Skip to main content
×
×
Home

Tracing the development of spatially modulated gestures in the manual modality in nonsigning Chinese-speaking children

  • WING-CHEE SO (a1) and KIT-YI MIRANDA WONG (a1)
Abstract
ABSTRACT

Speakers employ gestures to identify nonpresent objects in nonneutral locations (e.g., pointing to the left to indicate a toy car) and maintain the same spatial locations to indicate the same objects throughout the discourse. These kinds of gestures are referred to as shared-location spatially modulated gestures. Previous research showed that native signers form associations between locations and objects as early as age 5. We examined whether nonsigning children also produced shared-location spatially modulated gestures as early as age 5, and if so, whether such ability is associated with verbal memory, spatial memory, and eductive reasoning (i.e., the ability to derive meaning from the complex situations). Our results showed that nonsigning children use spatial devices in their gestures as early as age 5, but such ability is not well developed until age 7 to 11. It is interesting that different types of cognitive skills may come into play in the production of shared-location spatially modulated gestures across different age groups. We found significant correlations between the production of this type of gesture and spatial memory in the younger children and eductive reasoning in the older children. Thus, there is possibly a developmental shift of the cognitive processes involved in the production of shared-location spatially modulated gestures.

Copyright
Corresponding author
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Wing-Chee So, Department of Educational Psychology, Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong. E-mail: wingchee@cuhk.edu.hk
References
Hide All
Aronoff M., Meir I., Padden C., & Sandler W. (2003). Classifier constructions and morphology in two sign languages. In Emmorey K. (Ed.), Perspectives on classifier constructions in sign languages (pp. 5384). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Bellugi U., Lillo-Martin D., O'Grady L., & van Hoek K. (1990). The development of spatialized syntactic mechanisms in American Sign Language. In Edmondson W. & Karlsson F. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Fourth International Symposium on Sign Language Research (pp. 1625). Hamburg: Signum-Verlag Press.
Bellugi U., van Hoek K., Lillo-Martin D., & O'Grady L. (1993). The acquisition of syntax and space in young deaf signers. In Bishop D. & Mogford K. (Eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances (pp. 132149). Edinburgh: Churchill-Livingstone. (Original work published 1988)
Bowerman M., & Choi S. (2001). Shaping meanings for language: Universal and language-specific in the acquisition of semantic categories. In Bowerman M. & Levinson S. C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 475511). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Cassell J., & McNeill D. (1991). Non-verbal imagery and the poetics of prose. Poetics Today, 12, 375404.
Chan A. S. (2006). The Hong Kong List Learning Test (2nd ed.). Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong, Department of Psychology.
Chan A. S., Ho Y. C., & Cheung M. C. (1998). Music training improves verbal memory. Nature, 396, 128.
Chan A. S., Ho S., & Poon W. S. (2002). Neuropsychological sequelae of patients treated with microsurgical clipping or endovascular embolization for anterior communicating artery aneurysm. European Neurology, 47, 3744.
Chan A. S., Kwok J., Chiu H., Lam L., Pang A., & Chow L. (2000). Memory and organizational strategies in chronic and acute schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 41, 431445.
Cheung M.-C., Chan A. S., Law S. C., Chan J. H., & Tse V. K. (2000). Cognitive function of patients with nasopharyngeal carcinoma with and without temporal lobe radionecrosis. Archives of Neurology, 57, 13471352.
Chu M., Meyer A. S., Foulkes L., & Kita S. (2014). Individual differences in frequency and saliency of speech-accompanying gestures: The role of cognitive abilities and empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 694709.
Emmorey K. (1996). The confluence of space and language in signed language. In Bloom P., Peterson M. S., Nadel L., & Garrettt M. F. (Eds.), Language and Space (pp. 171209). Cambidge, MA: MIT Press.
Emmorey K. (2002). Language, cognition, and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Engberg-Pedersen E. (1993). Space in Danish sign language: The semantics and morphosyntax of the use of space in a visual language. Hamburg: Signum Press.
Gullberg M. (1998). Gesture as a communication strategy in second language discourse: A study of learners of French and Swedish. Lund, Sweden: Lund University Press.
Gullberg M. (2003). Gestures, referents, and anaphoric linkage in learner varieties. In Dimroth C. & Starren M. (Eds.), Information structure and the dynamics of language acquisition (pp. 311328). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Gullberg M. (2006). Handling discourse: Gestures, reference tracking, and communication strategies in early L2. Language Learning, 56, 155196.
Hostetter A. B., & Alibali M. W. (2007). Raise your hand if you're spatial: Relations between verbal and spatial skills and representational gesture production. Gesture, 7, 7395.
Kita S. (2009). Cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 145167.
Levinson S. C. (2003). Space in language and cognition: Explorations in cognitive diversity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lillo-Martin D., Bellugi U., Struxness L., & O'Grady M. (1985). The acquisition of spatially organized syntax. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 24, 7078.
Loew R. (1984). Roles and reference in American Sign Language: A developmental perspective (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota).
Ma T. M. K. (1999). The clinical validity of the Hong Kong List Learning Test in identifying patients with temporal lobe lesions (Unpublished master's thesis, Chinese University of Hong Kong).
McNeill D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
McNeill D. (2000). Catchments and contexts: Non-modular factors in speech and gesture production. In McNeill D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 312328). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
McNeill D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
McNeill D. (2015). Why we gesture? New York: Cambridge University Press.
McNeill D., & Levy E. T. (1993). Cohesion and gesture. Discourse Processes, 16, 363386.
Meyers J. E., & Meyers K. R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Raven J. C. (1936). Mental tests used in genetic studies: The performances of related individuals in tests mainly educative and mainly reproductive (Unpublished master's thesis, University of London).
Raven J., Raven J. C., & Court J. H. (2003). Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales: Section 1. General overview. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Sassenberg U., Foth M., Wartenburger I., & van der Meer E. (2011). Show your hands—Are you really clever? Reasoning, gesture production, and intelligence. Linguistics, 49, 105134.
Sekine K., & Furuyama N. (2010). Developmental change of discourse cohesion in speech and gestures among Japanese elementary school children. Rivista di Psicolinguistica Applicata, 10, 97116.
Senghas A. (2010). The emergence of two functions for spatial devices in Nicaraguan Sign Language. Human Development, 53, 287302.
Senghas A., & Coppola M. (2001). Children creating language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science, 12, 323328.
Slobin D. I., Hoiting N., Kuntze M., Lindert R., Weinberg A., Pyers J., . . . Thumann H. (2003). A cognitive/functional perspective on the acquisition of “classifiers.” In Emmorey K. (Ed.), Perspectives on classifier constructions in sign languages (pp. 271296). New York: Psychology Press.
So W. C., Coppola M., Licciardello V., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2005). The seeds of spatial grammar in the manual modality. Cognitive Science, 29, 10291043.
So W. C., Kita S., & Goldin-Meadow S. (2009). Using the hands to identify who does what to whom: Gesture and speech go hand-in-hand. Cognitive Science, 33, 115125.
So W. C., Lui M., Wong T. K., & Sit L. T. (2015). The use of hand gestures to communicate about nonpresent objects in mind among children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 58, 373382.
Supalla T. (1982). Structure and acquisition of verbs of motion and location in American Sign Language (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego).
Supalla T. (1995). An implicational hierarchy in verb agreement in American Sign Language. Unpublished manuscript, University of Rochester.
Talmy L. (2003). The representation of spatial structure in spoken and signed language. In Emmorey K. (Ed.), Perspectives on classifier construction in sign language (pp. 169196). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Yoshioka K. (2008). Gesture and information structure in first and second language. Gesture, 8, 236255.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Applied Psycholinguistics
  • ISSN: 0142-7164
  • EISSN: 1469-1817
  • URL: /core/journals/applied-psycholinguistics
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords:

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 2
Total number of PDF views: 21 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 129 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between 20th November 2017 - 21st February 2018. This data will be updated every 24 hours.