Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-c47g7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T11:31:52.583Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Spoken word recognition in children with and without specific language impairment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Chris Dollaghan*
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
*
Department of Communication Science and Disorders, 4033 Forbes Tower, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, Email: dollagha+@pitt.edu

Abstract

Deficits in lexical access in children with specific language impairments (SLI) have been inferred from word naming, recall, and categorization tasks, but no evidence exists concerning these children's ability to identify words in spoken input. We presented successive auditory time gatings of unfamiliar words; familiar, phonologically related words; and familiar, phonologically unrelated words to school-age children with and without SLI. The groups did not differ significantly in the point at which they recognized familiar words, but the subjects with SLI required significantly more of the acoustic-phonetic signal than did their peers to recognize unfamiliar words. For all word types, the subjects with SLI were significantly less likely to respond with correct initial consonants at the earliest gated interval than were their peers. Our results suggest that both representational and perceptual inefficiencies may contribute to slowed lexical access in children with SLI.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1998

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [ASHA]. (1990). Guidelines for screening forhearing impairment and middle-ear disorders. Asha, 32 (Supplement 2), 1724.Google Scholar
Baddeiey, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bleile, K. M, Wallach, H. (1992). A sociolinguistic investigation of the speech of African Ameri-can preschoolers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 1, 5462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, G. D. A., Watson, F. L. (1987). First in, first out: Word learning age and spoken wordfrequency as predictors of word familiarity and word naming latency. Memory Cognition, 15, 208216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R., Johnsen, S. (1990). Test of Nonverbal Intelligence 2. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Burgess, C, Tancnhaus, M., Seidenberg, M. (1989). Context and lexical access: Implications ofnonword interference for lexical ambiguity resolution. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 620632.Google ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (1981). The child as word learner. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J., Miller, G. (eds.), Linguistictheory and psychological reality (pp. 264293). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S., Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Lan-guage Development (Stanford University), 15, 1729.Google Scholar
Carr, T., McCauley, C, Sperber, R., Parmelee, C. (1982). Words, pictures and priming: Onsemantic activation, conscious identification, and the automaticity of information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8, 757777.Google ScholarPubMed
Cluff, M. S., Luce, P. A. (1990). Similarity neighborhoods of spoken two-syllable words: Retro-active effects on multiple activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Percep-tion and Performance, 16, 551563.Google Scholar
Cotton, S., Grosjean, F. (1984). The gating paradigm: A comparison of successive and individualpresentation formats. Perception Psychophysics, 35, 4148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, C., Kim, B. (1990). Effects of time gating and word length on isolated word-recognitionperformance. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 33, 808815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, C. H., Kim, B. W., Pecyna Rhyner, P., Bowen Chirillo, T. K. (1993). Effects of wordpredictability, child development, and aging on time-gated speech recognition performance. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 832841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dagenbach, D., Carr, T., Bamhart, T. (1990). Inhibitory semantic priming of lexical decisions dueto failure to retrieve weakly activated codes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 16, 328340.Google Scholar
Denckla, M. B., Rudel, R. G. (1976). Naming of object drawings by dyslexic and other learningdisabled children. Brain and Language, 3, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollaghan, C. (1985). Child meets word: "Fast mapping in preschool children. Journal of Speechand Hearing Research, 28, 449454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollaghan, C. (1987). Fast mapping in normal and language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 218222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollaghan, C. (1994). Childrens phonological neighborhoods: Half empty or half full? Journal of Child Lan-guage, 21, 257271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollaghan, C, Campbell, T. (in press). Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Manuscript submitted for publication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L., Dunn, L. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: Ameri-can Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Elliott, L., Hammer, M, Evan, K. (1987). Perception of gated, highly familiar spoken monosyl-labic nouns by children, teenagers, and older adults. Perception Psychophysics, 42, 150157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, L., Hammer, M., Scholl, M. (1989). Fine-grained auditory discrimination in normal chil-dren and children with language-learning problems. Journal of Speech and Hearing Re-search, 32, 112119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frauenfelder, U., Tyler, L. (1987). The process of spoken word recognition: An introduction. Cognition, 25, 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galhercole, S., Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered chil-dren: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language. 29, 336360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillam, R. B., Cowan, N., Day, L. S. (1995).Sequential memory in children with and withoutlanguage impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 393402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grainger, J. (1990). Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision andnaming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 228244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1980). Spoken word recognition processes and the gating paradigm. Perception Psychophysics, 28, 267283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosjean, F. (1985). The recognition of words after their acoustic offset: Evidence and implication. Perception Psychophysics, 38, 299310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F., Gee, J. P. (1987). Prosodic structure and spoken word recognition. Cognition, 25, 135155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammill, D., Newcomer, P. (1988). Test of Language Development 2: Intermediate. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Just, M., Carpenter, P. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences inworking memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R. (1994). A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specificlanguage impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 418421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R., Hale, C, Leonard, L., Nippold, M. (1986). Lexical storage and retrieval in language-impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 5, 3749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kail, R., Leonard, L. (1986). Word finding abilities in language-impaired children (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Monograph No. 25). Rockville, MD: AmericanSpeech-Language-Hearing Association.Google ScholarPubMed
Kamhi, A., Catts, H., Mauer, D., Apel, K., Gentry, B. (1988). Phonological and spatial processingabilities in language- and reading-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disor-ders, 53, 316327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, W. F., Curtiss, S., Tallal, P. (1992). Rapid automatized naming and gesture by normal andlanguage-impaired children. Brain and Language, 43, 623641.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay Elemetrics Corporation. (1994). Computerized Speech Lab Model 4300B. Pine Brook, NJ.Google Scholar
King, J., Just, M. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of workingmemory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 580602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. (1987). Is specific language impairment a useful construct In Rosenberg, S. (Ed.), Ad-vances in applied psycholinguistics (Vol. 1, pp. 139). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leonard, L., McGregor, K., Allen, G. (1992). Grammatical morphology and speech perception in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 10761085.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L., Nippold, M., Kail, R., Hale, C. (1983). Picture naming in language-impaired chil-dren. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 26, 609615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, S. (1994). Language and bilingual-bicultural children. In Reed, V. A. (Ed.), Introduction tochildren with language disorders (pp. 290317). New York: Merrill.Google Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25, 71102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1989). Access and integration: Projecting sound onto meaning. In Marslen-Wilson, W. (Ed.), Lexical representation and process (pp. 324). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J., Elman, J. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18. 186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McQueen, J. M. (1993). Rhyme decisions to spoken words and nonwords. Memory and Cognition, 21, 210222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moe, A., Hopkins, C, Rush, R. (1982). The vocabulary of first-grade children. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Montgomery, J. W., Scudder, R. R., Moore, C. A. (1990). Language-impaired childrens real-time comprehension of spoken language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 273290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, C. M., Ellis, A. W., Quinlan, P. T. (1992). Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition. Memory Cognition, 20, 705714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newcomer, P., Hammill, D. (1988). Test of Language Development 2: Primary. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Nygaard, L. C. (1993). Phonetic coherence in duplex perception: Effects of acoustic differences andlexical status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 268286.Google ScholarPubMed
Pisoni, D., Luce, P. (1987). Acoustic-phonetic representations in word recognition. Cognition, 25, 2152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. (1989). The language development survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reznick, J., Goldsmith, L. (1989). A multiple form word production checklist for assessing early language. Journal of Child Language, 16, 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salasoo, A., Pisoni, D. B. (1985). Interaction of knowledge sources in spoken word identification. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 210231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, M., McClelland, J. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sininger, Y., Klatzky, R., Kirchner, D. (1989). Memory scanning speed in language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 32, 289297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, R. E., Montgomery, J. W. (1995). Sentence processing in language-impaired children under conditions of filtering and time compression. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 137154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sussman, J. E. (1993). Perception of formant transition cues to place of articulation in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 12861299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S. S., Schreiner, C, Jenkins, W. M., Merzenich, M. M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 8184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., Piercy, M. (1975). Developmental aphasia: The perception of brief vowels and ex-tended stop consonants. Neuropsychologia, 13, 6974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tallal, P., Stark, R., Kallman, C, Mellits, D. (1981). A reexamination of some nonverbal percep-tual abilities of language-impaired and normal children as a function of age and sensory modality. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 24, 351357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, O. (1986). Language and communication differences. In Shames, G.Wiig, E. (eds.), Human communication disorders (pp. 126158). Columbus, OH: Merrill.Google Scholar
Tyler, L. K., Wessels, J. (1985). Is gating an on-line task? Evidence from naming latency data. Perception Psychophysics, 38, 217222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L. (1992). Young childrens age-of-acquisition estimates for spoken words. Memory Cognition, 20, 171182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, B. U., Miller, T. K. (1993). Auditory perception, phonological processing, and reading ability/disability. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 850863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed