Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-29T12:25:49.037Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exploratory movement and affordances in design

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2015

Thomas A. Stoffregen*
Affiliation:
School of Kinesiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota, USA
Bruno Mantel
Affiliation:
Normandie Université, France, and UniCaen, CesamS, F-14032 Caen, France
*
Reprint requests to: Thomas A. Stoffregen, School of Kinesiology, University of Minnesota, 224A Cooke Hall, 1900 University Avenue SE, Minneapolis, MN 55113, USA. E-mail: tas@umn.edu

Abstract

The design community has growing familiarity with the concept of affordances and with the utility of this concept in many areas of design. Less emphasis has been placed on natural processes by which people acquire knowledge about affordances. Consequently, little is known about how design might be optimized to enable users to detect the actions that are available in a given human–machine system. We review scientific research about what people do to obtain information about affordances. We discuss implications of this research for design.

Type
Special Issue Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adolph, K.E. (1995). Psychophysical assessment of toddlers’ ability to cope with slopes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21(4), 734750.Google ScholarPubMed
Adolph, K.E., Eppler, M., Marin, L., Weiss, I., & Clearfield, M.W. (2000). Exploration in the service of prospective control. Infant Behavior and Development 23(3–4), 441460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, K.B., & Flach, J.M. (2011). Display and Interface Design: Subtle Science, Exact Art. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bingham, G.P., Schmidt, R.C., & Rosenblum, L.D. (1989). Hefting for a maximum distance throw: a smart perceptual mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15(3), 507528.Google ScholarPubMed
Burns, C.M., & Hajdukiewicz, J.R. (2004). Ecological Interface Design. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Carello, C., Thuot, S., Anderson, K.L., & Turvey, M.T. (1999). Perceiving the sweet spot. Perception 28(3), 307320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, C.-H., Wade, M.G., & Stoffregen, T.A. (2009). Perceiving affordances for aperture passage in an environment-person-person system. Journal of Motor Behavior 41, 495500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fajen, B.R. (2005). The scaling of information to action in visually guided braking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 31(5), 11071123.Google ScholarPubMed
Fajen, B.R. (2007). Affordance-based control of visually guided action. Ecological Psychology 19(4), 383410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J.J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton–Mifflin.Google Scholar
Hove, P., Riley, M.A., & Shockley, K. (2006). Perceiving affordances of hockey sticks by dynamic touch. Ecological Psychology 18(3), 163189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E.L., Hollan, J.D., & Norman, D.A. (1985). Direct manipulation interfaces. Human–Computer Interaction 1, 311338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D., & Wade, M.G. (2009). Children at risk for developmental coordination disorder: judgment of changes in action capabilities. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 51(5), 397403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, D.N., Young, D.S., & McLaughlin, M. (1984). A roadside simulation of road crossing for children. Ergonomics 27(12), 12711281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maier, J.R.A., & Fadel, G.M. (2009 a). Affordance based design: a relational theory for design. Research in Engineering Design 20(1), 1327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maier, J.R.A., & Fadel, G.M. (2009 b). Affordance-based design methods for innovative design, redesign and reverse engineering. Research in Engineering Design 20(4), 225239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mantel, B., Bardy, B.G., & Stoffregen, T.A. (2008). Intermodal perception of whether an object is within reach. Proc. Multimodal Perception–Action Dynamics, Symp. 10th European Workshop of Ecological Psychology, Funchal, Madeira, Portugal, November.Google Scholar
Mantel, B., Bardy, B.G., & Stoffregen, T.A. (2010). Locomotor assessment of whether an object is reachable. Ecological Psychology 22, 192211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mantel, B., Hoppenot, P., & Colle, E. (2012). Perceiving for acting with teleoperated robots: ecological principles to human–robot interaction design. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics, Part A: Systems and Humans 42(6), 14601475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mark, L.M. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13(3), 361370.Google ScholarPubMed
Mark, L.M. (2007). Perceiving the actions of other people. Ecological Psychology 19(2), 107136.Google Scholar
Mark, L.S., Balliet, J.A., Craver, K.D., Douglas, S.D., & Fox, T. (1990). What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. Ecological Psychology 2(4), 325366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrenere, J., & Ho, W. (2000). Affordances: clarifying and evolving a concept. Proc. Graphic Interface, pp. 179186, Montreal.Google Scholar
Michaels, C.F., Weier, Z., & Harrison, S.J. (2007). Using vision and dynamic touch to perceive the affordances of tools. Perception 36(5), 750772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, D. A. (1988). The Psychology of Everyday Things. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Plumert, J.M., Kearney, J.K., & Cremer, J.F. (2004). Children's perception of gap affordances: bicycling across traffic-filled intersections in an immersive virtual environment. Child Development 75(4), 12431253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramenzoni, V., Riley, M. A., Davis, T., & Shockley, K. (2008). Tuning in to another person's action capabilities: perceiving maximal jumping-reach height from walking kinematics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 34(4), 919928.Google ScholarPubMed
Richardson, M.J., Marsh, K.L., & Barron, R.M. (2007). Judging and actualizing intrapersonal and interpersonal affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 33(4), 845859.Google ScholarPubMed
Riley, M.A., Wagman, J.B., Santana, M.-V., Carello, C., & Turvey, M.T. (2002). Perceptual behavior: recurrence analysis of a haptic exploratory procedure. Perception 31(4), 481510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person-and-action perception: expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 112(4), 585615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, R.E., Flasher, O.M., & Kadar, E.E. (1995). Dimensionless invariants for intentional systems: measuring the fit of vehicular activities to environmental layout. In Global Perspective on the Ecology of Human–Machine Systems (Flach, J.M., Hancock, P.A., Carid, J., & Vicente, K. J., Eds.), pp. 293357. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schneiderman, B. (1983). Direct manipulation: a step beyond programming languages. Computer 16(8), 5769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, H.Y., & Turvey, M.T. (1988). Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14(3), 404427.Google ScholarPubMed
Stoffregen, T.A. (2003). Affordances as properties of the animal-environment system. Ecological Psychology 15(2), 115134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoffregen, T.A., Bardy, B.G., & Mantel, B. (2006). Affordances in the design of enactive systems. Virtual Reality 10(1), 410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoffregen, T.A., Chen, Y.-C., & Koslucher, F.C. (2014). Motion control, motion sickness, and the postural dynamics of mobile devices. Experimental Brain Research 232(4), 13891397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoffregen, T.A., Gorday, K.M., Sheng, Y.-Y., & Flynn, S.B. (1999). Perceiving affordances for another person's actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 25(1), 120136.Google ScholarPubMed
Stoffregen, T.A., Yang, C.-M., Giveans, R., Flanagan, M., & Bardy, B.G. (2009). Movement in the perception of an affordance for wheelchair locomotion. Ecological Psychology 21(1), 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torenvliet, G. (2003). We can't afford it! Interactions 10(4), 1317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turvey, M.T. (1992). Affordances and prospective control: an outline of the ontology. Ecological Psychology 4(3), 173187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicente, K.J. (2002). Ecological interface design: progress and challenges. Human Factors 44(1), 6278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vicente, K.J., & Rasmussen, J. (1990). The ecology of human-machine systems: II. Mediating “direct perception” in complex work domains. Ecological Psychology 2(3), 207250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagman, J., & Carello, C. (2003). Haptically creating affordances: the user-tool interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 9(3), 175186.Google ScholarPubMed
Warren, W.H. (1984). Perceiving affordances: visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10(5), 683703.Google ScholarPubMed
Yasuda, M., Wagman, J.B., & Higuchi, T. (2014). Can perception of aperture passability be improved immediately after practice in actual passage? Dissociation between walking and wheelchair use. Experimental Brain Research 232(3), 753764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ye, L., Cardwell, W., & Mark, L.M. (2009). Perceiving multiple affordances for objects. Ecological Psychology 21(3), 195217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yonas, A., & Hartman, B. (1993). Perceiving the affordance of contact in four- and five-month-old infants. Child Development 64(1), 298308.Google ScholarPubMed
Yu, Y., Bardy, B.G., & Stoffregen, T.A. (2011). Influences of head and torso movement before and during affordance perception. Journal of Motor Behavior 43(1), 4554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, Y., & Stoffregen, T.A. (2012). Postural and locomotor contributions to affordance perception. Journal of Motor Behavior 44(5), 305311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed