anthropology 25, pp. 483–495.
Carroll, S. B. (2000). “Endless forms: The evolution of gene regulation and morphological diversity,” Cell 101, pp. 577–580.
Carroll, S. B. (2003). “Genetics and the making of Homo sapiens,” Nature 422, pp. 849–857.
Carroll, S. B. (2005a). Endless Forms Most Beautiful (New York, NY: W. W. Norton).
Carroll, S. B. (2005b). “Evolution at two levels: On genes and form,” PLOS Biology 3, p. e245.
Carroll, S. B. (2006). The Making of the Fittest: DNA and the ultimate forensic record of evolution (New York, NY: W. W. Norton).
Carroll, S. B., Grenier, J. K., and Weatherbee, S. D. (2001). From DNA to Diversity: Molecular genetics and the evolution of animal design (Malden, MA: Blackwell Science).
Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1998). “Synonymy avoidance, phonology, and the origin of syntax,” in Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and cognitive bases, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 279–296).
Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1999). The Origins of Complex Language (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Carterette, E. C., Shipley, C., and Buchwald, J. S. (1984). “On synthesizing animal speech: The case of the cat,” in Electronic Speech Synthesis: Techniques, technology, and applications, ed. Bristow, G. (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill, pp. 292–302).
Cartmill, E. A. and Byrne, R. W. (2007). “Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their audience's comprehension,” Current Biology 17, pp. 1345–1348.
Catchpole, C. K. (1980). “Sexual selection and the evolution of complex songs among warblers of the genus Acrocephalus,” Behaviour 74, pp. 149–166.
Catchpole, C. K. and Slater, P. L. B. (1995). Bird Song: Themes and variations (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press).
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1997). “Genes, peoples, and languages,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 94, pp. 7719–7724.
Cela-Conde, C. J. and Ayala, F. J. (2003). “Genera of the human lineage,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 100, pp. 7684–7689.
Chappell, J. and Kacelnik, A. (2002). “Tool selectivity in a non-primate, the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides),” Animal Cognition 5, pp. 71–78.
Charlton, B. D., Reby, D., and McComb, K. (2008). “Effect of combined source F0 and filter (formant) variation on red deer hind responses to male roars,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123, pp. 2936–2943.
Charrier, I., Mathevon, N., and Jouventin, P. (2001). “Mother's voice recognition by seal pups,” Nature 412, p. 873.
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1980). “Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys,” Animal Behavavior 28, pp. 362–367.
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1985). “Vervet monkey alarm calls: Manipulation through shared information?,” Behaviour 94, pp. 150–166.
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1988). “Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys,” Animal Behavavior 36, pp. 477–486.
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1990a). “Attending to behaviour versus attending to knowledge: Examining monkeys' attribution of mental states,” Animal Behavavior 40, pp. 742–753.
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1990b). How Monkeys See the World: Inside the mind of another species (Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press).
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1998). “Why monkeys don't have language,” in The Tanner Lectures on Human Values, ed. Petersen, G. (Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press).
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon Metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press).
Chiba, T. and Kajiyama, M. (1941). The Vowel: Its nature and structure (Tokyo: Tokyo-Kaiseikan).
,Chimpanzee Sequencing and Analysis Consortium, The (2005). “Initial sequence of the chimpanzee genome and comparison with the human genome,” Nature 437, pp. 69–87.
Chomsky, C. (1969). The Acquisition of Syntax in Children from 5 to 10 (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Chomsky, N. (1956). “Three models for the description of language,” I.R.E. Transactions on Information Theory IT-2, pp. 113–124.
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures (The Hague: Mouton).
Chomsky, N. (1959). “Review of ‘Verbal Behavior’ by B. F. Skinner,” Language 35, pp. 26–58.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Chomsky, N. (1975a). Reflections on Language (New York, NY: Pantheon).
Chomsky, N. (1975b). The Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory (New York, NY: Plenum Press).
Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and Representations (Oxford: Blackwell).
Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of Language: Its nature, origin, and use (Westport, CT: Praeger).
Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and Problems of Knowledge: The Managua lectures (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Chomsky, N. (1990). “On formalization and formal linguistics,” Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 8, pp. 143–147.
Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Chomsky, N. (2005). “Three factors in language design,” Linguistic Inquiry 36, pp. 1–22.
Chomsky, N. (2010). “Some simple evo devo theses: How true might they be for language?,” in The Evolution of Human Language: Biolinguistic perspectives, ed. Larson, R., Deprez, V., and Yamakido, H. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chomsky, N. and Halle, M. (1968). The Sound Pattern of English (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Chomsky, N. and Miller, G. A. (1963). “Introduction to the formal analysis of natural languages,” in Handbook of Mathematical Psychology, ed. Luce, R. D., Bush, R. R., and Galanter, E. (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 269–322).
Christiansen, M. and Chater, N. (2008). “Language as shaped by the brain,” Behavioral & Brain Sciences 31, pp. 489–509.
Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S. (2003). “Language evolution: Consensus and controversies,” Trends in Cogntive Science 7, pp. 300–307.
Clack, J. A. (1992). “The stapes of Acanthostega gunnari and the role of the stapes in early tetrapods,” in The Evolutionary Biology of Hearing, ed. Webster, D. B., Fay, R. F., and Popper, A. N. (New York, NY: Springer-Verlag, pp. 405–420).
Clack, J. A. (1994). “The earliest known tetrapod braincase and the evolution of the stapes and fenstra ovalis,” Nature 369, pp. 392–394.
Clack, J. A. (1997). “The evolution of tetrapod ears and the fossil record,” Brain Behavior and Evolution 50, pp. 198–212.
Clark, A. G., Glanowski, S., Nielsen, R., Thomas, P. D., Kejariwal, A., Todd, M. A., Tanenbaum, D. M., Civello, D., Lu, F., Murphy, B., Ferriera, S., Wang, G., Zheng, X., White, T. J., Sninsky, J. J., Adams, M. D., and Cargill, M. (2003). “Inferring nonneutral evolution from human-chimp-mouse orthologous gene trios,” Science 302, pp. 1960–1963.
Clark, C. W., Borsani, J. F., and Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, G. (2002). “Vocal activity of fin whales, Balaenoptera physalus, in the Ligurian Sea,” Marine Mammal Science 18, pp. 286–295.
Clark, D. A., Mitra, P. P., and Wang, S. S. (2001). “Scalable architecture in mammalian brains,” Nature 411, pp. 189–193.
Clark, E. V. (1987). “The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition,” in Mechanisms of Language Acquisition, ed. MacWhinney, B. (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 1–33).
Clark, G. (1971). World Prehistory: A new outline (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Clarke, E., Reichard, U. H., and Zuberbühler, K. (2006). “The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon calls,” PLOS ONE 1, p. e73.
Clayton, M. R. L. (1996). “Free rhythm: Ethnomusicology and the study of music without metre,” Bulletin of the School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London 59, pp. 323–332.
Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J., and Dickinson, A. (2003a). “Can animals recall the past and plan for the future?,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4, pp. 685–691.
Clayton, N. S. and Dickinson, A. D. (1998). “Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays,” Nature 395, pp. 272–278.
Clayton, N. S., Yu, K. S., and Dickinson, A. (2003b). “Interacting cache memories: Evidence of flexible memory use by scrub jays,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 29, pp. 14–22.
Clegg, M. and Aiello, L. C. (2000). “Paying the price of speech? An analysis of mortality statistics for choking on food,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 111, p. 126.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1991). The Evolution of Parental Care (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Clutton-Brock, T. H. and Harvey, P. H. (1980). “Primates, brains and ecology,” Journal of Zoology (London) 207, pp. 151–169.
Coates, M. I. and Clack, J. A. (1990). “Polydactyly in the earliest known tetrapod limbs,” Nature 347, pp. 66–69.
Coates, M. I. and Clack, J. A. (1991). “Fish-like gills and breathing in the earliest known tetrapod,” Nature 352, pp. 234–235.
Cole, M., Chorover, S. L., and Ettlinger, G. (1961). “Cross-modal transfer in man,” Nature 191, pp. 1225–1226.
Collins, R. L. (1970). “The sound of one paw clapping: An inquiry into the origins of left handedness,” in Contributions to Behavior-Genetic Analysis: The mouse as a prototype, ed. Lindzey, G. and Thiessen, D. D. (New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Croft, pp. 115–136).
Condillac, É. B. d. (1971 [1747]). Essai sur l'origine des connaissances humaines (Gainesville, FL: Scholar's Facsimiles and Reprints).
Connor, R. C. and Peterson, D. M. (1994). The Lives of Whales and Dolphins (New York, NY: Henry Holt).
Cope, D. (1996). Experiments in Musical Intelligence (Madison: WI: A-R Editions).
Coppens, Y. (1994). “East Side Story: The origin of humankind,” Scientific American May, pp. 88–95.
Coqueugniot, H., Hublin, J.-J., Veillon, F., Houët, F., and Jacob, T. (2004). “Early brain growth in Homo erectus and implications for cognitive ability,” Nature 431, pp. 299–302.
Corballis, M. C. (1983). Human Laterality (New York, NY: Academic Press).
Corballis, M. C. (1991). The Lopsided Ape (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Corballis, M. C. (2002a). “Did language evolve from manual gestures?,” in The Transition to Language, ed. Wray, A. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 161–179).
Corballis, M. C. (2002b). From Hand to Mouth: The origins of language (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Corballis, M. C. (2003). “From hand to mouth: The gestural origins of language,” in Language Evolution, ed. Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 201–218).
Coutinho, C. C., Fonseca, R. N., Mansurea, J. J. C., and Borojevic, R. (2003). “Early steps in the evolution of multicellularity: Deep structural and functional homologies among homeobox genes in sponges and higher metazoans,” Mechanisms of Development 120, pp. 429–440.
Cracraft, J. and Donoghue, M. J. (eds) (2004). Assembling the Tree of Life (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Crain, S. (1991). “Language acquisition in the absence of experience,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14, pp. 597–650.
Crelin, E. (1987). The Human Vocal Tract (New York, NY: Vantage Press).
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L., and Boesch, C. (2004). “Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning?,” Ethology 110, pp. 221–243.
Croft, W. and Cruse, D. A. (2003). Cognitive Linguistics (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Cross, I. (2003). “Music, cognition, culture and evolution,” in The Cognitive Neuroscience of Music, ed. Peretz, I. and Zatorre, R. J. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 42–56).
Crothers, J. (1978). “Typology and universals of vowel systems,” in Universals of Human Language, ed. Greenberg, J., Ferguson, C. A., and Moravcsik, E. A. (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, pp. 93–152).
Crystal, D. (2002). The Cambridge Encyclopedia of Language (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Curio, E. (1978). “The adaptive significance of avian mobbing I: Teleonomic hypotheses and predictions,” Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 48, pp. 175–183.
Curio, E., Ernst, V., and Vieth, W. (1978). “Cultural transmission of enemy recognition: One function of mobbing,” Science 202, pp. 899–901.
Curtis, S. (1977). Genie: A psycholinguistic study of a modern-day “wild child” (New York, NY: Academic Press).
Cutting, J. E. (1982). “Plucks and bows are categorically perceived, sometimes,” Perception & Psychophysics 31, pp. 462–476.
Cutting, J. E. and Rosner, B. S. (1974). “Category boundaries in speech and music,” Perception & Psychophysics 16, pp. 564–570.
Dart, R. A. (1925). “Australopithecus africanus: The ape-man of South Africa,” Nature 115, pp. 195–199.
Darwin, C. (1859). On the Origin of Species (London: John Murray).
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex (London: John Murray).
Darwin, C. (1872a). On the Origin of Species (London: John Murray).
Darwin, C. (1872b). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals (London: John Murray).
Darwin, C. (1875). The Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication (London: John Murray).
Daumer, G. F. (1873). Kaspar Hauser: Sein Wesen, seine Unschuld (Leipaig: Dornach).
Davenport, R. K. and Rogers, C. M. (1970). “Intermodal equivalence of stimuli in apes,” Science 168, pp. 279–280.
Davidson, I. and Noble, W. (1993). “Tools and language in human evolution,” in Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution, ed. by Gibson, K. R. and Ingold, T.. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 363–388).
Davis, H. (1992). “Transitive inference in rats (Rattus norvegicus),” Journal of Comparative Psychology 106, pp. 342–349.
Davis, M. (1958). Computability and Unsolvability (New Yorl, NY: McGraw-Hill).
Davis, M. (ed.) (1965). The Undecidable: Basic papers on undecidable propositions, unsolvable problems and computable functions (Hewlett, NY: Raven Press).
Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Dawkins, R. (1986). The Blind Watchmaker (New York, NY: W.W. Norton).
Dawkins, R. (2004). The Ancestor's Tale (New York, NY: W. W. Norton).
Dawkins, R. and Krebs, J. R. (1978). “Animal signals: Information or manipulation?,” in Behavioural Ecology, ed. Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B. (Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, pp. 282–309).
Day, M. H. and Williams, E. H. (1980). “Laetoli Pliocene hominid footprints and bipedalism,” Nature 286, pp. 385–387.
Deacon, T. W. (1984). “Connections of the inferior periarcuate area in the brain of Macaca fascicularis: An experimental and comparative investigation of language circuitry and its evolution.” Unpublished PhD thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.
Deacon, T. W. (1990a). “Fallacies of progression in theories of brain-size evolution,” International Journal of Primatology 11, pp. 193–235.
Deacon, T. W. (1990b). “Problems of ontogeny and phylogeny in brain-size evolution,” International Journal of Primatology 11, pp. 237–282.
Deacon, T. W. (1992). “The neural circuitry underlying primate calls and human language,” in Language Origins: A multidisciplinary approach, ed. Wind, J., Chiarelli, B. A., Bichakjian, B., and Nocentini, A. (Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic, pp. 301–323).
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The co-evolution of language and the brain (New York, NY: Norton).
Deaner, R. O., Nunn, C. L., and Schaik, C. P. (2000). “Comparative tests of primate cognition: Different scaling methods produce different results,” Brain Behavior and Evolution 55, pp. 44–52.
Beer, G. (1971). Homology: An unsolved problem (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Boer, B. (2001). The Origins of Vowel Systems (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Dediu, D. and Ladd, D. R. (2007). “Linguistic tone is related to the population frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two brain size genes, ASPM and Microcephalin,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104, pp. 10944–10949.
DeGusta, D., Gilbert, W. H., and Turner, S. P. (1999). “Hypoglossal canal size and hominid speech,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA 96, pp. 1800–1804.
Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Demski, L. S. and Gerald, J. W. (1974). “Sound production and other behavioral effects of midbrain stimulation in free-swimming toadfish Opsanus beta,” Brain, Behavior and Evolution 9, pp. 41–59.
Dennett, D. C. (1983). “Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The ‘Panglossian paradigm’ defended,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6, pp. 343–390.
Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness Explained (Boston, MA: Little, Brown).
Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin's Dangerous Idea (New York, NY: Simon & Schuster).
Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of Minds (New York, NY: Basic Books).
Robertis, E. M. and Sasai, Y. (1996). “A common plan for dorsoventral patterning in Bilateria,” Nature 380, pp. 37–40.
D'Errico, F. (2003). “The invisible frontier: A multiple-species model for the origin of behavioral modernity,” Evolutionary Anthropology 12, pp. 188–202.
D'Errico, F., Villa, P., Llona, A. C. P., and Idarraga, R. R. (1998). “A Middle Palaeolithic origin of music? Using cave-bear bone accumulations to assess the Divje Babe I bone ‘flute,’Antiquity 72, pp. 65–76.
Dessalles, J.-L. (1998). “Altruism, status and the origin of relevance,” in Approaches to the Evolution of Language, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 130–147).
Dessalles, J.-L. (2000). “Language and hominid politics,” in The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form, ed. Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Hurford, J. R. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 62–80).
Devlin, A. M., Cross, J. H., Harkness, W., Chong, W. K., Harding, B., Vargha-Khadem, F., and Neville, B. G. R. (2003). “Clinical outcomes of hemispherectomy for epilepsy in childhood and adolescence,” Brain 126, pp. 556–566.
Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Peacemaking Among Primates (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Waal, F. B. M. (1988). “The communicative repertoire of captive bonobos (Pan paniscus), compared to that of chimpanzees,” Behaviour 106, pp. 183–251.
Diamond, J. (1992). The Third Chimpanzee (New York, NY: HarperCollins).
Diamond, J. (1997). Guns, Germs and Steel (New York, NY: W. W. Norton).
Dissanayake, E. (1992). Homo Aestheticus: Where art comes from and why (New York, NY: Free Press).
Dissanayake, E. (2000). “Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother–infant interaction,” in The Origins of Music, ed. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B., and Brown, S. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 389–410).
Dobzhansky, T. (1973). “Nothing in biology makes sense except in the light of evolution,” American Biology Teacher 35, pp. 125–129.
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Donald, M. (1998). “Mimesis and the executive suite: Missing links in language evolution,” in Approaches to the Evolution of Language, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 44–67).
Dorsaint-Pierre, R., Penhune, V. B., Watkins, K. E., Neelin, P., Lerch, J. P., Bouffard, M., and Zatorre, R. J. (2006). “Asymmetries of the planum temporale and Heschl's gyrus: Relationship to language lateralization,” Brain 129, pp. 1164–1176.
Doupe, A. J. and Kuhl, P. K. (1999). “Birdsong and human speech: Common themes and mechanisms,” Annual Review of Neuroscience 22, pp. 567–631.
Drayna, D., Manichaikul, A., Lange, M., Snieder, H., and Spector, T. (2001). “Genetic correlates of musical pitch recognition in humans,” Science 291, pp. 1969–1972.
Dronkers, N. F. and Baldo, J. V. (2001). “Neural basis of speech production,” in International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, ed. Smelser, N. J. and Baltes, P. B. (Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 14875–14879).
Dubois, E. (1897). “Sur le rapport du poids de l'encéphale avec la grandeur du corps chez mammifères,” Bulletin et Mémoires de la Société d'Anthropologie de Paris 8, pp. 337–376.
Dubois, E. (1898). “Abstract of remarks on the brain-cast of Pithecanthropus erectus,” Journal of Anatomy and Physiology 33, pp. 273–276.
DuBrul, E. L. (1958). Evolution of the Speech Apparatus (Springfield, IL: Thomas).
DuBrul, E. L. (1962). “The general phenomenon of bipedalism,” American Zoologist 2, pp. 205–208.
Duchin, L. E. (1990). “The evolution of articulate speech: Comparative anatomy of the oral cavity in Pan and Homo,” Journal of Human Evolution 19, pp. 684–695.
Dudley, H. and Tarnoczy, T. H. (1950). “The speaking machine of Wolfgang von Kempelen,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 22, pp. 151–166.
Dugatkin, L. A. (1993). “Sexual selection and imitation: Females copy the mate choice of others,” American Naturalist 139, pp. 1384–1389.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1992). “Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates,” Journal of Human Evolution 20, pp. 469–493.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). “Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16, pp. 681–735.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). “Theory of mind and the evolution of language,” in Approaches to the Evolution of Language, ed. Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 92–110).
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1999). “Culture, honesty and the Free Rider Problem,” in The Evolution of Culture, ed. Dunbar, R. I. M., Knight, C., and Power, C.. (Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, pp. 194–213).
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2003). “The origin and subsequent evolution of language,” in Language Evolution, ed. Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S. (Oxford: Oxford Unviersity Press, pp. 219–234).
Dunford, C. (1977). “Kin selection for ground squirrel alarm calls,” American Naturalist 111, pp. 782–785.
Durham, W. (1991). Coevolution: Genes, culture, & human diversity (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press).
Eaton, R. L. (1979). “A beluga whale imitates human speech,” Carnivore 2, pp. 22–23.
Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection (New York, NY: Basic Books).
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1970). Ethology: The biology of behavior (New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston).
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1973). “The expressive behaviour of the deaf- and blind-born,” in Social Communication and Movement, ed. Von. Cranach, M. and Vine, J. (London: Academic, pp. 163–194).
Ekman, P. (1992). “Facial expressions of emotion: An old controversy and new findings,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 335, pp. 63–70.
Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the Face (Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall).
Elder, J. H. (1934). “Auditory acuity of the chimpanzee,” Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 17, pp. 157–183.
Elowson, A. M., Snowdon, C. T., and Lazaro-Perea, C. (1998a). “‘Babbling’ and social context in infant monkeys: Parallels to human infants,” Trends in Cognitive Science 2, pp. 31–37.
Elowson, A. M., Snowdon, C. T., and Lazaro-Perea, C. (1998b). “Infant ‘babbling’ in a nonhuman primate: Complex vocal sequences with repeated call types,” Behaviour 135, pp. 643–664.
Emery, N. J. and Clayton, N. S. (2001). “Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies in scrub jays,” Nature 414, pp. 443–446.
Emery, N. J. and Clayton, N. S. (2004). “The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes,” Science 306, pp. 1903–1907.
Emlen, S. T. and Oring, L. W. (1977). “Ecology, sexual selection and the evolution of mating systems,” Science 197, pp. 215–223.
Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, Cognition and the Brain: Insights from sign language research (London: Lawrence Erlbaum).
Emmorey, K. (2005). “Sign languages are problematic for a gestural origins theory of language evolution,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28, pp. 130–131.
Enard, W., Gehre, S., Hammerschmidt, K., Holter, S. M., Blass, T., Somel, M. et al., (2009). “A humanized version of Foxp2 affects cortico-basal ganglia circuits in mice,” Cell 137(5), pp. 961–971.
Enard, W., Przeworski, M., Fisher, S. E., Lai, C. S. L., Wiebe, V., Kitano, T., Monaco, A. P., and Paäbo, S. (2002). “Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language,” Nature 418, pp. 869–872.
Endler, J. A. (1986). “The newer synthesis? Some conceptual problems in evolutionary biology,” Oxford Surveys in Evolutionary Biology 3, pp. 224–243.
Enquist, M. (1985). “Communication during aggressive interactions with particular reference to variation in choice of behaviour,” Animal Behavior 33, pp. 1152–1161.
Enquist, M. and Leimar, O. (1993). “The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms,” Animal Behavior 45, pp. 747–757.
Erwin, D. H. and Davidson, E. H. (2002). “The last common bilaterian ancestor,” Development 129, pp. 3021–3032.
Etcoff, N. L. and Magee, J. J. (1992). “Categorical perception of facial expressions,” Cognition 44, pp. 227–240.
Ettlinger, G. and Blakemore, C. B. (1969). “Cross-modal transfer set in the monkey,” Neuropsychologia 7, pp. 41–47.
Evans, C. S. and Evans, L. (2007). “Representational signalling in birds,” Biology Letters 3, pp. 8–11.
Evans, C. S., Evans, L., and Marler, P. (1993). “On the meaning of alarm calls: Functional reference in an avian vocal system,” Animal Behavior 46, pp. 23–38.
Evans, C. S. and Marler, P. (1994). “Food-calling and audience effects in male chickens, Gallus gallus: Their relationships to food availability, courtship and social facilitation,” Animal Behavior 47, pp. 1159–1170.
Evans, P. D., Anderson, J. R., Vallender, E. J., Gilbert, S. L., Malcom, C. M., Dorus, S., and Lahn, B. T. (2004). “Adaptive evolution of ASPM, a major determinant of cerebral cortical size in humans,” Human Molecular Genetics 13, pp. 489–494.
Evans, P. D., Gilbert, S. L., Mekel-Bobrov, N., Vallender, E. J., Anderson, J. R., Vaez-Azizi, L., Tishkoff, S. A., Hudson, R. R., and Lahn, B. T. (2005). “Microcephalin, a gene regulating brain size, continues to evolve adaptively in humans,” Science 309, pp. 1717–1720.
Evans, W. E. and Bastian, J. R. (1969). “Marine mammal communication: Social and ecological factors,” in The Biology of Marine Mammals, ed. Andersen, H. T. (New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 425–475).
Falk, D. (1975). “Comparative anatomy of the larynx in man and the chimpanzee: Implications for language in Neanderthal,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 43, pp. 123–132.
Falk, D. (1980). “A reanalysis of the South African Australopithecine natural endocasts,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 53, pp. 525–539.
Falk, D. (1983). “Cerebral cortices of East African early hominids,” Science 221, pp. 1072–1074.
Falk, D. (1987). “Hominid paleoneurology,” Annual Review of Anthropology 16, pp. 13–30.
Falk, D. (2000). “Hominid brain evolution and the origins of music,” in The Origins of Music, ed. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B., and Brown, S. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 197–216).
Falk, D. (2004). “Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese?,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27, pp. 491–450.
Falk, D., Hildebolt, C., Smith, K., Morwood, M. J., Sutikna, T., Brown, P., Jatmiko, Saptomo, E. W., Brunsden, B., and Prior, F. (2005). “The Brain of LB1, Homo floresiensis,” Science 308, pp. 242–245.
Fant, G. (1960). Acoustic Theory of Speech Production (The Hague: Mouton).
Fant, G. (1975). “Non-uniform vowel normalization,” Speech Transactions Laboratory Quarterly Progress and Status Report 2–3, pp. 1–19.
Farrar, F. W. (1870). “Philology & Darwinism,” Nature 1, pp. 527–529.
Farrar, F. W. (1996). “On language,” in The Origin of Language, ed. Harris, R. (Bristol: Thoemmes Press, pp. 42–80).
Fay, D. and Cutler, A. (1977). “Malapropisms and the structure of the mental lexicon,” Linguistic Inquiry 8, pp. 505–520.
Feher, O., Mitra, P. P., Sasahara, K., and Tchernichovski, O. (2008). “Evolution of song culture in the zebra finch,” in The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 7th International Conference, ed. Smith, A., Smith, K., and Ferrer, R. i Cancho (Singapore: World Scientific Press, pp. 423–424).
Feldman, M. W. and Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1976). “Cultural and biological evolutionary processes, selection for a trait under complex transmission,” Theoretical Population Biology 9, pp. 238–259.
Feldman, M. W. and Laland, K. N. (1996). “Gene-culture coevolutionary theory,” Trends in Ecology and Evolution 11, pp. 453–457.
Fernald, A. (1992). “Human maternal vocalizations to infants as biologically relevant signals: An evolutionary perspective,” in The Adapted Mind, ed. Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 391–428).
Ferrari, P. F., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2001). “Mirror neurons for mouth actions in monkey ventral premotor cortex,” Society for Neurosciences Abstracts 27, No. 729.4.
Feuerbach, P. J. A. (1832). Kaspar Hauser: Beispiel eines Verbrechens am Seelenleben des Menschen (Ansbach: Dolfuss).
Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978). “Vocal repertoire of the black-capped chickadee,” Auk 95, pp. 34–48.
Ficken, M. S. and Witkin, S. R. (1977). “Responses of black-capped chickadees to predators,” Auk 94, pp. 156–157.
Finlay, B. L. and Darlington, R. B. (1995). “Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains,” Science 268, pp. 1578–1584.
Finlay, B. L., Darlington, R. B., and Nicastro, N. (2001). “Developmental structure in brain evolution,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24, pp. 263–308.
Firth, J. R. (1930). Speech (London: Ernest Benn).
Firth, J. R. (1937). The Tongues of Men (London: Ernest Benn).
Fischer, J. (1998). “Barbary macaques categorize shrill barks into two call types,” Animal Behavioe 55, pp. 799–807.
Fisher, C. (2002). “The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: A reply to Tomasello (2000),” Cognition 82, pp. 259–278.
Fisher, R. A. (1930). The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection (Oxford: Clarendon Press).
Fisher, S. E. and DeFries, J. C. (2002). “Developmental dyslexia: Genetic dissection of a complex cognitive trait,” Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3, pp. 767–780.
Fisher, S. E., Vargha-Khadem, F., Watkins, K. E., Monaco, A. P., and Pembrey, M. E. (1998). “Localisation of a gene implicated in a severe speech and language disorder,” Nature Genetics 18, pp. 168–170.
Fitch, W. T. (1994). Vocal tract length perception and the evolution of language (UMI Dissertation Services, Ann Arbor, Michigan).
Fitch, W. T. (1997). “Vocal tract length and formant frequency dispersion correlate with body size in rhesus macaques,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 102, pp. 1213–1222.
Fitch, W. T.Acoustic exaggeration of size in birds by tracheal elongation: Comparative and theoretical analyses,” Journal of Zoology (London) 248, pp. 31–49.
Fitch, W. T. (2000a). “Skull dimensions in relation to body size in nonhuman mammals: The causal bases for acoustic allometry,” Zoology 103, pp. 40–58.
Fitch, W. T. (2000b). “The evolution of speech: A comparative review,” Trends in Cognitive Science 4, pp. 258–267.
Fitch, W. T. (2000c). “The phonetic potential of nonhuman vocal tracts: Comparative cineradiographic observations of vocalizing animals,” Phonetica 57, pp. 205–218.
Fitch, W. T. (2002). “Comparative vocal production and the evolution of speech: Reinterpreting the descent of the larynx,” in The Transition to Language, ed. Wray, A. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 21–45).
Fitch, W. T. (2004a). “Kin selection and ‘mother tongues’: A neglected component in language evolution,” in Evolution of Communication Systems: A comparative approach, ed. Oller, D. K. and Griebel, U. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 275–296).
Fitch, W. T. (2004b). “Vocal production system: Evolution,” in MIT Encyclopedia of Communication Sciences and Disorders, ed. Kent, R. D. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 56–59).
Fitch, W. T. (2005a). “Protomusic and protolanguage as alternatives to protosign,” Behavioral & Brain Sciences 28, pp. 132–133.
Fitch, W. T. (2005b). “The evolution of language: A comparative review,” Biology and Philosophy 20, pp. 193–230.
Fitch, W. T. (2005c). “The evolution of music in comparative perspective,” in The Neurosciences and Music II: From perception to performance, ed. Avanzini, G., Lopez, L., Koelsch, S., and Majno, M. (New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences, pp. 29–49).
Fitch, W. T. (2006a). “Production of vocalizations in mammals,” in Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, ed. Brown, K. (Oxford: Elsevier, pp. 115–121).
Fitch, W. T. (2006b). “The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective,” Cognition 100, pp. 173–215.
Fitch, W. T. (2007). “Evolving meaning: The roles of kin selection, allomothering and paternal care in language evolution,” in Emergence of Communication and Language, ed. Lyon, C., Nehaniv, C., and Cangelosi, A. (New York, NY: Springer, pp. 29–51).
Fitch, W. T. (2009). “Fossil cues to the evolution of speech,” in The Cradle of Language, ed. Botha, R. P. and Knight, C. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 112–134).
Fitch, W. T. and Fritz, J. B. (2006). “Rhesus macaques spontaneously perceive formants in conspecific vocalizations,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 120, pp. 2132–2141.
Fitch, W. T. and Giedd, J. (1999). “Morphology and development of the human vocal tract: A study using magnetic resonance imaging,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106, pp. 1511–1522.
Fitch, W. T. and Hauser, M. D. (1998). “Differences that make a difference: Do locus equations result from physical principles that characterize all mammalian vocal tracts?,” Behavioral & Brain Sciences 21, pp. 264–265.
Fitch, W. T. and Hauser, M. D. (2002). “Unpacking ‘honesty’: Vertebrate vocal production and the evolution of acoustic signals,” in Acoustic Communication, ed. Simmons, A. M., Fay, R. F., and Popper, A. N. (New York, NY: Springer, pp. 65–137).
Fitch, W. T. and Hauser, M. D. (2004). “Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate,” Science 303, pp. 377–380.
Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., and Chomsky, N. (2005). “The evolution of the Language Faculty: Clarifications and implications,” Cognition 97, pp. 179–210.
Fitch, W. T. and Kelley, J. P. (2000). “Perception of vocal tract resonances by whooping cranes, Grus americana,” Ethology 106, pp. 559–574.
Fitch, W. T. and Reby, D. (2001). “The descended larynx is not uniquely human,” Proceedings of the Royal Society London, B 268, pp. 1669–1675.
Fletcher, N. H. and Rossing, T. D. (1991). The Physics of Musical Instruments (New York, NY: Springer-Verlag).
Fobes, J. L. and King, J. E. (1982). “Measuring primate learning abilities,” in Primate Behavior, ed. Fobes, J. L. and King, J. E. (New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 289–326).
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Fodor, J. A. (2000). The Mind Doesn't Work That Way (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Foley, R. A. (1995). “The adaptive legacy of human evolution: A search for the environment of evolutionary adaptedness,” Evolutionary Anthropology 4, pp. 194–203.
Foley, R. A. (1998). “The context of human genetic evolution,” Genome Research 8, pp. 339–347.
Foley, R. A. and Lee, P. C. (1991). “Ecology and energetics of encephalization in hominid evolution,” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B 334, pp. 223–232.
Ford, J. K. B. and Fisher, H. D. (1983). “Group-specific dialects of killer whales (Orcinus orca) in British Columbia,” in Communication and Behavior of Whales, ed. Payne, R. (Boulder, CO: Westview Press, pp. 129–161).
Forey, P. L. and Janvier, P. (1993). “Agnathans and the origin of jawed vertebrates,” Nature 361, pp. 129–134.
Foster, K. R., Wenseleers, T., and Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2006). “Kin selection is the key to altruism,” Trends in Ecology and Evolution 21, pp. 57–60.
Fouts, R. and Mills, S. T. (1997). Next of Kin (New York, NY: Harper).
Fragaszy, D. M., Visalberghi, E., and Fedigan, L. M. (2004). The Complete Capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press).
Frank, S. A. (1998). Foundations of Social Evolution (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Freeman, S. (1987). “Male red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus) assess the RHP of neighbors by watching contests,” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 21, pp. 307–311.
Frey, R. and Riede, T. (2003). “Sexual dimorphism of the larynx of the Mongolian Gazelle (Procapra gutturosa Pallas, 1777) (Mammalia, Artiodactyla, Bovidae),” Zoologischer Anzeiger 242, pp. 33–62.
Friederici, A., Meyer, M., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2000). “Auditory language comprehension: An event-related fMRI study on the processing of syntactic and lexical information,” Brain and Language 74, pp. 289–300.
Friedmann, H. (1955). “The honey-guides,” Bulletin of the United States National Museum 208, pp. 1–292.
Frishberg, N. (1979). “Historical change: From iconic to arbitrary,” in The Signs of Language, ed. Klima, E. S. and Bellugi, U. (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 67–87).
Frith, U. (2001). “Mind blindness and the brain in autism,” Neuron 32, pp. 969–979.
Fromkin, V. A. (1973). Speech Errors as Linguistic Evidence (The Hague: Mouton).
Fromkin, V. A. (ed.) (1978). Tone: A linguistic survey (New York, NY: Academice Press).
Fruth, B. and Hohmann, G. (1996). “Nest building behaviour in the great apes: The great leap forward?,” in Great Ape Societies, ed. McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 225–240).
Futuyma, D. J. (1979). Evolutionary Biology (Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates).
Gabunia, L., Vekua, A., Lordkipanidze, D., Swisher, C. C., Ferring, R., Justus, A., et al. (2000). “Earliest Pleistocene hominid cranial remains from Dmanisi, Republic of Georgia: Taxonomy, geological setting, and age,” Science 288, pp. 1019–1025.
Galdikas, B. M. F. (1982). “Orang-utan tool use at Tanjung Puting Reserve, Central Indonesian Borneo (Kalimantan Tengah),” Journal of Human Evolution 10, pp. 19–33.
Galef, B. G. (1988). “Imitation in animals: History, definitions, and interpretation of data from the psychological laboratory,” in Social Learning: Psychological and biological perspectives, ed. Zentall, T. and Galef, B. G. (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 3–28).
Gallese, V. and Goldman, A. (1998). “Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading,” Trends in Cognitive Science 2, pp. 493–501.
Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The Organization of Learning (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Gallistel, C. R. (2000). “The replacement of general-purpose learning models with adaptively specialized learning modules,” in The New Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, M. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1179–1191).
Gallup, G. G.. (1970). “Chimpanzees: Self-recognition,” Science 167, pp. 86–87.
Gallup, G. G.. (1991). “Toward a comparative psychology of self-awareness: Species limitations and cognitive consequences,” in The Self: An interdisciplinary approach, ed. Goethals, G. R. and Strauss, J. (New York, NY: Springer-Verlag, pp. 121–135).
Gannon, P. J., Holloway, R. L., Broadfield, D. C., and Braun, A. R. (1998). “Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale: Humanlike pattern of Wernicke's brain language area homolog,” Science 279, pp. 220–222.
Gans, C. and Northcutt, R. G. (1983). “Neural crest and the origin of Vertebrates: A new head,” Science 220, pp. 268–274.
Garcia, J. and Koelling, R. A. (1966). “Relation of cue to consequences in avoidance learning,” Psychonomic Science 4, pp. 123–124.
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of Mind: The theory of multiple intelligences (London: Heinemann).
Gardner, H. (1985). The Mind's New Science: A history of the cognitive revolution (New York, NY: Basic Books).
Gardner, R. A. and Gardner, B. T. (1969). “Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee,” Science 165, pp. 664–672.
Garner, R. L. (1892). The Speech of Monkeys (London: William Heinemann).
Garrett, M. (1988). “Processes in language production,” in Linguistics: The Cambridge survey, Vol. III: Language: Psychological and biological aspects, ed. Newmeyer, F. J. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Gatesy, S. M. and Biewener, A. A. (1991). “Bipedal locomotion: Effects of speed, size and limb posture in birds and humans,” Journal of Zoology, London 224, pp. 127–147.
Gathercole, V. C. (1987). “The contrastive hypothesis for the acquisition of word meaning: A reconsideration of the theory,” Journal of Child Language 14, pp. 493–532.
Gaupp, E. (1904). “Das Hyobranchialskelet der Wirbeltiere,” Ergebnisse der Anatomie und Entwicklungsgeschichte 14, pp. 808–1048.
Gautier, J. P. (1971). “Etude morphologique et fonctionnelle des annexes extra-laryngées des cercopithecinae; liaison avec les cris d'espacement,” Biologica Gabonica 7, pp. 230–267.
Gazdar, G., Klein, E., Pullum, G. K., and Sag, I. (1985). Generalized Phrase Structure Grammar (Oxford: Basil Blackwell).
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). “Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition?,” Brain 123, pp. 1293–1326.
Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., and Keysers, C. (2006). “Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans,” Current Biology 16, pp. 1824–1829.
Gehring, W. J. and Ikeo, K. (1999). “Pax 6: Mastering eye morphogenesis and eye evolution,” Trends Genet 15, pp. 371–377.
Geissmann, T. (1984). “Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song analyzed in 2 hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus s H. lar),” Folia primatologica 42, pp. 216–225.
Geissmann, T. (1987). “Songs of hybrid gibbons Hylobates pileatus x H. lar,” International Journal of Primatology 8, p. 540.
Geissmann, T. (2000). “Gibbon song and human music from an evolutionary perspective,” in The Origins of Music, ed. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B., and Brown, S. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 103–123).
Geissmann, T. (2002). “Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs,” Biological Reviews 77, pp. 57–76.
Gentilucci, M. and Corballis, M. C. (2006). “From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition,” Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 30, pp. 949–960.
Gentner, T. Q., Fenn, K. M., Margoliash, D., and Nusbaum, H. C. (2006). “Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds,” Nature 440, pp. 1204–1207.
George, S. L. (1978). “A longitudinal and cross-sectional analysis of the growth of the post-natal cranial base angle,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 49, pp. 171–178.
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., and Király, I. (2002). “Rational imitation in preverbal infants,” Nature 415, p. 755.
Gerhart, J. and Kirschner, M. (1997). Cells, Embryos, and Evolution (Toronto: Blackwell Science).
Gersting, J. L. (1999). Mathematical Structures for Computer Science (New York, NY: W. H. Freeman).
Geschwind, N. (1970). “Intermodal equivalence of stimuli in apes,” Science 170, p. 1249.
Ghazanfar, A. A. and Hauser, M. D. (1999). “The neuroethology of primate vocal communication: Substrates for the evolution of speech,” Trends in Cognitive Science 3, pp. 377–384.
Giedd, J. N., Castellanos, F. X., Rajapakse, J. C., Vaituzis, A. C., and Rapoport, J. L. (1997). “Sexual dimorphism of the developing human brain,” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 21, pp. 1185–1201.
Gilbert, S. F. (2003). Developmental Biology (Sunderland, MA: Sinauer).
Gilbert, S. F., Opitz, J. M., and Raff, R. A. (1996). “Resynthesizing evolutionary and developmental biology,” Developmental Biology 173, pp. 357–372.
Gillan, D. D. D. (1981). “Reasoning in the chimpanzee II: Transitive inference,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 7, pp. 150–164.
Giurfa, M., Zhang, S., Jenett, A., Menzel, R., and Srinivasan, M. V. (2001). “The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect,” Nature 410, pp. 930–933.
Givón, T. (1995). Functionalism and Grammar (Amsterdam: John Benjamins).
Givón, T. (2002). Bio-Linguistics: The Santa Barbara lectures (Amsterdam: John Benjamins).
Glass, L. and Mackey, M. C. (1988). From Clocks to Chaos: The rhythms of life (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Gleason, J. B. (ed.) (2005). The Development of Language (Boston, MA: Pearson, Allyn & Bacon).
Gleitman, L. and Papafragou, A. (2005). “Language and thought,” in Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning, ed. Holyoak, K. J. and Morrison, R. G. (New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 633–661).
Goddard, C. and Wierzbicka, A. (eds) (2002). Meaning and Universal Grammar: Theory and empirical findings (Amsterdam: John Benjamins).
Godfray, H. C. J. (1991). “Signalling of need by offspring to their parents,” Nature 352, pp. 328–330.
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing Gesture: How our hands help us think (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Goldin-Meadow, S. and Mylander, C. (1998). “Spontaneous sign systems created by deaf children in two cultures,” Nature 391, pp. 278–281.
Goldsmith, J. A. (1990). Autosegmental and Metrical Phonology (Oxford: Blackwell).
Goldstein, L., Byrd, D., and Saltzman, E. (2006a). “The role of vocal tract gestural action units in understanding the evolution of phonology,” in From Action to Language via The mirror neuron system, ed. Arbib, M. A. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 215–249).
Goldstein, L., Whalen, D. H., and Best, C. T. (eds) (2006b). Laboratory Phonology 8 (Amsterdam: Walter de Gruyter).
Golinkoff, R. M., Mervis, C. B., and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1994). “Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework,” Journal of Child Language 21, pp. 125–155.
Goodall, J. (1968). “The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve, Tanzania,” Animal Behaviour Monographs 1, pp. 161–311.
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Goodman, N. (1983). Fact, Fiction and Forecast (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Goodwin, B. C. (2001). How the Leopard Changed its Spots: The evolution of complexity (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press).
Gopnik, M. (1990). “Feature-blind grammar and dysphasia,” Nature 344, p. 715.
Gottlieb, G. (1974). “On the acoustic basis of species identification in wood ducklings (Aix sponsa),” Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology 87, pp. 1038–1048.
Gottlieb, G. (1992). Individual Development and Evolution: The genesis of novel behavior (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).
Gould, J. L. and Marler, P. (1987). “Learning by instinct,” Scientific American 256, pp. 74–85.
Gould, S. J. (1975). “Allometry in primates, with emphasis on scaling and evolution of the brain,” in Approaches to Primate Paleobiology, ed. Szalay, F. (Basel: S. Karger, pp 244–292).
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and Phylogeny (Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press).
Gould, S. J. (1987). “Integrity and Mr. Rifkin,” in An Urchin in the Storm: Essays about books and ideas, ed. Gould, S. J. (New York: Norton, pp. 229–239).
Gould, S. J. (1991). “Exaptation: A crucial tool for evolutionary psychology,” Journal of Social Issues 47, pp. 43–65.
Gould, S. J. (1996). The Mismeasure of Man (New York, NY: W. Norton & Co.).
Gould, S. J. (1997). “The exaptive excellence of spandrels as a term and prototype,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 94, pp. 10750–10755.
Gould, S. J. (2002). The Structure of Evolutionary Theory (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Gould, S. J. and Lewontin, R. C. (1979). “The spandrels of San Marco and the panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme,” Proceedings of the Royal Society, B 205, pp. 581–598.
Gould, S. J. and Vrba, E. S. (1982). “Exaptation – a missing term in the science of form,” Paleobiology 8, pp. 4–15.
Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H., and Marler, P. (1984). “Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) screams: Representational signalling in the recruitment of agonistic aid,” Animal Behavior 32, pp. 182–193.
Gowlett, J. A. J. (1992). “Tools: The Paleolithic record,” in Cambridge Encyclopedia of Human Evolution, ed. Jones, S., Martin, R. D., and Pilbeam, D. R. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 350–360).
Grafen, A. (1982). “How not to measure inclusive fitness,” Nature 298, pp. 425–426.
Grafen, A. (1984). “Natural selection, kin selection and group selection,” in Behavioural Ecology, ed. Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B. (Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates, pp. 62–84).
Grafen, A. (1990a). “Biological signals as handicaps,” Journal of Theoretical Biology 144, pp. 517–546.
Grafen, A. (1990b). “Sexual selection unhandicapped by the Fisher process,” Journal of Theoretical Biology 144, pp. 473–516.
Gray, R. D. and Atkinson, Q. D. (2003). “Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin,” Nature 426, pp. 435–439.
Graybiel, A. M. (1994). “The basal ganglia and adaptive motor control,” Science 265, pp. 1826–1831.
Graybiel, A. M. (2005). “The basal ganglia: Learning new tricks and loving it,” Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15, pp. 638–644.
Greenewalt, C. H. (1968). Bird Song: Acoustics and physiology (Washington: Smithsonian Institution Press).
Greenfield, P. M. (1991). “Language, tools, and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14, pp. 531–595.
Greenfield, P. M. and Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1990). “Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: Processed of learning and invention in the evolution and development of language,” in “Language” and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes: Comparative developmental perspectives, ed. Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R. (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp. 540–578).
Grice, H. P. (1957). “Meaning,” Philosophical Review 66, pp. 377–388.
Grice, H. P. (1975). “Logic and conversation,” in The Logic of Grammar, ed. Davidson, D. and Harman, G. (Encino, CA: Dickenson, pp. 64–153).
Griffin, D. R. (1976). The Question of Animal Awareness (New York, NY: Rockefeller University Press).
Griffin, D. R. (1992). Animal Minds (Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press).
Griffin, D. R. (2001). Animal Minds: Beyond cognition to consciousness (Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press).
Groszer, M., Keays, D., Deacon, R., Bono, J., Prasad-Mulcare, S., Gaub, S., Baum, M., French, C., Nicod, J., Coventry, J., Enard, W., Fray, M., Brown, S. D. M.., Pääbo, S., Channon, K. M., Costas, R. M., Eilers, J., Ehret, G., Nicholas, J., Rawlins, P., and Fisher, S. E. (2008). “Impaired synaptic plasticity and motor learning in mice with a point mutation implicated in human speech deficits,” Current Biology 18, pp. 354–362.
Guilloud, N. B. and McClure, H. M. (1969). “Air sac infection in the Orang-utan,” Proceedings of the Second International Congress of Primatology 3, pp. 143–147.
Guinee, L. and Payne, K. (1988). “Rhyme-like repetition in songs of humpback whales,” Ethology 79, pp. 295–306.
Guttenplan, S. (1986). The Languages of Logic (Oxford: Blackwell).
Haesler, S., Rochefort, C., Geogi, B., Licznerski, P., Osten, P., and Scharff, C. (2007). “Incomplete and inaccurate vocal imitation after knockdown of FoxP2 in songbird basal ganglia nucleus area X,” PLOS Biology 5, p. e321.
Haesler, S., Wada, K., Nshdejan, A., Morrisey, E. E., Lints, T., Jarvis, E. D., and Scharff, C. (2004). “FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners,” Journal of Neuroscience 24, pp. 3164–3175.
Hahn, J. and Münzel, S. (1995). “Knochenflöten aus dem Aurignacien des Geissenklösterle bei Blaubeuren, Alb-Donau-Kreis,” Fundberichte aus Baden-Würtemberg 20, pp. 1–12.
Hailman, J. P. and Ficken, M. S. (1987). “Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: Chick-a-dee calling qualifies as ‘language’ by structural linguistics,” Animal Behavior 34, pp. 1899–1901.
Haldane, J. B. S. (1955). “Population genetics,” New Biology 18, pp. 34–51.
Hall, B. K. (ed.) (1994). Homology: The hierarchical basis of comparative biology (San Diego, CA: Academic Press).
Hall, B. K. (1998). Evolutionary Developmental Biology (London: Chapman & Hall).
Hall, K. and Schaller, G. B. (1964). “Tool using behavior of the California sea otter,” Journal of Mammalogy 45, pp. 287–298.
Hall, R. A. (1966). Pidgin and Creole Languages (Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press).
Hamilton, W. D. (1963). “The evolution of altruistic behavior,” American Naturalist 97, pp. 354–356.
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). “The genetical evolution of social behavior,” Journal of Theoretical Biology 7, pp. 1–52.
Hamilton, W. D. (1975). “Innate social aptitudes of man: An approach from evolutionary genetics,” in Biosocial Anthropology, ed. Fox, R. (New York, NY: John Wiley, pp. 133–155).
Hammerschmidt, K., Freudenstein, T., and Jürgens, U. (2001). “Vocal development in squirrel monkeys,” Behaviour 138, pp. 1179–1204.
Hammond, M. (1995). “Metrical phonology,” Annual Review of Anthropology 24, pp. 313–342.
Hampton, R. R. (1994). “Sensitivity to information specifying the line of gaze of humans in sparrows (Passer domesticus),” Behaviour 130, pp. 41–45.
Happé, F. G. E. (1995). “The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism,” Child Development 66, pp. 843–855.
Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., and Tomasello, M. (2002). “The domestication of social cognition in dogs,” Science 298, pp. 1634–1636.
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., and Tomasello, M. (2000). “Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see,” Animal Behavior 59, pp. 771–785.
Hare, B., Plyusnina, I., Ignacio, N., Schepina, O., Stepika, A., Wrangham, R. W., and Trut, L. (2005). “Social cognitive evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of experimental domestication,” Current Biology 15, pp. 226–230.
Hare, B. and Tomasello, M. (2004). “Chimpanzees are more skillful in competitive than cooperative cognitive tasks,” Animal Behavior 68, pp. 571–581.
Harnad, S. (1990). “The symbol grounding problem,” Physica D 42, pp. 335–346.
Harnad, S., Steklis, H. S., and Lancaster, J. (eds) (1976). Origin and Evolution of Language and Speech (New York: New York Academy of Sciences).
Harries, M. L. L., Hawkins, S., Hacking, J., and Hughes, I. (1998). “Changes in the male voice at puberty: Vocal fold length and its relationship to the fundamental frequency of the voice,” Journal of Laryngology & Otology 112, pp. 451–454.
Harris, R. (2007). “Concepts where there are none,” in Times Higher Education (London).
Harris, T. R, Fitch, W. T., Goldstein, L. M., and Fashing, P. J. (2006). “Black and white colobus monkey (Colobus guereza) roars as a source of both honest and exaggerated information about body mass,” Ethology 112, pp. 911–920.
Harris, Z. S. (1951). Methods in Structural Linguistics (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press).
Harrison, D. F. N. (1995). The Anatomy and Physiology of the Mammalian Larynx (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press).
Harvey, P. H. and Bradbury, J. W. (1991). “Sexual selection,” in Behavioural Ecology, ed. Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B. (Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Scientific Publications, pp. 203–233).
Harvey, P. H. and Pagel, M. D. (1991). The Comparative Method in Evolutionary Biology (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Haslinger, B., Erhard, P., Altenmuller, E., Schroeder, U., Boecker, H., and Ceballos-Baumann, A. O. (2005). “Transmodal sensorimotor networks during action observation in professional pianists,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17, pp. 282–293.
Haspelmath, M. (1999). “Why is grammaticalization irreversible?,” Linguistics 37, pp. 1043–1068.
Hast, M. (1983). “Comparative anatomy of the larynx: Evolution and function,” in Vocal Fold Physiology: Biomechanics, acoustics and phonatory control, ed. Titze, I. R. and Scherer, R. C. (Denver, CO: Denver Center for the Performing Arts, pp. 3–14).
Haug, H. (1987). “Brain sizes, surfaces, and neuronal sizes of the cortex cerebri: A stereological investigation of man and his variability and a comparison with some mammals (primates, whales, marsupials, insectivores, and one elephant),” American Journal of Anatomy 180, pp. 126–142.
Hausberger, M., Henry, L., and Richard, M. (1995a). “Testosterone-induced singing in female European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris),” Ethology 99, p. 193.
Hausberger, M., Richard-Yris, M.-A., Henry, L., Lepage, L., and Schmidt, I. (1995b). “Song sharing reflects the social organization in a captive group of European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris),” Journal of Comparative Psychology 109, pp. 222–241.
Hauser, M. D. (1988). “How infant vervet monkeys learn to recognize starling alarm calls,” Behaviour 105, pp. 187–201.
Hauser, M. D. (1992). “Costs of deception: Cheaters are punished in rhesus monkeys,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 89, pp. 12137–12139.
Hauser, M. D. (1996). The Evolution of Communication (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
Hauser, M. D. (2000). Wild Minds: What animals really think (New York, NY: Henry Holt).
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., and Fitch, W. T. (2002). “The Language Faculty: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?,” Science 298, pp. 1569–1579.
Hauser, M. D., Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., and Patalano, A. L. (2002b). “Spontaneous number discrimination of multi-format auditory stimuli in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus),” Cognition 86, pp. B23–B32.
Hauser, M. D. and Fitch, W. T. (2003). “What are the uniquely human components of the language faculty?,” in Language Evolution, ed. Christiansen, M. and Kirby, S. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 158–181).
Hauser, M. D. and Marler, P. (1993). “Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) I: Socioecological factors influencing call production,” Behavioral Ecology 4, pp. 194–205.
Hauser, M. D. and McDermott, J. (2003). “The evolution of the music faculty: A comparative perspective,” Nature Neuroscience 6, pp. 663–668.
Hauser, M. D. and Nelson, D. (1991). “Intentional signaling in animal communication,” Trends in Ecology and Evolution 6, pp. 186–189.
Hauser, M. D. and Schön Ybarra, M. (1994). “The role of lip configuration in monkey vocalizations: Experiments using xylocaine as a nerve block,” Brain and Language 46, pp. 232–244.
Hauser, M. D. and Wrangham, R. W. (1987). “Manipulation of food calls in captive chimpanzees: A preliminary report,” Folia primatologica 48, pp. 24–35.
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., Blurton Jones, N. G., Alvarez, H., and Charnov, E. L. (1998). “Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 95, pp. 1336–1339.
Hay, R. L. and Leakey, M. D. (1982). “The fossil footprints of Laetoli,” Scientific American 246, pp. 50–55.
Hayes, C. (1951). The Ape in Our House (New York, NY: Harper).
Hayes, K. J. and Hayes, C. (1951). “The intellectual development of a home-raised chimpanzee,” Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 95, pp. 105–109.
Healy, S. D. and Hurly, T. A. (2004). “Spatial learning and memory in birds,” Brain, Behavior and Evolution 63, pp. 211–220.
Heath, R. G. (1963). “Electrical self-stimulation of the brain in man,” American Journal of Psychiatry 120, pp. 571–577.
Heffner, R. S. (2004). “Primate hearing from a mammalian perspective,” Anatomical Record 281A, pp. 1111–1122.
Heimlich, H. J. (1975). “A life-saving maneuver to prevent food-choking,” Journal of the American Medical Association 234, pp. 398–401.
Heine, B., Claudi, U., and Hünnemeyer, F. (1991). Grammaticalization: A conceptual framework (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press).
Heine, B. and Kuteva, T. (2002). “On the evolution of grammatical forms,” in The Transition to Language, ed. Wray, A. (Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 376–397).
Held, R. and Hein, A. (1963). “Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior,” Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 56, pp. 872–876.
Hellige, J. B. (ed.) (2001). Hemispheric Asymmetry: What's right and what's left? (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Henderson, J., Hurly, T. A., Bateson, M., and Healy, S. D. (2006). “Timing in free-living rufous hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus,” Current Biology 16, pp. 512–515.
Henshilwood, C., D'Errico, F., Yates, R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Duller, G., Mercier, N., Sealy, J., Valladas, H., Watts, I., and Wintle, A. (2002). “Emergence of modern human behavior: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa,” Science 295, pp. 1278–1280.
Henton, C. (1992). “The abnormality of male speech,” in New Departures in Linguistics, ed. Wolf, G. (New York, NY: Garland Publishing, pp. 27–59).
Hepper, P. G. (1991). “An examination of fetal learning before and after birth,” Irish Journal of Psychology 12, pp. 95–107.
Herder, J. G. (1996[1772]). Essay on the Origin of Language [Über den Ursprung der Sprache], trans. John H. Moran (Stuttgart: Verlag Freies Geistesleben).
Herman, L. M., Richards, D. G., and Wolz, J. P. (1984). “Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins,” Cognition 16, pp. 129–219.
Hernandez-Aguilar, R. A., Moore, J., and Pickering, T. R. (2007). “Savanna chimpanzees use tools to harvest the underground storage organs of plants,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104, pp. 19210–19213.
Herodotus, (1964 [450–420 bc]). The Histories (Baltimore, MD: Penguin).
Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H., and Cable, C. (1976). “Natural concepts in in the pigeon,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2, pp. 285–311.
Herrnstein, R. J., Vaughan, W.., Mumford, D. B., and Kosslyn, S. M. (1989). “Teaching pigeons an abstract relational rule: Insideness,” Perception & Psychphysics 46, pp. 56–64.
Hewes, G. W. (1973). “Primate communication and the gestural origin of language,” Current Anthropology 14, pp. 5–24.
Hewes, G. W. (1975). Language Origins: A bibliography (The Hague: Mouton).
Hewes, G. W. (1977). “Language origin theories,” in Language Learning by a Chimpanzee: The Lana Project, ed. Rumbaugh, D. M. (New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 5–53).
Hewes, G. W. (1983). “The invention of phonemically-based language,” in Glossogenetics: The origin and evolution of language, ed. Grolier, É. d. (New York, NY: Harwood Academic Publishers, pp. 143–162).
Hewes, G. W. (1996). “A history of the study of language origins and the gestural primacy hypothesis,” in Handbook of Human Symbolic Evolution, ed. Lock, A. and Peters, C. R. (Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 571–595).
Hewitt, G., MacLarnon, A., and Jones, K. E. (2002). “The functions of laryngeal air sacs in primates: A new hypothesis,” Folia Primatologica 73, pp. 70–94.
Hienz, R. D., Jones, A. M., and Weerts, E. M. (2004). “The discrimination of baboon grunt calls and human vowel sounds by baboons,” Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 116, pp. 1692–1697.
Hight, G. and Jury, K. (1970). “Hill country sheep production II: Lamb mortality and birth weights in Romney and Border Leicester × Romney flocks,” New Zealand Journal of Agricultural Research 13, pp. 735–752.
Hiiemäe, K. and Palmer, J. B. (2003). “Tongue movements in feeding and speech,” Critical Reviews in Oral Biology and Medicine 14, pp. 413–429.
Hill, K., Boesch, C., Goodall, J., Pusey, A. E., Williams, J., and Wrangham, R. W. (2001). “Mortality rates among wild chimpanzees,” Journal of Human Evolution 40, pp. 437–450.
Hilloowala, R. A. (1975). “Comparative anatomical study of the hyoid apparatus in selected primates,” American Journal of Anatomy 142, pp. 367–384.
Hinde, R. A. (1981). “Animal signals: Ethological and games-theory approaches are not incompatible,” Animal Behavior 29, pp. 535–542.
Hinton, G. E. and Nowlan, S. J. (1987). “How learning can guide evolution,” Complex Systems 1, pp. 495–502.
Hinton, L., Nichols, J., and Ohala, J. (eds) (1994). Sound Symbolism (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
Hockett, C. F. (1960). “Logical considerations in the study of animal communication,” in Animal Sounds and Communication, ed. Lanyon, W. E. and Tavolga, W. N. (Washington, DC: American Institute of Biological Sciences, pp. 392–430).
Hockett, C. F. (1963). “The problem of universals in language,” in Universals of Language, ed. Greenberg, J. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1–29).
Hockett, C. F. and Ascher, R. (1964). “The human revolution,” Current Anthropology 5, pp. 135–147.
Hoelzel, A. R. (1986). “Song characteristics and response to playback of male and female robins Erithacus rubecula,” Ibis 128, pp. 115–127.
Hofstadter, D. R. (1979). Godel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid (New York, NY: Basic Books).
Holbrook, R. T. and Carmody, F. J. (1937). “X-ray studies of speech articulations,” University of California Publications in Modern Philology 20, pp. 187–238.
Holland, P. W. H. (1999). “The future of evolutionary developmental biology,” Nature 402 suppl., pp. C41–C42.
Holland, P. W. H., Garcia-Fernández, J., Williams, N. A., and Sidow, N. (1994). “Gene duplication and the origins of vertebrate development,” Development, 1994, pp. 125–133.
Holloway, R. L. (1966). “Cranial capacity, neural reorganization and hominid evolution: A search for more suitable parameters,” American Anthropologist 68, pp. 103–121.
Holloway, R. L. (1969). “Culture: A human domain,” Current Anthropology 10, pp. 395–407.
Holloway, R. L. (1996). “Evolution of the human brain,” in Handbook of Human Symbolic Evolution, ed. Lock, A. and Peters, C. R. (Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 74–108).
Holloway, R. L. (2008). “The human brain evolving: A personal retrospective,” Annual Review of Anthropology 37, pp. 1–19.
Holy, T. E. and Guo, Z. (2005). “Ultrasonic songs of male mice,” PLOS Biology 3, p. e386.
Hopcroft, J. E., Motwani, R., and Ullman, J. D. (2000). Introduction to Automata Theory, Languages and Computation (Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley).
Hopfield, J. J. (1982). “Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 79, pp. 2554–2558.
Hopkins, W. D. and Russell, J. L. (2004). “Further evidence of a right hand advantage in motor skill by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes),” Neuropsychologia 42, pp. 990–996.
Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., Freeman, H., Buehler, N., Reynolds, E., and Schapiro, S. J. (2005). “The distribution and development of handedness for manual gestures in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes),” Psychological Science 16, pp. 487–493.
Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P., and Leavens, D. A. (2007). “Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human,” Animal Behavior 73, pp. 281–286.
Hopson, J. A. (1966). “The origin of the mammalian middle ear,” American Zoologist 6, pp. 437–450.
Howie, J. M. (1991). Automata and Languages (Oxford: Oxford Univeristy Press).
Hrdy, S. B. (1981). The Woman Who Never Evolved (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
Hrdy, S. B. (1999). Mother Nature (New York, NY: Pantheon Books).
Hrdy, S. B. (2005). “Comes the child before man: How cooperative breeding and prolonged postweaning dependence shaped human potentials,” in Hunter-Gatherer Childhoods, ed. Hewlett, B. and Lamb, M. (London: Aldine Transaction, pp. 65–91).
Hu, Y., Meng, J., Wang, Y., and Li, C. (2005). “Large Mesozoic mammals fed on young dinosaurs,” Nature 433, pp. 150–152.
Huey, R. B., Hertz, P. E., and Sinervo, B. (2003). “Behavioral drive versus behavioral inertia in evolution: A null model approach,” American Naturalist 161, pp. 357–385.
Huffman, M. A. (1997). “Current evidence for self-medication in primates: A multidisciplinary perspective,” Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 40, pp. 171–200.
Huffman, M. A. and Seifu, M. (1989). “Observations on the illness and consumption of a possibly medicinal plant Vernonia amygdalina by a wild chimpanzee in the Mahale mountains National Park, Tanzania,” Primates 30, pp. 51–63.
Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., and Honig, W. K. (eds) (1978). Cognitive Processes in Animal Behavior (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates).
Humboldt, W. (1836). Über die Kawi-Sprache auf der Insel Java (Berlin: Druckerei der Königlichen Akademie der Wissenschaften).
Humphrey, N. K. (1976). “The social function of intellect,” in Growing Points in Ethology, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. and Hinde, R. A. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 303–317).
Hunt, G. R. and Gray, R. D. (2003). “Diversification and cumulative evolution in New Caledonian crow tool manufacture,” Proceedings of the Royal Society London, B 270, pp. 867–874.
Hunt, G. R. and Gray, R. D. (2004a). “Direct observations of pandanus-tool manufacture and use by a New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides),” Animal Cognition 7, pp. 114–120.
Hunt, G. R. and Gray, R. D. (2004b). “The crafting of hook tools by wild New Caledonian crows,” Proceedings of the Royal Society London, B 271 Suppl. 3, pp. S88–90.
Hurford, J. (1990). “Nativist and functional explanations in language acquisition,” in Logical issues in Language Acquistion, ed. Roca, I. M. (Dordrecht: Foris Publications, pp. 85–136).
Hurford, J. (1994). “Linguistics and evolution: A background briefing for non-linguists,” Discussions in Neuroscience 10, pp. 149–157.
Hurford, J. (2000). “The emergence of syntax,” in The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form, ed. Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Hurford, J. R. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 219–230).
Hurford, J. (2004). “Language beyond our grasp: What mirror neurons can, and cannot, do for language evolution,” in The Evolution of Communication Systems: A comparative approach, ed. Oller, D. K. and Griebel, U. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 297–313).
Hurford, J. (2007). The Origins of Meaning (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Huxley, J. S. (1932). Problems of Relative Growth (London: Methuen).
Huxley, T. H. (1863). Evidence as to Man's Place in Nature (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill).
Huybregts, R. (1985). “The weak inadequacy of CFPSGs,” in Van Periferie naar Kern, ed. Haan, G., Trommelen, M., and Zonneveld, W. (Dordrecht: Foris Publications, pp. 81–99).
Hyde, J. S. and Linn, M. C. (1988). “Gender differences in verbal ability: A meta-analysis,” Psychological Bulletin 104, pp. 53–69.
Iacoboni, M., Molnar-Szakacs, I., Gallese, V., Buccino, G., Mazziotta, J. C., and Rizzolatti, G. (2005). “Grasping the intentions of others with one's own mirror neuron system,” PLOS Biology 3, p. e79.
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., and Rizzolatti, G. (1999). “Cortical mechanisms of human imitation,” Science 286, pp. 2526–2528.
Insley, S. J. (2001). “Mother–offspring vocal recognition in northern fur seals is mutual but asymmetrical,” Animal Behavior 61, pp. 129–137.
,International Chicken Genome Sequencing Consortium, The (2004). “Sequence and comparative analysis of the chicken genome provide unique perspectives on vertebrate evolution,” Nature 432, pp. 695–717.
Isaac, G. L. (1978). “Food sharing and human evolution: Archaeological evidence from the Plio-Pleistocene of East Africa,” Journal of Anthropological Research 34, pp. 311–325.
Isack, H. A. and Reyer, H.-U. (1989). “Honeyguides and honey gatherers: Interspecific communication in a symbiotic relationship,” Science 243, pp. 1343–1346.
Iwatsubo, T., Kuzuhara, S., Kanemitsu, A., Shimada, H., and Toyokura, Y. (1990). “Corticofugal projections to the motor nuclei of the brainstem and spinal cord in humans,” Neurology 40, pp. 309–312.
Jablonski, N. G. (1998). “The response of catarrhine primates to Pleistocene environmental fluctuations in East Asia,” Primates 39, pp. 29–37.
Jackendoff, R. (1999). “Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity,” Trends in Cognitive Science 3, pp. 272–279.
Jackendoff, R. (2002). Foundations of Language (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).
Jackendoff, R. and Lerdahl, F. (1982). “A grammatical parallel between music and language,” in Music, Mind, and Brain: The neuropsychology of music, ed. Clynes, M. E. (New York, NY: Plenum, pp. 83–117).
Jackendoff, R. and Lerdahl, F. (2006). “The capacity for music: What is it, and what's special about it?,” Cognition 100, pp. 33–72.
Jackson, A. P., Eastwood, H., Bell, S. M., Adu, J., Toomes, C., Carr, I. M., Roberts, E., Hampshire, D. J., Crow, Y. J., Mighell, A. J., Karbani, G., Jafri, H., Rashid, Y., Mueller, R. F., Markham, A. F., and Woods, C. G. (2002). “Identification of microcephalin, a protein implicated in determining the size of the human brain,” American Journal of Human Genetics 71, pp. 136–142.
Jacob, F. (1977). “Evolution and tinkering,” Science 196, pp. 1161–1166.
Jacobs, G. H. and Rowe, M. P. (2004). “Evolution of vertebrate colour vision,” Clinical and Experimental Optometry 87, pp. 206–216.
Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie, und allgemeine Lautgesetze (Uppsala: Almqvist and Wiksell).
Jakobson, R. (1960). “Linguistics and poetics,” in Style in Language, ed. Sebeok, T. A. (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 350–377).
Jakobson, R. (1968). Child Language, Aphasia, and Phonological Universals (The Hague: Mouton).
Jakobson, R., Fant, G., and Halle, M. (1957). Preliminaries to Speech Analysis: The distinctive features and their acoustic correlates (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology (New York, NY: Henry Holt).
Janik, V. M. and Slater, P. J. B. (1997). “Vocal learning in mammals,” Advances in the Study of Behavior 26, pp. 59–99.
Janik, V. M. and Slater, P. J. B. (1998). “Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls,” Animal Behavior 56, pp. 829–838.
Janik, V. M. and Slater, P. J. B. (2000). “The different roles of social learning in vocal communication,” Animal Behavior 60, pp. 1–11.
Jenkins, L. (1999). Biolinguistics: Exploring the biology of language (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press).
Jerison, H. J. (1973). Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence (New York: Academic Press).
Jerison, H. J. (1975). “Fossil evidence of the evolution of the human brain,” Annual Review of Anthropology 4, pp. 27–58.
Jespersen, O. (1922). Language: Its nature, development and origin (New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Co.).
Ji, Q., Luo, Z.-X., Yuan, C.-X., Wible, J. R., Zhang, J.-P., and Georgi, J. A. (2002). “The earliest eutherian mammal,” Nature 416, pp. 816–822.
Johanson, D. C. and Edgar, B. (1996). From Lucy to Language (New York, NY: Simon & Schuster Editions).
Johanson, D. C. and White, T. D. (1979). “A systematic assessment of early African hominids,” Science 203, pp. 321–330.
Johnson, M. H. (2005). “Sensitive periods in functional brain development: Problems and prospects,” Developmental Psychobiology 46, pp. 287–292.
Johnstone, R. A. (1995). “Sexual selection, honest advertisement and the handicap principle: Reviewing the evidence,” Biological Reviews 7, pp. 1–65.
Johnstone, R. A. and Grafen, A. (1992). “The continuous Sir Philip Sidney game: A simple model of biological signalling,” Journal of Theoretical Biology 156, pp. 215–234.
Jolly, A. (1966). “Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence,” Science 153, pp. 501–506.
Jonas, S. (1981). “The supplementary motor region and speech emission,” Journal of Communication Disorders 14, pp. 349–373.
Jonas, S. (1982). “The thalamus and aphasia, including transcortical aphasia: A review,” Journal of Communication Disorders 15, pp. 31–41.
Jones, G. and Ransome, R. D. (1993). “Echolocation calls of bats are influenced by maternal effects and change over a lifetime,” Proceedings of the Royal Society B 252B, pp. 125–128.
Jones, S. W. (1798 [1786]). “On the Hindus: The third anniversary discourse,” Asiatick Researches 1, pp. 415–431 (delivered February 2, 1786).
Joshi, A. K. (2002). “Tree-adjoining grammars,” in Handbook of Computational Linguisitcs (New York, NY: Oxford University Press, pp. 1–31).
Joshi, A. K., Vijay-Shanker, K., and Weir, D. J. (1991). “The convergence of Mildly Context-Sensitive formalisms,” in Processing of Linguistic Structure, ed. Sells, P., Shieber, S. M., and Wasow, T. (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press, pp. 31–81).
Joyce, G. F. (2002). “The antiquity of RNA-based evolution,” Nature 382, pp. 525–528.
Jungers, W. J., Pokempner, A. A., Kay, R. F., and Cartmill, M. (2003). “Hypoglossal canal size in living hominoids and the evolution of human speech,” Human Biology 75, pp. 473–484.
Jürgens, U. (1979). “Vocalizations as an emotional indicator: A neuroethological study in the squirrel monkey,” Behaviour 69, pp. 88–117.
Jürgens, U. (1994). “The role of the periaqueductal grey in vocal behaviour,” Behavioural Brain Research 62, pp. 107–117.
Jürgens, U. (1995). “Neuronal control of vocal production in non-human and human primates,” in Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication, ed. Zimmerman, E. and Newman, J. D. (New York, NY: Plenum Press, pp. 199–206).
Jürgens, U. (1998). “Neuronal control of mammalian vocalization, with special reference to the squirrel monkey,” Naturwissenschaften 85, pp. 376–388.
Jürgens, U. (2002). “Neural pathways underlying vocal control,” Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 26, pp. 235–258.
Jürgens, U., Kirzinger, A., and Cramon, D. Y. (1982). “The effects of deep-reaching lesions in the cortical face area on phonation: A combined case report and experimental monkey study,” Cortex 18, pp. 125–139.
Jürgens, U. and Ploog, D. W. (1976). “Zur Evolution der Stimme?,” Archives of Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankungen 222, pp. 117–237.
Jürgens, U. and Pratt, R. (1979). “Cingular vocalization pathway: Squirrel monkey,” Experimental Brain Research 34, pp. 499–510.
Jürgens, U. and Cramon, D. (1982). “On the role of the anterior cingulate cortex in phonation: A case report,” Brain and Language 15, pp. 234–248.
Juslin, P. and Sloboda, J. A. (eds) (2001). Music and Emotion: Theory and research (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Kahane, J. (1982). “Growth of the human prepubertal and pubertal larynx,” Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 25, pp. 446–455.
Kako, E. (1999). “Elements of syntax in the systems of three language-trained animals,” Animal Learning & Behavior 27, pp. 1–14.
Kamil, A. C. and Jones, J. E. (1997). “Clark's nutcrackers learn geometric relationships among landmarks,” Nature 390, pp. 276–279.
Kaminski, J., Call, J., and Fischer, J. (2004). “Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for ‘fast mapping,’Science 304, pp. 1682–1683.
Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2005). “Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object choice task,” Animal Behavior 69, pp. 11–18.
Kandel, E. R. and Schwartz, J. H. (1985). Principles of Neural Science (New York, NY: Elsevier).
Kappelman, J. (1996). “The evolution of body mass and relative brain size in fossil hominids,” Journal of Human Evolution 30, pp. 243–276.
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E. S., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). “Is there a social module? Language, face-processing and theory of mind in individuals with William's Syndrome,” Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 7, pp. 196–208.
Kay, E. D. and Condon, K. (1987). “Skeletal changes in the hindlimbs of bipedal rats,” The Anatomical Record 218, pp. 1–4.
Kay, R. F., Cartmill, M., and Balow, M. (1998). “The hypoglossal canal and the origin of human vocal behavior,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA) 95, pp. 5417–5419.
Keeley, L. H. (1980). Experimental Determination of Stone Tool Uses: A microwear analysis (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press).
Kegl, J. (2002). “Language emergence in a language-ready brain: Acquisition issues,” in Language Acquisition in Signed Languages, ed. Morgan, G. and Woll, B. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 207–254).
Keith, A. (1948). A New Theory of the Evolution of Man (London: Watts).
Kelemen, G. (1963). “Comparative anatomy and performance of the vocal organ in vertebrates,” in Acoustic Behavior of Animals, ed. Busnel, R. (Amsterdam: Elsevier Publishing Company, pp. 489–521).
Kelemen, G. (1969). “Anatomy of the larynx and the anatomical basis of vocal performance,” in The Chimpanzee, ed. Bourne, G. (Basel: S. Karger, pp. 165–187).
Kelemen, G. and Sade, J. (1960). “The vocal organ of the howling monkey (Alouatta palliata),” Journal of Morphology 107, pp. 123–140.
Keller, R. (1995). On Language Change: The invisible hand in language (New York, NY: Routledge).
Kellogg, W. N. and Kellogg, L. A. (1933). The Ape and the Child (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill).
Kemp, T. S. (2005). The Origin and Evolution of Mammals (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Kendon, A. (1991). “Some considerations for a theory of language origins,” Man 26, pp. 199–221.
Kennedy, G. E. (2005). “From the ape's dilemma to the weanling's dilemma: Early weaning and its evolutionary context,” Journal of Human Evolution 48, pp. 123–145.
Kenward, B., Weir, A. A., Rutz, C., and Kacelnik, A. (2005). “Tool manufacture by naive juvenile crows,” Nature 433, p. 121.
Keysers, C., Kohler, E., Umiltà, M. A., Nannetti, L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2003a). “Audiovisual mirror neurons and action recognition,” Experimental Brain Research 153, pp. 628–636.
Keysers, C., Wicker, B., Gazzola, V., Anton, J.-L., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2003b). “A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch,” Neuron 42, pp. 335–346.
Khaitovich, P., Muetzel, B., She, X., Lachmann, M., Hellmann, I., Dietzsch, J., Steigele, S., Do, H.-H., Weiss, G., Enard, W., Heissig, F., Arendt, T., Nieselt-Struwe, K., Eichler, E. E., and Paabo, S. (2004). “Regional patterns of gene expression in human and chimpanzee brains,” Genome Research 14, pp. 1462–1473.
Kier, W. M. and Smith, K. K. (1985). “Tongues, tentacles and trunks: The biomechanics of movement in muscular-hydrostats,” Zoological Journal of the Linneaen Society 83, pp. 307–324.
Kimura, D. (1973). “The assymetry of the human brain,” Scientific American 228, pp. 70–80.
Kimura, D. (1993). Neuromotor Mechanisms in Human Communication (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Kimura, M. (1983). The Neutral Theory of Molecular Evolution (New York: Cambridge University Press).
King, J. L. and Jukes, T. H. (1969). “Non-Darwinian evolution,” Science 164, pp. 788–798.
Kinzey, W. G. (1987). “Monogamous primates: A primate model for human mating systems,” in The Evolution of Human Behavior: Primate models, ed. Kinzey, W. G. (Albany, NY: State University of New York Press, pp. 105–114).
Kirby, S. (1999). Function, Selection and Innateness: The emergence of language universals (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
Kirby, S. (2000). “Syntax without natural selection: How compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners,” in The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form, ed. Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Hurford, J. R. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 303–323).
Kirby, S. (2002). “Natural language from artifical life,” Artificial Life 8, pp. 185–215.
Kirby, S., Cornish, H., and Smith, K. (2008). “Cumulative cultural evolution in the laboratory: An experimental approach to the origins of structure in human language,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105, pp. 10681–10686.
Kirby, S., Dowman, M., and Griffiths, T. L. (2007). “Innateness and culture in the evolution of langauge,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 104, pp. 5241–5245.
Kirby, S., Smith, K., and Brighton, H. (2004). “From UG to universals: Linguistic adaptation through iterated learning,” Studies in Language 28, pp. 587–607.
Kirschner, M. W. and Gerhart, J. C. (2005). The Plausibility of Life: Resolving Darwin's dilemma (London: Yale University Press).