Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
  • Cited by 28
Publisher:
Cambridge University Press
Online publication date:
March 2021
Print publication year:
2021
Online ISBN:
9781108870474

Book description

This Element examines visual perception in the context of activities that involve moving about in complex, dynamic environments. A central theme is that the ability of humans and other animals to perceive their surroundings based on vision is profoundly shaped by the need to adaptively regulate locomotion to variations in the environment. As such, important new insights into what and how we perceive can be gleaned by investigating the connection between vision and the control of locomotion. I present an integrated summary of decades of research on the perception of self-motion and object motion based on optic flow, the perception of spatial layout and affordances, and the control strategies for guiding locomotion based on visual information. I also explore important theoretical issues and debates, including the question of whether visual control relies on internal models.

References

Adolph, K. E. (2008a). The growing body in action: What infant locomotion tells us about perceptually guided action. In Klatzky, R., MacWhinney, B., & Behrmann, M. (eds.), Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action (pp. 275321). Psychology Press.
Adolph, K. E. (2008b). Learning to move. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 213218. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00577.x
Angelaki, D. E., Gu, Y., & Deangelis, G. C. (2011). Visual and vestibular cue integration for heading perception in extrastriate visual cortex. J. Physiol., 589(Pt 4), 825833. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2010.194720
Banks, M. S., Ehrlich, S. M., Backus, B. T., & Crowell, J. A. (1996). Estimating heading during real and simulated eye movements. Vision Research, 36(3), 431443. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(95)00122-0
Barsingerhorn, A. D., Zaal, F. T. J. M., Smith, J., & Pepping, G.-J. (2012). On possibilities for action: the past, present and future of affordance research. Avant, 3(2).
Barton, S. L., Matthis, J. S., & Fajen, B. R. (2017). Visual regulation of gait: zeroing in on a solution to the complex terrain problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(10), 17731790. doi:10.1037/xhp0000435
Bastin, J., Craig, C., & Montagne, G. (2006). Prospective strategies underlie the control of interceptive actions. Hum. Mov. Sci., 25(6), 718732. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2006.04.001
Bastin, J., Fajen, B. R., & Montagne, G. (2010). Controlling speed and direction during interception: an affordance-based approach. Exp. Brain Res., 201(4), 763780. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2092-y
Battaglia, P. W., Hamrick, J. B., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2013). Simulation as an engine of physical scene understanding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A, 110(45), 1832718332. doi:10.1073/pnas.1306572110
Bootsma, R. J. (2009). The (current) future is here! Perception, 38(6), 851.
Bootsma, R. J., Ledouit, S., Casanova, R., & Zaal, F. T. J. M. (2016). Fractional-order information in the visual control of lateral locomotor interception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(4), 517529. doi:10.1037/xhp0000162
Britten, K. H. (2008). Mechanisms of self-motion perception. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 31, 389410. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112953
Britten, K. H., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (1998). Electrical microstimulation of cortical area MST biases heading perception in monkeys. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 5963.
Chapman, S. (1968). Catching a baseball. American Journal of Physics, 36(10), 868870. doi:10.1119/1.1974297
Chardenon, A., Montagne, G., Buekers, M. J., & Laurent, M. (2002). The visual control of ball interception during human locomotion. Neuroscience Letters, 334(1), 1316.
Cheng, J. C. K., & Li, L. (2012). Effects of reference objects and extra-retinal information about pursuit eye movements on curvilinear path perception from retinal flow. Journal of Vision, 12(3):12, 1–21. doi:10.1167/12.3.12
Craik, K. J. W. (1943). The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Creem-Regehr, S. H., Stefanucci, J. K., & Thompson, W. B. (2015). Perceiving absolute scale in virtual environments: How theory and application have mutually informed the role of body-based perception. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 62 (pp. 195224). Elsevier.
Cullen, K. E. (2019). Vestibular processing during natural self-motion: implications for perception and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(6), 346363. doi:10.1038/s41583-019-0153-1
Cuturi, L. F., & MacNeilage, P. R. (2013). Systematic biases in human heading estimation. PLoS ONE, 8(2), e56862.
Dachner, G. C., & Warren, W. H. (2014). Behavioral dynamics of heading alignment in pedestrian following. Transportation Research Procedia, 2, 6976. doi:10.1016/j.trpro.2014.09.010
Diaz, G. J., Phillips, F., & Fajen, B. R. (2009). Intercepting moving targets: a little foresight helps a lot. Experimental Brain Research, 195(3), 345360. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-1794-5
Dixon, M. W., Wraga, M., Proffitt, D. R., & Williams, G. C. (2000). Eye height scaling of absolute size in immersive and nonimmersive displays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(2), 582.
Dokka, K., DeAngelis, G. C., & Angelaki, D. E. (2015). Multisensory integration of visual and vestibular signals improves heading discrimination in the presence of a moving object. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(40), 1359913607. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2267-15.2015
Dokka, K., MacNeilage, P. R., DeAngelis, G. C., & Angelaki, D. E. (2013). Multisensory self-motion compensation during object trajectory judgments. Cerebral Cortex.
Dokka, K., Park, H., Jansen, M., DeAngelis, G. C., & Angelaki, D. E. (2019). Causal inference accounts for heading perception in the presence of object motion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A, 116(18), 90609065. doi:10.1073/pnas.1820373116
Duffy, C. J., & Wurtz, R. H. (1991). Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. Journal of Neurophysiology, 65(6), 13291345.
Durant, S., & Zanker, J. M. (2020). The combined effect of eye movements improve head centred local motion information during walking. PLoS One, 15(1), e0228345. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0228345
Elder, D. M., Grossberg, S., & Mingolla, E. (2009). A neural model of visually guided steering, obstacle avoidance, and route selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 15011531.
Escobar-Alvarez, H. D., Johnson, N., Hebble, T., Klingebiel, K., Quintero, S. A. P., Regenstein, J., & Browning, N. A. (2017). R-ADVANCE: Rapid adaptive prediction for vision-based autonomous navigation, control, and evasion. Journal of Field Robotics, 35(1), 91100. doi:10.1002/rob.21744
Fajen, B. R. (2005a). Calibration, information, and control strategies for braking to avoid a collision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(3), 480501. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.480
Fajen, B. R. (2005b). Perceiving possibilities for action: On the necessity of calibration and perceptual learning for the visual guidance of action. Perception, 34, 717740. doi:10.1068/p5405
Fajen, B. R. (2005c). The scaling of information to action in visually guided braking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(5), 11071123. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.1107
Fajen, B. R. (2007). Affordance-based control of visually guided action. Ecological Psychology, 19(4), 383410.
Fajen, B. R. (2013). Guiding locomotion in complex, dynamic environments. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(Article 85), 115. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00085/abstract
Fajen, B. R., Diaz, G., & Cramer, C. (2011). Reconsidering the role of movement in perceiving action-scaled affordances. Human Movement Science, 30(3), 504533. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2010.07.016
Fajen, B. R., & Matthis, J. S. (2011). Direct perception of action-scaled affordances: The shrinking gap problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1442.
Fajen, B. R., & Matthis, J. S. (2013). Visual and non-visual contributions to the perception of object motion during self-motion. PLoS One, 8(2), e55446. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055446
Fajen, B. R., Parade, M. S., & Matthis, J. S. (2013). Humans perceive object motion in world coordinates during obstacle avoidance. J Vis, 13(8). doi:10.1167/13.8.25
Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2008). Information, affordances, and the control of action in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40, 79107.
Fajen, B. R., & Turvey, M. T. (2003). Perception, categories, and possibilities for action. Adaptive Behavior, 11(4), 276278.
Fajen, B. R., & Warren, W. H. (2003). Behavioral dynamics of steering, obstable avoidance, and route selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(2), 343362.
Fajen, B. R., & Warren, W. H. (2004). Visual guidance of intercepting a moving target on foot. Perception, 33(6), 689715. doi:10.1068/p5236
Fajen, B. R., & Warren, W. H. (2007). Behavioral dynamics of intercepting a moving target. Experimental Brain Research, 180(2), 303319. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-0859-6
Fath, A. J., & Fajen, B. R. (2011). Static and dynamic visual information about the size and passability of an aperture. Perception, 40(8), 887904. doi:10.1068/p6917
Fink, P. W., Foo, P. S., & Warren, W. H. (2009). Catching fly balls in virtual reality: A critical test of the outfielder problem. Journal of Vision, 9(13), 1414. doi:10.1167/9.13.14
Floreano, D., Ijspeert, A. J., & Schaal, S. (2014). Robotics and neuroscience. Curr Biol, 24(18), R910R920. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.07.058
Floreano, D., Zufferey, J.-C., Klaptocz, A., Germann, J., & Kovac, M. (2017). Aerial locomotion in cluttered environments. In Robotics Research (Vol. 100 2, pp. 2139). Cham: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-29363-9_2
Fukusima, S. S., Loomis, J. M., & Da Silva, J. A. (1997). Visual perception of egocentric distance as assessed by triangulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23(1), 86.
Ghose, K., Horiuchi, T. K., Krishnaprasad, P. S., & Moss, C. F. (2006). Echolocating Bats Use a Nearly Time-Optimal Strategy to Intercept Prey. PLoS Biology, 4(5), e108.
Gibson, J. J. (1950). The Perception of the Visual World. Cambridge: The Riverside Press.
Gibson, J. J. (1958). Visually controlled locomotion and visual orientation in animals. British Journal of Psychology, 49(3), 182194. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1958.tb00656.x
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception Текст. doi:10.4324/9781315740218-10
Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. Hilldale, USA, 1(2).
Gibson, J. J., Olum, P., & Rosenblatt, F. (1955). Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. The American Journal of Psychology, 68(3), 372385.
Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems.
Godthelp, H. (1986). Vehicle control during curve driving. Human Factors, 28(2), 211221.
Godthelp, J. (1985). Precognitive control: Open-and closed-loop steering in a lane-change manoeuvre. Ergonomics, 28(10), 14191438.
Graziano, M. S., Andersen, R. A., & Snowden, R. J. (1994). Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions. Journal of Neuroscience, 14(1), 5467.
Greenlee, M. W., Frank, S. M., Kaliuzhna, M., Blanke, O., Bremmer, F., Churan, J., … Smith, A. T. (2016). Multisensory integration in self motion perception. Multisensory Research, 29(67), 525556.
Gu, Y., Angelaki, D. E., & Deangelis, G. C. (2008). Neural correlates of multisensory cue integration in macaque MSTd. Nat Neurosci, 11(10), 12011210. doi:10.1038/nn.2191
Gu, Y., DeAngelis, G. C., & Angelaki, D. E. (2007). A functional link between area MSTd and heading perception based on vestibular signals. Nat Neurosci, 10(8), 10381047. doi:10.1038/nn1935
Gu, Y., Deangelis, G. C., & Angelaki, D. E. (2012). Causal links between dorsal medial superior temporal area neurons and multisensory heading perception. J Neurosci, 32(7), 22992313. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5154-11.2012
Hayhoe, M. M. (2017). Vision and action. Annual Review of Vision Science, 3, 389413.
Hildreth, E. C., Beusmans, J. M. H., Boer, E. R., & Royden, C. S. (2000). From vision to action: Experiments and models of steering control during driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1106.
Hsu, J. (2019). Machines on mission possible. Nature Machine Intelligence, 1(3), 124127. doi:10.1038/s42256-019-0034-3
Israël, I., & Warren, W. H. (2005). Vestibular, proprioceptive, and visual influences on the perception of orientation and self-motion in humans. Head direction cells and the neural mechanisms of spatial orientation, 347381.
Jirsa, V. K., & Kelso, S. (2004). Coordination dynamics: Issues and trends. Springer Science & Business Media.
Kaiser, M. K., & Mowafy, L. (1993). Optical specification of time-to-passage: Observers’ sensitivity to global tau. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(5), 1028.
Kelly, J. W., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (2005). The importance of perceived relative motion in the control of posture. Exp Brain Res, 161(3), 285292. doi:10.1007/s00221-004-2069-9
Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic Patterns: The Self-Organization of Brain and Behavior. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.
Kim, N.-G., & Turvey, M. T. (1999). Eye movements and a rule for perceiving direction of heading. Ecological Psychology, 11(3), 233248.
Konczak, J., Meeuwsen, H. J., & Cress, M. E. (1992). Changing affordances in stair climbing: The perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(3), 691.
Kuo, A. D., & Donelan, J. M. (2010). Dynamic principles of gait and their clinical implications. Physical Therapy, 90(2), 157174.
Lappe, M., Bremmer, F., & Van den Berg, A. V. (1999). Perception of self-motion from visual flow. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 329336.
Lappi, O., & Mole, C. (2018). Visuomotor control, eye movements, and steering: A unified approach for incorporating feedback, feedforward, and internal models. Psychological Bulletin, 144(10), 9811001. doi:10.1037/bul0000150
Latash, M. L. (2008). Synergy. Oxford University Press.
Layton, O. W., & Fajen, B. R. (2016a). A neural model of MST and MT explains perceived object motion during self-motion. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(31), 80938102. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4593-15.2016
Layton, O. W., & Fajen, B. R. (2016b). Competitive dynamics in MSTd: A mechanism for robust heading perception based on optic flow. PLoS Computational Biology, 12(6).
Layton, O. W., & Fajen, B. R. (2016c). Sources of bias in the perception of heading in the presence of moving objects: Object-based and border-based discrepancies. J Vis, 16(1), 9. doi:10.1167/16.1.9
Layton, O. W., & Fajen, B. R. (2016d). The temporal dynamics of heading perception in the presence of moving objects. J Neurophysiol, 115(1), 286300. doi:10.1152/jn.00866.2015
Layton, O. W., & Fajen, B. R. (2017). Possible role for recurrent interactions between expansion and contraction cells in MSTd during self-motion perception in dynamic environments. J Vis, 17(5), 5. doi:10.1167/17.5.5
Layton, O. W., Mingolla, E., & Browning, N. A. (2012). A motion pooling model of visually guided navigation explains human behavior in the presence of independently moving objects. Journal of Vision, 12(1), 2020. doi:10.1167/12.1.20
Lee, D. N. (1980). Visuo-motor coordination in space-time. In Stelmach, G. E. & Requin, J. (Eds.), Tutorials in Motor Behavior (pp. 281295). Amsterdam: North-Holland. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.1977.tb00281.x
Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception, 5(4), 437459. doi:10.1068/p050437
Lee, D. N. (1974). Visual information during locomotion. In MacLeod, H. & Pick, H. (Eds.) Perception: Essays in Honor of J. J. Gibson, Ithaca: Cornell University Press.
Lee, D. N., Lishman, J. R., & Thomson, J. A. (1982). Regulation of gait in long jumping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8(3), 448459.
Lenoir, M., Musch, E., Janssens, M., Thiery, E., & Uyttenhove, J. (1999). Intercepting moving objects during self-motion. Journal of Motor Behavior, 31(1), 5567.
Li, L., Chen, J., & Peng, X. (2009). Influence of visual path information on human heading perception during rotation. Journal of Vision, 9(3):29, 1–14. doi:10.1167/9.3.29
Li, L., & Cheng, J. C. (2011). Perceiving path from optic flow. Journal of Vision, 11(1). doi:10.1167/11.1.22
Li, L., Ni, L., Lappe, M., Niehorster, D. C., & Sun, Q. (2018). No special treatment of independent object motion for heading perception. Journal of Vision, 18(4), 19–16. doi:10.1167/18.4.19
Li, L., Stone, L. S., & Chen, J. (2011). Influence of optic-flow information beyond the velocity field on the active control of heading. Journal of Vision, 11(4), 99. doi:10.1167/11.4.9
Li, L., Sweet, B. T., & Stone, L. S. (2006). Humans can perceive heading without visual path information. Journal of Vision, 6(9), 22. doi:10.1167/6.9.2
Li, L., & Warren, W. H. (2000). Perception of heading during rotation: Sufficiency of dense motion parallax and reference objects. Vision Research, 40(28), 38733894.
Longuet-Higgins, H. C., & Prazdny, K. (1980). The interpretation of a moving retinal image. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences, 208(1173), 385397.
Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (1998). Visually controlled locomotion: Its dependence on optic flow, three-dimensional space perception, and cognition. Ecological Psychology, 10(3–4), 271285.
Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (2004). Model-based control of perception/action. In Optic Flow and Beyond (pp. 421441). Dordrecht: Springer.
Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Fujita, N., & Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 906.
Macuga, K. L., Beall, A. C., Smith, R. S., & Loomis, J. M. (2019). Visual control of steering in curve driving. Journal of Vision, 19(5), 11. doi:10.1167/19.5.1
Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 361370.
Mark, L. S., Balliett, J. A., Craver, K. D., Douglas, S. D., & Fox, T. (1990). What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. Ecological Psychology, 2(4), 325366.
Matthis, J. S., Muller, K. S., Bonnen, K., & Hayhoe, M. M. (2020). Retinal optic flow during natural locomotion. doi:10.1101/2020.07.23.217893
McNamee, D., & Wolpert, D. M. (2019). Internal models in biological control. Annual Review of Control, Robotics, and Autonomous Systems, 2, 339364.
Michaels, C. F., & Oudejans, R. R. D. (1992). The optics and actions of catching fly balls: Zeroing out optical acceleration. Ecological Psychology, 4(4), 199222. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco0404_1
Michaels, C. F., Prindle, S., & Turvey, M. T. (1985). A note on the natural basis of action categories: The catching distance of mantids. Journal of Motor Behavior, 17(2), 255264. doi:10.1080/00222895.1985.10735348
Mischiati, M., Lin, H. T., Herold, P., Imler, E., Olberg, R., & Leonardo, A. (2015). Internal models direct dragonfly interception steering. Nature, 517(7534), 333338. doi:10.1038/nature14045
Mohler, B. J., Thompson, W. B., Creem-Regehr, S. H., Willemsen, P., Pick, J., Herbert, L., & Rieser, J. J. (2007). Calibration of locomotion resulting from visual motion in a treadmill-based virtual environment. ACM Transactions on Applied Perception (TAP), 4(1), 4–es.
Mole, C. D., Lappi, O., Giles, O., Markkula, G., Mars, F., & Wilkie, R. M. (2019). Getting Back Into the Loop: The Perceptual-Motor Determinants of Successful Transitions out of Automated Driving. Hum Factors, 18720819829594. doi:10.1177/0018720819829594
Morice, A. H. P., François, M., Jacobs, D. M., & Montagne, G. (2010). Environmental constraints modify the way an interceptive action is controlled. Experimental Brain Research, 202(2), 397411. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2147-0
Olberg, R. M., Worthington, A. H., Fox, J. L., Bessette, C. E., & Loosemore, M. P. (2005). Prey size selection and distance estimation in foraging adult dragonflies. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol, 191(9), 791797. doi:10.1007/s00359-005-0002-8
Olberg, R. M., Worthington, A. H., & Venator, K. R. (2000). Prey pursuit and interception in dragonflies. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 186(2), 155162. doi:10.1007/s003590050015
Oudejans, R. R., Michaels, C. F., van Dort, B., & Frissen, E. J. P. (1996). To cross or not to cross: The effect of locomotion on street-crossing behavior. Ecological Psychology, 8(3), 259267. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco0803_4
Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Dolné, M. A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22(4), 879891.
Perrone, J. A. (2018). Visual-vestibular estimation of the body’s curvilinear motion through the world: A computational model. J Vis, 18(4), 1. doi:10.1167/18.4.1
Perrone, J. A. (1992). Model for the computation of self-motion in biological systems. JOSA A, 9(2), 177194.
Philbeck, J. W., Loomis, J. M., & Beall, A. C. (1997). Visually perceived location is an invariant in the control of action. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(4), 601612.
Plumert, J. M., & Kearney, J. K. (2014). How do children perceive and act on dynamic affordances in crossing traffic-filled roads. Child Dev Perspect, 8(4), 207212. doi:10.1111/cdep.12089
Postma, D. B. W. (2019). Affordance-based control in running to catch fly balls. Ph.D. thesis, University of Groningen.
Postma, D. B. W., Lemmink, K. A. P. M., & Zaal, F. T. J. M. (2018). The affordance of catchability in running to intercept fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(9), 13361347. doi:10.1037/xhp0000531
Postma, D. B. W., Smith, J., Pepping, G.-J., van Andel, S., & Zaal, F. T. J. M. (2017). When a fly ball is out of reach: Catchability judgments are not based on optical acceleration cancelation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(868), 14271428. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00535
Raudies, F., & Neumann, H. (2013). Modeling heading and path perception from optic flow in the case of independently moving objects. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 23. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00023
Reed, E. S. (1988). James J. Gibson and the Psychology of Perception. Yale University Press.
Regan, D., & Beverley, K. I. (1982). How do we avoid confounding the direction we are looking and the direction we are moving. Science, 215(4529), 194196.
Riddell, H., Li, L., & Lappe, M. (2019). Heading perception from optic flow in the presence of biological motion. J Vis, 19(14), 25. doi:10.1167/19.14.25
Rieger, J. H. (1983). Information in optical flows induced by curved paths of observation. JOSA, 73(3), 339344. doi:10.1364/JOSA.73.000339
Rieger, J. H., & Lawton, D. T. (1985). Processing differential image motion. JOSA A, 2, 354359.
Rieser, J. J., Ashmead, D. H., Talor, C. R., & Youngquist, G. A. (1990). Visual perception and the guidance of locomotion without vision to previously seen targets. Perception, 19(5), 675689. doi:10.1068/p190675
Rieser, J. J., Pick, H. L., Ashmead, D. H., & Garing, A. E. (1995). Calibration of human locomotion and models of perceptual-motor organization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 480.
Rio, K. W., Rhea, C. K., & Warren, W. H. (2014). Follow the leader: Visual control of speed in pedestrian following. J Vis, 14(2). doi:10.1167/14.2.4
Royden, C. S. (1994). Analysis of misperceived observer motion during simulated eye rotations. Vision Research, 34(23), 32153222. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)90085-X
Royden, C. S., Banks, M. S., & Crowell, J. A. (1992). The perception of heading during eye movements. Nature, 360(6404), 583585.
Royden, C. S., Crowell, J. A., & Banks, M. S. (1994). Estimating heading during eye movements. Vision Research, 34(23), 31973214. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)90084-1
Royden, C. S., & Hildreth, E. C. (1996). Human heading judgments in the presence of moving objects. Perception & Psychophysics, 58(6), 836856.
Rushton, S. K., Chen, R., & Li, L. (2018). Ability to identify scene-relative object movement is not limited by, or yoked to, ability to perceive heading. Journal of Vision, 18(6), 1111. doi:10.1167/18.6.11
Rushton, S. K., Harris, J. M., Lloyd, M. R., & Wann, J. P. (1998). Guidance of locomotion on foot uses perceived target location rather than optic flow. Current Biology, 8(21), 11911194. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(07)00492-7
Saito, H.- a., Yukie, M. , Tanaka, K. , Hikosaka, K. , Fukada, Y. , & Iwai, E. (1986). Integration of direction signals of image motion in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 6(1), 145157.
Saunders, J. A. (2010). View rotation is used to perceive path curvature from optic flow. Journal of Vision, 10(13), 2525. doi:10.1167/10.7.806
Saunders, J. A., & Ma, K.-Y. (2011). Can observers judge future circular path relative to a target from retinal flow. Journal of Vision, 11(7), 1616. doi:10.1167/11.7.16
Senders, J. W., Kristofferson, A. B., Levison, W. H., Dietrich, C. W., & Ward, J. L. (1967). The attentional demand of automobile driving. Highway Research Record, 195, 15-33.
Srinivasan, M. V. (2011). Visual control of navigation in insects and its relevance for robotics. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 21(4), 535543. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.020
Steinmetz, S. T., Layton, O. W., Powell, N. V., & Fajen, B. R. (2020). Affordance-based versus current-future accounts of choosing whether to pursue or abandon the chase of a moving target. J Vis, 20(3), 8. doi:10.1167/jov.20.3.8
Stoffregen, T. A., Yang, C.-M., Giveans, M. R., Flanagan, M., & Bardy, B. G. (2009). Movement in the perception of an affordance for wheelchair locomotion. Ecological Psychology, 21(1), 136.
Stone, L. S., & Perrone, J. A. (1997). Human heading estimation during visually simulated curvilinear motion. Vision Research, 37(5), 573590. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00204-0
Strogatz, S. (2001). Nonlinear dynamics and chaos: with applications to physics, biology, chemistry, and engineering (studies in nonlinearity).
Sunkara, A., DeAngelis, G. C., & Angelaki, D. E. (2016). Joint representation of translational and rotational components of optic flow in parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(18), 50775082. doi:10.1073/pnas.1604818113
Tanaka, K., & Saito, H.-A. (1989). Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 62(3), 626641.
Thomson, J. A. (1983). Is continuous visual monitoring necessary in visually guided locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9(3), 427.
Turvey, M. T. (1990). Coordination. American Psychologist, 45(8), 938.
Turvey, M. T. (2007). Action and perception at the level of synergies. Human Movement Science, 26(4), 657697.
Turvey, M. T., & Carello, C. (1996). Dynamics of Bernstein’s level of synergies. Dexterity and its development, 339376.
van Andel, S., Cole, M. H., & Pepping, G.-J. (2017). A systematic review on perceptual-motor calibration to changes in action capabilities. Human Movement Science, 51, 5971. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2016.11.004
Van den Berg, A. V., & Brenner, E. (1994a). Humans combine the optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of heading. Vision Research, 34(16), 21532167. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)90324-7
Van den Berg, A. V., & Brenner, E. (1994b). Why two eyes are better than one for judgements of heading. Nature, 371(6499), 700702. doi:10.1038/371700a0
Wallis, G., Chatziastros, A., & Bülthoff, H. (2002a). An unexpected role for visual feedback in vehicle steering control. Current Biology, 12(4), 295299. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00685-1
Wallis, G., Chatziastros, A., & Bülthoff, H. (2002b). An unexpected role for visual feedback in vehicle steering control. Current Biology, 12(4), 295299.
Wann, J., & Land, M. (2000). Steering with or without the flow: Is the retrieval of heading necessary? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(8), 319324. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01513-8
Wann, J. P., & Swapp, D. K. (2000). Why you should look where you are going. Nature Neuroscience, 3 (7), 647.
Warren, P., & Rushton, S. (2007). Perception of object trajectory: Parsing retinal motion into self and object. Journal of Vision.
Warren, P., & Rushton, S. (2009a). Perception of scene-relative object movement: Optic flow parsing and the …. Vision Research.
Warren, P. A., & Rushton, S. K. (2009b). Optic flow processing for the assessment of object movement during ego movement. Current Biology, 19(18), 15551560. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.057
Warren, P. A., Rushton, S. K., & Foulkes, A. J. (2012). Does optic flow parsing depend on prior estimation of heading? Journal of Vision, 12(11), 8.
Warren, W. H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 683703.
Warren, W. H. (1988). Action modes and laws of control for the visual guidance of action. In Complex Movement Behavior: The Motor-Action Controversy (pp. 339380). Elsevier.
Warren, W. H. (1995). Self-motion: Visual perception and visual control. In Perception of Space and Motion (pp. 263325). Elsevier.
Warren, W. H. (1998a). The state of flow. High-Level Motion Processing: Computational, Neurobiological and Psychophysical Perspectives, 315358.
Warren, W. H. (1998b). Visually controlled locomotion: 40 years later. Ecological Psychology, 10 (34), 177219.
Warren, W. H. (2004). Optic flow. In Chalupa, L. & Werner, J. (eds.), The Visual Neurosciences (pp. 12471259). Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.
Warren, W. H. (2006). The dynamics of perception and action. Psychological Review, 113(2), 358389.
Warren, W. H. (2007). Action-scaled information for the visual control of locomotion. Closing the gap: The scientific writings of David N. Lee, 243258.
Warren, W. H. (2018). Collective motion in human crowds. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 096372141774674096372141774679. doi:10.1177/0963721417746743
Warren, W. H., Blackwell, A. W., Kurtz, K. J., Hatsopoulos, N. G., & Kalish, M. L. (1991). On the sufficiency of the velocity field for perception of heading. Biological Cybernetics, 65(5), 311320.
Warren, W. H., & Fajen, B. R. (2004). From optic flow to laws of control. In Optic Flow and Beyond (pp. 307337). Springer.
Warren, W. H., & Fajen, B. R. (2008). Behavioral dynamics of visually guided locomotion. In Coordination: Neural, Behavioral and Social Dynamics (pp. 4575). Springer.
Warren, W. H., & Hannon, D. J. (1988). Direction of self-motion is perceived from optical flow. Nature, 336(6195), 162163. doi:10.1038/336162a0
Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., Zosh, W. D., Duchon, A. P., & Sahuc, S. (2001). Optic flow is used to control human walking. Nature Neuroscience, 4(2), 213. doi:10.1038/84054
Warren, W. H., Mestre, D. R., Blackwell, A. W., & Morris, M. W. (1991). Perception of circular heading from optical flow. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17(1), 28.
Warren, W. H., Morris, M. W., & Kalish, M. (1988). Perception of translational heading from optical flow. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(4), 646.
Warren, W. H., & Saunders, J. A. (1995). Perceiving heading in the presence of moving objects. Perception, 24(3), 315331. doi:10.1068/p240315
Warren, W. H., & Whang, S. (1987). Visual guidance of walking through apertures: Body-scaled information for affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13(3), 371383.
Warren, W. H., Young, D. S., & Lee, D. N. (1986). Visual control of step length during running over irregular terrain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 12(3), 259266. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.12.3.259
Wilkie, R. M., & Wann, J. P. (2006). Judgments of path, not heading, guide locomotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfo-rmance, 32(1), 88.
Wraga, M. (1999a). The role of eye height in perceiving affordances and object dimensions. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(3), 490507.
Wraga, M. (1999b). Using eye height in different postures to scale the heights of objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perform-ance, 25(2), 518.
Yilmaz, E. H., & Warren, W. H. (1995). Visual control of braking: A test of the !ṫ hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21(5), 996.
Zhao, H., Straub, D., & Rothkopf, C. A. (2019). The visual control of interceptive steering: How do people steer a car to intercept a moving target. J Vis, 19(14), 11. doi:10.1167/19.14.11
Zhao, H., & Warren, W. H. (2015). Online and model-based approaches to the visual control of action. Vision Research, 110(PB), 190202. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2014.10.008
Zhao, H., & Warren, W. H. (2017). Intercepting a moving target in fog: Online or model-based control? 17, 113. doi:10.1167/17.5.12.doi

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Book summary page views

Total views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between #date#. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed.

Accessibility standard: Unknown

Accessibility compliance for the PDF of this book is currently unknown and may be updated in the future.