Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Japanese EFL learners’ sentence processing of conceptual plurality: An analysis focusing on reciprocal verbs

  • YU TAMURA (a1), JUNYA FUKUTA (a2), YOSHITO NISHIMURA (a3), YUI HARADA (a4), KAZUHISA HARA (a5) and DAIKI KATO (a6)...

Abstract

This study aimed to investigate how Japanese learners of English as a foreign language, whose first language does not have obligatory morphological number marking, process conceptual plurality. The targeted structure was reciprocal verbs, which require conceptual plurality to interpret their meanings correctly. The results of a sentence completion task confirmed that participants could use reciprocal verbs reciprocally in English. In a self-paced reading experiment, participants read sentences with reciprocal verbs and those with optionally transitive verbs (e.g., while the king and the queen kissed/left the baby read the book in the bed). There was no reading time delay for reciprocal verbs but a delay for optionally transitive verbs. Therefore, the participants succeeded in processing second language conceptual plurality in the online sentence comprehension task.

Copyright

Corresponding author

ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Yu Tamura, Faculty of Foreign Language Studies, Kansai University, 3-3-35 Yamate-cho, Suita-shi, Osaka, 564-8680, Japan. E-mail: yutamura@kansai-u.ac.jp

References

Hide All
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 (R package version 1.1-12). Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/package=lme4
Bock, K., & Eberhard, K. M. (1993). Meaning, sound and syntax in English number agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 5799. doi:10.1080/01690969308406949.
Bock, K., Eberhard, K. M., Cutting, J. C., Meyer, A. S., & Schriefers, H. (2001). Some attractions of verb agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 83128. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0753
Bock, K., & Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken agreement. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 4593. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(91)90003-7
Bock, K., Nicol, J. L., & Cutting, J. C. (1999). The ties that bind: Creating number agreement in speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 330346. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2616
Eberhard, K. M. (1997). The marked effect of number on subject–verb agreement. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 147164.
Eberhard, K. M. (1999). The accessibility of conceptual number to the processes of subject–verb agreement in English. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 560578. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2662
Eberhard, K. M., Cutting, J. C., & Bock, K. (2005). Making syntax of sense: Number agreement in sentence production. Psychological Review, 112, 531559. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.3.531
Ferreira, F., & McClure, K. (1997). Parsing of garden-path sentences with reciprocal verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 273306. doi: 10.1080/016909697386862
Fiengo, R., & Lasnik, H. (1973). The logical structure of reciprocal sentences in English. Foundations of Language, 9, 447468.
Foote, R. (2010). Age of acquisition and proficiency as factors in language production: Agreement in bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 99118. doi: 10.1017/S136672890999040X
Gleitman, L. R., Gleitman, H., Miller, C., & Ostrin, R. (1996). Similar, and similar concepts. Cognition, 58, 321376. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00686-9
Hartsuiker, R. J., & Barkhuysen, P. N. (2006). Language production and working memory: The case of subject-verb agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 181204. doi:10.1080/01690960400002117
Hoshino, N., Dussias, P. E., & Kroll, J. F. (2010). Processing subject–verb agreement in a second language depends on proficiency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 8798. doi:10.1017/S1366728909990034
Hoshino, N., Kroll, J. F., & Dussias, P. E. (2012). Psycholinguistic perspectives on second language speech production. In M. Sanz & J. M. Igoa (Eds.), Applying language science to language pedagogy: Contributions of linguistics and psycholinguistics to second language teaching (pp. 107127). Newcastle, UK: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
Humphreys, K. R., & Bock, K. (2005). Notional number agreement in English. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 689695. doi:10.3758/BF03196759
Jackson, C. N., Mormer, E., & Brehm, L. (2018). The production of subject-verb agreement among Swedish and Chinese second language speakers of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1017/S0272263118000025
Jiang, N. (2007). Selective integration of linguistic knowledge in adult second language learning. Language Learning, 57, 133. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2007.00397.x
Juffs, A. (1998). Main verb versus reduced relative clause ambiguity resolution in L2 sentence processing. Language Learning, 48, 107147. doi:10.1111/1467-9922.00034
Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (1996). Garden path sentences and error data in second language sentence processing. Language Learning, 46, 283323. doi:10.1111/j.1467-1770.1996.tb01237.x
Kaup, B., Kelter, S., & Habel, C. (2002). Representing referents of plural expressions and resolving plural anaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 405450. doi:10.1080/01690960143000272
Kreiner, H., Garrod, S., & Sturt, P. (2013). Number agreement in sentence comprehension: The relationship between grammatical and conceptual factors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28, 829874. doi:10.1080/01690965.2012.667567
Kusanagi, K., Tamura, Y., & Fukuta, J. (2015). The notional number attraction in English as a foreign language: A self-paced reading study. Journal of the Japan Society for Speech Sciences, 16, 7796.
Lenth, R. (2016). lsmeans (R package version 2.25) Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/package=lsmeans
Linck, J. A., & Cunnings, I. (2015). The utility and application of mixed-effects models in second language research. Language Learning, 65, 185207. doi:10.1111/lang.12117
Lo, S., & Andrews, S. (2015). To transform or not to transform: Using generalized linear mixed models to analyse reaction time data. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 116. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01171
Nakanishi, K., & Tomioka, S. (2004). Japanese plurals are exceptional. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 13, 113140. doi:10.1023/B:JEAL.0000019058.46668.c1
Nicol, J. L., Forster, K. I., & Veres, C. (1997). Subject–verb agreement processes in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 569587. doi:10.1006/jmla.1996.2497
Nicol, J. L., & Greth, D. (2003). Production of subject-verb agreement in Spanish as a second language. Experimental Psychology, 50, 196203. doi:10.1026//1617-3169.50.3.196
Nicol, J. L., Teller, M., & Greth, D. (2001). Production of verb agreement in monolingual, bilingual, and second-language speakers. In J. L. Nicol (Ed.), One mind, two languages: Bilingual sentence processing (pp. 117133). Maiden: Blackwell.
Patson, N. D. (2014). The processing of plural expressions. Linguistics and Language Compass, 8, 319329. doi:10.1111/lnc3.12085
Patson, N. D., & Ferreira, F. (2009). Conceptual plural information is used to guide early parsing decisions: Evidence from garden-path sentences with reciprocal verbs. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 464486. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.02.003
Patson, N. D., George, G., & Warren, T. (2014). The conceptual representation of number. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 13491365. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.863372
Patson, N. D., & Warren, T. (2010). Evidence for distributivity effects in comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 782789. doi:10.1037/a0018783
Patson, N. D., & Warren, T. (2011). Building complex reference objects from dual sets. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 443459. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2011.01.005
Pearlmutter, N. J., Garnsey, S. M., & Bock, K. (1999). Agreement processes in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 427456. doi:10.1006/jmla.1999.2653
R Core Team (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/
Shibuya, M., & Wakabayashi, S. (2008). Why are L2 learners not always sensitive to subject-verb agreement? EUROSLA Yearbook, 8, 235258. doi:10.1075/eurosla.8.13shi
Wei, X., Chen, B., Liang, L., & Dunlap, S. (2015). Native language influence on the distributive effect in producing second language subject–verb agreement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 23702383. doi:10.1080/17470218.2015.1014821

Keywords

Type Description Title
WORD
Supplementary materials

Tamura et al. supplementary material
Tamura et al. supplementary material 1

 Word (95 KB)
95 KB

Japanese EFL learners’ sentence processing of conceptual plurality: An analysis focusing on reciprocal verbs

  • YU TAMURA (a1), JUNYA FUKUTA (a2), YOSHITO NISHIMURA (a3), YUI HARADA (a4), KAZUHISA HARA (a5) and DAIKI KATO (a6)...

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed