Skip to main content
×
×
Home

Different phonological mechanisms facilitate vocabulary learning at early and late stages of language acquisition: Evidence from Polish 9-year-olds learning English

  • MARTA MARECKA (a1), JAKUB SZEWCZYK (a2), ANNA JELEC (a3), DONATA JANISZEWSKA (a3), KAROLINA RATAJ (a3) and KATARZYNA DZIUBALSKA-KOŁACZYK (a3)...
  • Please note a correction has been issued for this article.
Abstract

To acquire a new word, learners need to create its representation in phonological short-term memory (STM) and then encode it in their long-term memory. Two strategies can enable word representation in STM: universal segmentation and phonological mapping. Universal segmentation is language universal and thus should predict word learning in any language, while phonological mapping is language specific. This study investigates the mechanisms of vocabulary learning through a comparison of vocabulary learning task results in multiple languages. We tested 44 Polish third graders learning English on phonological STM, phonological awareness in Polish and in English, and on three tasks, which involved learning novel word forms in Polish (first language), in English (second language), and in a language that did not resemble any language known to participants (an unknown language). Participants’ English proficiency was controlled through a vocabulary task. The results suggest that word learning engages different mechanisms for familiar and unfamiliar languages. Phonological awareness in English predicted learning second language and unknown language words, and phonological STM predicted learning words of the unknown language. We propose that universal segmentation facilitates word learning only in an unfamiliar language, while in familiar languages speakers use phonological mapping in order to learn new words.

Copyright
Corresponding author
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Marta Marecka, Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, Ingardena 6, 30-060 Kraków, Poland. E-mail: marta.t.marecka@gmail.com
References
Hide All
Anderson, S. R. (1985). Phonology in the twentieth century. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Gathercole, S. E., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158173.
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1975). Working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 4790). New York: Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586595.
Bogdanowicz, M. (2009). Nieznany Język. Dysleksja 3. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych.
Bogdanowicz, M., Jaworowska, A., Krasowicz-Kupis, G., Matczak, A., Pelc-Pekala, O., Pietras, I., . . . Szczerbinski, M. (2009). Dysleksja 3. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych.
Bowey, J. A. (1996). On the association between phonological memory and receptive vocabulary in five-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 4478.
Bowey, J. A. (2001). Nonword repetition and young children's receptive vocabulary: A longitudinal study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 441469.
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1995). On the nature of the phonological output planning processes involved in verbal rehearsal: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 48, 191220.
Carroll, S. E. (2004). Segmentation: Learning how to “hear words” in the L2 speech stream. Transactions of the Philological Society, 102, 227254.
Čeponienė, R., Service, E., Kurjenluoma, S., Cheour, M., & Näätänen, R. (1999). Children's performance on nonword repetition depends on auditory trace quality: Evidence from event-related potentials. Developmental Psychology, 35, 709720.
Cheung, H. (1996). Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary language. Developmental Psychology, 32, 867873.
Cutler, A. (1990). Exploiting prosodic probabilities in speech segmentation. In Altmann, G. T. M. (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives (pp. 105121). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cutler, A. (2008). The abstract representations in speech processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 16011619.
Dahan, D., & Magnuson, J. S. (2006). Spoken word recognition. In Traxler, M. J. & Gernsbacher, M. A. (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 249284). London: Elsevier/Academic Press.
Davies, M. (2010). The corpus of contemporary American English as the first reliable monitor corpus of English. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 25, 447464.
Davis, M. H., & Gaskell, M. G. (2009). A complementary systems account of word learning: Neural and behavioural evidence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364, 37733800.
de Jong, P. F., Seveke, M.-J. E. J., & van Veen, M. (2000). Phonological sensitivity and the acquisition of new words in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 275301.
Dziubalska-Kołaczyk, K., Zydorowicz, P., & Jankowski, M. (2013). English morphonotactics: A corpus study. Phonetician, 107–108, 5367.
Eiter, B. M., & Inhoff, A. W. (2010). Visual word recognition during reading is followed by subvocal articulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 457470.
Ellis, N. C., & Hennelly, R. A. (1980). A bilingual word-length effect: Implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 4351.
Elman, J. L., & McClelland, J. L. (1984). The trace model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 183.
Endress, A. D., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). Word segmentation with universal prosodic cues. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 177199.
Farnia, F., & Geva, E. (2011). Cognitive correlates of vocabulary growth in English language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 711738.
French, L. M. (2006). Phonological working memory and second language acquisition: A developmental study of Francophone children learning English in Quebec. Lewiston, NY: Edwin Mellen Press.
Gathercole, S. E. (1995). Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 23, 8394.
Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513543.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The role of phonological memory in vocabulary acquisition: A study of young children learning new names. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 439454.
Gathercole, S. E., Hitch, G. J., Service, E., & Martin, A. J. (1997). Phonological short-term memory and new word learning in children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 966979.
Gathercole, S. E., & Masoura, E. V. (2003). Contrasting contributions of phonological short-term memory and long-term knowledge to vocabulary learning in a foreign language. Memory, 13, 422429.
Gathercole, S. E., Tiffany, C., Briscoe, J., Thorn, A., & ALSPAC Team. (2005). Developmental consequences of poor phonological short-term memory function in childhood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 598611.
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., & Emslie, H. (1994). The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition: A test of phonological working memory. Memory, 2, 103127.
Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28, 887898.
Gorman, B. K. (2012). Relationships between vocabulary size, working memory, and phonological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners. American Journal of Speech-Language, 21, 109123.
Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., & Scott, S. K. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99, 1091110916.
Goswami, U., Wang, H. L. S., Cruz, A., Fosker, T., Mead, N., & Huss, M. (2011). Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 325337.
Gow, D. W. Jr., & Gordon, P. C. (1995). Lexical and prelexical influences on word segmentation: Evidence from priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 344359.
Hu, C.-F. (2003). Phonological memory, phonological awareness, and foreign language word learning. Language Learning, 53, 429462.
Hu, C.-F. (2008). Rate of acquiring and processing L2 color words in relation to L1 phonological awareness. Modern Language Journal, 92, 3952.
Hu, C.-F., & Schuele, C. M. (2005). Learning nonnative names: The effect of poor native phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 343362.
Jones, G. (2011). A computational simulation of children's performance across three nonword repetition tests. Cognitive Systems Research, 12, 113121.
Jones, G. (2012). Why chunking should be considered as an explanation for developmental change before short-term memory capacity and processing speed. Cognitive Science, 3, 167.
Jones, G., Gobet, F., Freudenthal, D., Watson, S. E., & Pine, J. M. (2013). Why computational models are better than verbal theories: The case of nonword repetition. Developmental Science, 17, 298310.
Jones, G., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. M. (2007). Linking working memory and long-term memory: A computational model of the learning of new words. Developmental Science, 10, 853873.
Jones, G., & Witherstone, H. L. (2011). Lexical and sublexical knowledge influences the encoding, storage, and articulation of nonwords. Memory & Cognition, 39, 588599.
Larsen, J. D., & Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Disruption of verbal STM by irrelevant speech, articulatory suppression, and manual tapping: Do they have a common source? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 56, 12491268.
Lee, S., & Gorman, B. K. (2013). Nonword repetition performance and related factors in children representing four linguistic groups. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 479495.
Lumley, T., & Miller, A. (2004). Leaps: Regression subset selection (R Package version). Vienna, Austria: R Foundation.
Martin, N. A., & Brownell, R. (2010). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (4th ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Masoura, E. V., & Gathercole, S. E. (1999). Phonological short-term memory and foreign language learning. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 383388.
Matczak, A., Piotrowska, A., & Ciarkowska, W. (2008). WISC-R—Skala Inteligencji Wechslera dla Dzieci—Wersja Zmodyfikowana. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych.
Mcanally, K. I., & Stein, J. F. (1996). Auditory temporal coding in dyslexia. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 263, 961965.
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lervåg, A., Lyster, S.-A. H., Klem, M., Hagtvet, B., & Hulme, C. (2012). Nonword-repetition ability does not appear to be a causal influence on children's vocabulary development. Psychological Science, 23, 10921098.
Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children's phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 319.
Mirman, D., Magnuson, J. S., Estes, K. G., & Dixon, J. A. (2008). The link between statistical segmentation and word learning in adults. Cognition, 108, 271280.
Mitton, R. (1992). A description of a computer-usable dictionary file based on the Oxford Advanced Learner's Dictionary of Current English. Retrieved from ftp://svr-ftp.eng.cam.ac.uk/pub/comp.speech/info/CUVOALD_dictionary
Norris, D., & Cutler, A. (1985). Juncture detection. Linguistics, 23, 689705.
Norris, D., & McQueen, J. M. (2008). Shortlist B: A Bayesian model of continuous speech recognition. Psychological Review, 115, 357395.
Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1995). Verbal short-term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 48, 98107.
Port, R. (2007). How are words stored in memory? Beyond phones and phonemes. New Ideas in Psychology, 25, 143170.
Port, R. F. (2008). The graphical basis of phones and phonemes. In Bohn, O.-S. & Munro, M. J. (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning (pp. 349365). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Przepiórkowski, A., Bańko, M., Górski, R. L., & Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2012). Norodowy korpus języka polskiego. Warsaw: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.
Ramachandra, V., Hewitt, L. E., & Brackenbury, T. (2011). The relationship between phonological memory, phonological sensitivity, and incidental word learning. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40, 93109.
Raven, J. C., Szustrowa, T., & Jaworowska, A. (2003). Test Matryc Ravena w Wersji Kolorowej (TMK). Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych.
Read, C., Yun-Fei, Z., Hong-Yin, N., & Bao-Qing, D. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing. Cognition, 24, 3144.
Repovs, G., & Baddeley, A. D. (2006). The multi-component model of working memory: Explorations in experimental cognitive psychology. Neuroscience, 139, 521.
Richardson, U., Thomson, J. M., Scott, S. K., & Goswami, U. (2004). Auditory processing skills and phonological representation in dyslexic children. Dyslexia, 10, 215233.
Selkirk, E. O. E., Aronoff, M., & Oehrle, R. T. (1984). On the major class features and syllable theory. In Aronoff, M. & Oehrle, R. T. (Eds.), Language sound structure (pp. 107136). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Service, E. (1992). Phonology, working memory, and foreign-language learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 45, 2150.
Service, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (1993). Differences between young and older adults in learning a foreign vocabulary. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 608623.
Service, E., & Kohonen, V. (1995). Is the relation between phonological memory and foreign language learning accounted for by vocabulary acquisition? Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 155172.
Service, E., Maury, S., & Luotoniemi, E. (2007). Individual differences in phonological learning and verbal STM span. Memory & Cognition, 35, 11221135.
Shebani, M. F., Van De Vijver, F. J., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2005). A strict test of the phonological loop hypothesis with Libyan data. Memory & Cognition, 33, 196202.
Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 369373.
Sobkowiak, W. (2006). Phonetics of EFL dictionary definitions. Poznań, Poland: Wydawnictwo Poznańskie.
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 13211337.
Storkel, H. L. (2003). Learning new words: II. Phonotactic probability in verb learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 13121323.
Storkel, H. L., Armbrüster, J., & Hogan, T. P. (2006). Differentiating phonotactic probability and neighborhood density in adult word learning. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 11751192.
Storkel, H. L., & Rogers, M. A. (2000). The effect of probabilistic phonotactics on lexical acquisition. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 14, 407425.
Szczerbiński, M., & Pelc-Pękała, O. (2009). Usuwanie Fonemów dla klasy III. Dysleksja 3. Warsaw: Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych.
Szewczyk, J., Marecka, M., Chiat, S., & Wodniecka, Z. (2017). Nonword repetition depends on the frequency of sublexical representations at different grain sizes: Evidence from a multi-factorial analysis. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Talcott, J. B., Witton, C., Hebb, G. S., Stoodley, C. J., Westwood, E. A., France, S. J., . . . Stein, J. F. (2002). On the relationship between dynamic visual and auditory processing and literacy skills; results from a large primary-school study. Dyslexia, 8, 204225.
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., & Pearson, N. A. (2013). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Windfuhr, K. L., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The relationship between paired associate learning and phonological skills in normally developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 160173.
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 329.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Applied Psycholinguistics
  • ISSN: 0142-7164
  • EISSN: 1469-1817
  • URL: /core/journals/applied-psycholinguistics
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 17
Total number of PDF views: 172 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 538 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between 30th October 2017 - 16th August 2018. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

A correction has been issued for this article: