Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T02:23:48.129Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Common and distinct cognitive bases for reading in English–Cantonese bilinguals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2008

GIGI LUK
Affiliation:
York University
ELLEN BIALYSTOK*
Affiliation:
York University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Ellen Bialystok, Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada. E-mail: ellenb@yorku.ca

Abstract

The study explores the relationship between phonological awareness and early reading for bilingual children learning to read in two languages that use different writing systems. Participants were 57 Cantonese–English bilingual 6-year-olds who were learning to read in both languages. The children completed cognitive measures, phonological awareness tasks, and word identification tests in both languages. Once cognitive abilities had been controlled, there was no correlation in word identification ability performance across languages, but the correspondence in phonological awareness measures remained strong. This pattern was confirmed by a principal components analysis and hierarchical regression that demonstrated a different role for each phonological awareness factor in reading performance in each language. The results indicate that phonological awareness depends on a set of cognitive abilities that is applied generally across languages and that early reading depends on a common set of cognitive abilities in conjunction with skills specific to different writing systems.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anthony, J. L., & Lonigan, C. J. (2004). The nature of phonological awareness: Converging evidence from four studies of preschool and early grade school children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 4355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Luk, G., & Kwan, E. (2005). Bilingualism, biliteracy, and learning to read: Interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 4361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., McBride-Chang, C., & Luk, G. (2005). Bilingualism, language proficiency, and learning to read in two writing systems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 580590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolger, D. J., Perfetti, C. A., & Schneider, W. (2005). Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 92104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branum-Martin, L., Mehta, P. D., Fletcher, J. M., Carlson, C. D., Ortiz, A., Carlo, M., et al. (2006). Bilingual phonological awareness: Multilevel construct validation among Spanish-speaking kindergarteners in transitional bilingual education classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 170181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children's reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 3142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, 77111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, C. K. K., & Siegel, L. S. (2001). Phonological processing in reading Chinese among normally achieving and poor readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 2343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, H.-C., & Yip, M. C. W. (2001). Processing syllabic and sub-syllabic information in Cantonese. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 2, 199210.Google Scholar
Cormier, P., & Dea, S. (1997). Distinctive patterns of relationship of phonological awareness and working memory with reading development. Reading and Writing, 9, 193206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulmas, F. (1989). The writing systems of the world. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech: The diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Durgunoğlu, A. Y. (2002). Cross-linguistic transfer in literacy development and implications for language learners. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 189204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durgunoğlu, A. Y., Nagy, W. E., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2001). Does strength of phonological representations predict phonological awareness in preschool children? Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 301325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geva, E., & Siegel, L. S. (2000). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gholamain, M., & Geva, E. (1999). Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in English and Persian. Language Learning, 49, 183217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gomez, C., & Reason, R. (2002). Cross-linguistic transfer of phonological skills: A Malaysian perspective. Dyslexia, 8, 2233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U. C., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gottardo, A., Siegel, L. S., Yan, B., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2001). Factors related to English reading performance in children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-language transfer of phonological processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 530542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanley, J. R., Tzeng, O., & Huang, H. S. (1999). Learning to read Chinese. In Harris, M. & Hatano, G. (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 173195). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hatcher, L. (1994). A step-by-step approach to using the SAS® system for factor analysis and structural equation modeling. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.Google Scholar
Ho, C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. (1997a). Learning to read Chinese beyond the logographic phase. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 276289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. (1997b). Phonological skills are important in learning to read Chinese. Developmental Psychology, 33, 946951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, C. F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 5579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, H. S., & Hanley, J. R. (1995). Phonological awareness and visual skills in learning to read Chinese and English. Cognition, 54, 7398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulme, C., Caravolas, M., Málkova, G., & Briqstocke, S. (2005). Phoneme isolation ability is not simply a consequence of letter-sound knowledge. Cognition, 97, B1B11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Snowling, M., Caravolas, M., & Carroll, J. (2005). Phonological skills are (probably) one cause of success in learning to read: A comment on Castles and Coltheart. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 351365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., & Pfeiffer, S. L. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leong, C. K., Cheng, P. W., & Tan, L.-H. (2005). The role of sensitivity to rhymes, phonemes and tones in reading English and Chinese pseudowords. Reading and Writing, 18, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manis, F. R., Lindsey, K. A., & Bailey, C. E. (2004). Development of reading in grades K–2 in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 19, 214224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C. (1995). What is phonological awareness? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 179192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Bialystok, B., Chong, K. K. Y., & Li, Y. (2004). Levels of phonological awareness in three cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 89, 93111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S. (2000). Developmental issues in Chinese children's character acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 5055.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., & Kail, R. V. (2002). Cross-cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Development, 73, 13921407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muter, V., & Snowling, M. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 320337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 443468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakhill, J., & Kyle, F. (2000). The relation between phonological awareness and working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75, 152164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Passenger, T., Stuart, M., & Terrell, C. (2000). Phonological processing and early literacy. Journal of Research in Reading, 23, 5566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (2003). The universal grammar of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1998). Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Rohl, M., & Pratt, C. (1995). Phonological awareness, verbal working memory and the acquisition of literacy. Reading and Writing, 7, 327360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, R., & Stuart, M. (2001). Orthographic analogies and early reading: Exploration of performance and variation in two transfer tasks. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 571598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schatschneider, C., Francis, C. J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., & Mehta, P. (1999). The dimensionality of phonological awareness: An application of item response theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 439449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siok, W. T., & Fletcher, P. (2001). The role of phonological awareness and visual–orthographic skills in Chinese reading acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 37, 886–699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature, 431, 7176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L., Sáez, L., Gerber, M., & Leafstedt, J. (2004). Literacy and cognitive functioning in bilingual and nonbilingual children at or not at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tan, L. H., Laird, A. R., Li, K., & Fox, P. T. (2005). Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 8391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wade-Woolley, L., & Geva, E. (2000). Processing novel phonemic contrasts in the acquisition of L2 word reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 4, 295311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & McBride-Chang, C. (1996). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities. Annals of Child Development, 12, 177206.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmins, K., & Rashotte, C. (1993). Development of young readers' phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 83103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Cheng, C. X., & Chen, S. W. (2006). Contribution of morphological awareness to Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 542553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Park, Y., & Lee, K. R. (2006). Korean–English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language phonological and orthographic transfer. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 148158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language and writing system transfer. Cognition, 97, 6788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wayland, R. P., & Guion, S. G. (2004). Training English and Chinese listeners to perceive Thai tones: A preliminary report. Language Learning, 54, 681712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, R. W., & Johnson, M. B. (1987). Woodcock–Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery. Riverside, CA: DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar