Hostname: page-component-76dd75c94c-7vt9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T07:41:46.804Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Communicative initiations in normal and late-talking toddlers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Rhea Paul*
Affiliation:
Portland State University
Mary E. Shiffer
Affiliation:
North Clackamas School District
*
Rhea Paul, Department of Speech, P.O.B. 751, Portland State University, Portland, OR 97207-0751

Abstract

Initiation of communication in videotaped, unstructured mother–child interactions was examined in two groups of 2-year-olds: those with normal language development and those with late acquisition of expressive language. Results revealed that the late-talkers (LTs) expressed significantly fewer intentions, but that the difference between the two groups could be accounted for entirely by the difference in one type of intention: the expression of joint attentional intentions. Investigation of the forms of expression of intentions showed that the normal group used significantly more verbal forms of expression, as expected. The predominant form for the normal group was word combinations, while the predominant form for the LTs was vocalization. The implications of these results for understanding the mechanisms involved in early language delay are discussed.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1991

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aram, D., & Nation, J. (1980). Preschoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 232244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. (1976). Language in context. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (1969). Scales of infant development. New York: Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bzock, K., & League, R. (1971). Assessing language skills in infancy. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. (1981). Exploring children's communicative intents. In Miller, J. (Ed.), Assessing language production in children (pp. 111138). Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Coggins, T., & Carpenter, R. (1981). The communicative intention inventory. Journal of Applied Psycholinguistics, 2, 213234.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S., Bates, E., Reznick, J. S., & Morisset, C. (1989). The validity of a parent report instrument at twenty months. Journal of Child Language, 16, 239350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenburg, W., & Dodds, K. (1967). The Denver developmental screening test. Journal of Pediatrics, 71, 181191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, K., & Tomblin, J. (1978). A follow-up study of children with articulation and language disorders. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 43, 227241.Google ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. (1965). Four factor index of social status. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
McReynolds, R., & Kearns, M. (1983). Single subject experimental designs in communication disorders. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for. Research in Child Development 38 (Serial No. 149).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, R. (1989, June). Outcomes of expressive language delay in toddlers: Age three. Paper presented at the Symposium for Research in Child Language Development, Madison, WI.Google Scholar
Paul, R., & Cohen, D. J. (1984). Outcomes of severe disorders of language acquisition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 405422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, R., & Jennings, P. (1990, November). Phonological behavior in late-talking toddlers. Paper presented at the National Convention of the American Speech-Language and Hearing Association, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Paul, R., Spangle-Looney, S., & Dahm, P. (1991). Communication and socialization skills at ages two and three in “late-talking” young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 34, 858865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. (1989). The Language Development Survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. (1990, June). Outcomes of specific expressive language delay in toddlers. Paper presented at the Symposium for Research in Child Language Development, Madison, WI.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynell, J. (1984). Developmental language scales. London: NFER Nelson.Google Scholar
Reznick, J. S., & Goldsmith, L. A. (1989). A multiple form word production checklist for assessing early language. Journal of Child Language, 76, 91100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, L. (1978). Communicative and cognitive abilities and disabilities in the sensorimotor period. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 24, 161180.Google Scholar
Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Ciccetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Thai, D. (1989, April). Language and gesture in late-talkers. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, MO.Google Scholar
Wetherby, A., Cain, D., Yonclas, D., & Walker, V. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 240252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wetherby, A., & Pruning, C. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive-social abilities in autistic children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 27, 364377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wetherby, A., Yonclas, D., & Bryan, A. (1989). Communicative profiles of preschool children with handicaps: Implications for early identification. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 148158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed