Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T07:53:54.789Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Metalinguistic ability and early reading achievement

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Judith A. Bowey*
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Rinu K. Patel
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
*
Dr. Judith A. Bowey, Department of Psychology, University of Queensland, St. Lucia 4067, Australia

Abstract

This study explored the conceptual status of metalinguistic ability by determining whether or not metalinguistic ability can account for variation in early reading achievement independently of more general language abilities. First-grade children were given a test battery assessing phonemic awareness, syntactic awareness, receptive vocabulary, syntactic proficiency, word decoding ability, and reading comprehension ability. Strong zero-order correlations were observed among all experimental measures. However, multiple regression analyses revealed that metalinguistic ability did not contribute to the prediction of early reading achievement when general language ability effects were statistically controlled.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bialystok, E., & Ryan, E. B. (1985). Toward a definition of metalinguistic skill. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 31, 229251.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1986a). Syntactic awareness and verbal performance from preschool to fifth grade. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 285308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1986b). Syntactic awareness in relation to reading skill and ongoing reading comprehension monitoring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 282299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1988). Metalinguistic functioning in children. Geelong, Australia: Deakin University Press.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A. (1980). Unpublished data.Google Scholar
Bradley, L. (1980). Assessing reading difficulties: A diagnostic and remedial approach. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 746747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, L., (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlational analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ehri, L. C., & Wilce, L. S. (1980). The influence of orthography on readers' conceptualization of the phonemic structure of words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 371385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hakes, D. T. (in collaboration with Evans, J. S. & Tunmer, W. E.). (1980). The development of metalinguistic abilities in children. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Gleitman, L. R., & Gleitman, H. (1978). What did the brain say to the mind? A study of the detection and report of ambiguity by young children. In Sinclair, A., Jarvella, R. J. & Levelt, W. J. M. (Eds.), The child's conception of language (pp. 97132). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I., Liberman, A., Mattingly, I., & Shankweiler, D. (1980). Orthography and the beginning reader. In Kavanagh, J. F. & Venezky, R. L. (Eds.), Orthography, reading, and dyslexia (pp. 137153). Baltimore, MD: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Lundberg, I., Olofsson, A., & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maclean, M., Bryant, P., & Bradley, L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255281.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A. (1986). Phonological awareness: The role of reading experience. Cognition, 10, 6592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menyuk, P. (1963). A preliminary evaluation of grammatical capacity in children. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 429439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menyuk, P. (1969). Sentences children use. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Morais, J., Alegria, J., & Content, A. (1987). The relationships between segmental analysis and alphabetic literacy: An interactive view. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 7, in press.Google Scholar
Morais, J., Gary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morais, J., Cluytens, M., & Alegria, J. (1984). Segmentation abilities of dyslexies and normal readers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, 221222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newcomer, P. L., & Hammill, D. D. (1982). Test of Language Development-Primary. Austin, TX: Services for Professional Educators.Google Scholar
Read, C., Zhang, Y.-N., Nie, H.-Y., & Ding, B.-Q. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing. Cognition, 24, 3144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, S. (1978). Self-initiated corrections in the speech of infant-school children. Journal of Child Language, 5, 365371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, E. B., & Ledger, G. W. (1979). Grammatically judgments, sentence repetitions, and sentence corrections of children learning to read. International Journal of Psycholinguistics, 6, 2340.Google Scholar
Smith, C. L., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (1982). Metalinguistic awareness and language development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 449468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Feeman, D. J. (1984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 278303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Bowey, J. A. (1984). Metalinguistic awareness and reading acquisition. In Tunmer, W. E., Pratt, C., & Herriman, M. L. (Eds.), Language awareness in children: Theory, research and implications. (pp. 144168). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Herriman, A. Y. (1984). The development of metalinguistic awareness: A conceptual overview. In Tunmer, W. E., Pratt, C., & Herriman, M. L. (Eds.), Language awareness in children: Theory, research and implications (pp. 1235). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Velleman, P. F., & Velleman, A. Y. (1985). The data desk handbook. Ithaca, NY: Data Description.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321363.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 92212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weaver, P. A. (1979). Improving reading comprehension: Effects of sentence organization instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 15, 130146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. P. (1980). Teaching decoding with an emphasis on phoneme analysis and phoneme blending. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willows, D. M., & Ryan, E. B. (1986). The development of grammatical sensitivity and its relationship to early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 253266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, R. W. (1973). Woodcock Reading Mastery tests. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar