Hostname: page-component-6bb9c88b65-wr9vw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-07-21T19:29:54.929Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

What’s wrong with motivational interviewing? I. Theoretical and methodological critiques

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 July 2025

Lars G. Forsberg
Affiliation:
MIC Lab AB, Stockholm, Sweden
Lisa Forsberg*
Affiliation:
Uehiro Oxford Institute, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
William R. Miller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA
*
Corresponding author: Lisa Forsberg; Email: lisa.forsberg@uehiro.ox.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Motivational Interviewing (MI) has demonstrated significant effects in diverse areas of practice, with over 2,000 controlled clinical trials published. Some criticisms of MI have emerged along the way.

Aims:

We examine theoretical and methodological critiques of MI.

Method:

We discuss three significant theoretical and methodological criticisms of MI: (1) that MI lacks conceptual stability; (2) that MI lacks a theoretical foundation; and (3) that MI is just common factors in psychotherapy.

Results:

It is true that definitions and descriptions of MI have evolved over the years. Mastery of MI clearly varies across providers, and when the quality of an intervention is unmeasured, it is unclear what has been trained or delivered. Reliable and valid tools to assess MI fidelity are available but often unused in outcome studies. It remains unclear what levels of proficiency are necessary to improve client outcomes. Some attempts to minimize variability in the delivery of MI appear to have reduced its effectiveness. In respect of the second critique is that MI lacks a theoretical foundation. It is unclear whether and how this is a disadvantage in research and practice. Various theories have been proposed and specific causal chain predictions have been tested. A third critique is that MI is merely common factors found among psychotherapists. The contribution of such relational skills is testable. There are specific aspects of MI related to client language that influence client outcomes above and beyond its relational components.

Conclusions:

The critiques reflect important factors to consider when delivering, training, and evaluating MI research.

Information

Type
Main
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies

Introduction

Motivational interviewing (MI) is ‘a particular way of talking with people about change and growth to strengthen their own motivation and commitment’ (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2023). Some MI skills are general and used in other forms of everyday communication. At the same time, MI is a complicated skill set that supports practitioners in conversations about behaviour change where the path towards change can be hard to see. Like downhill skiing, MI is easy to understand and difficult to learn to use skillfully. MI skills are best acquired by regularly practising with reliable feedback. MI has been widely used in treating a variety of health concerns, with more than 2000 controlled clinical trials published in the four decades since it was first described (Miller, Reference Miller1983). Meta-analyses have reported significant effects of MI for reducing complex behaviours where individuals are often ambivalent, seeing both advantages and disadvantages; for example in alcohol, tobacco, and cannabis use (Calomarde-Gómez et al., Reference Calomarde-Gómez, Jiménez-Fernández, Balcells-Oliveró, Gual and López-Pelayo2021; Lundahl et al., Reference Lundahl, Kunz, Brownell, Tollefson and Burke2010), changing health behaviour in medical (Lundahl et al., Reference Lundahl, Moleni, Burke, Butters, Tollefson, Butler and Rollnick2013; Palacio et al., Reference Palacio, Garay, Langer, Taylor, Wood and Tamariz2016) and dental care (Borrelli et al., Reference Borrelli, Tooley and Scott-Sheldon2015), and increasing treatment adherence, retention, and completion (Hettema et al., Reference Hettema, Steele and Miller2005; Lawrence et al., Reference Lawrence, Fulbrook, Somerset and Schulz2017; Wong-Anuchit et al., Reference Wong-Anuchit, Chantamit-o-pas, Schneider and Mills2019). The broad diffusion of MI is reflected in controlled trials with significant effects as diverse as enhanced adoption of clean drinking water practices in Zambia (Thevos et al., Reference Thevos, Quick and Yanjuli2000), improved communication skills for Swedish veterinarians in animal health management (Svensson et al., Reference Svensson, Wickström, Forsberg, Betnér, von Brömssen, Reyher, Bard and Emanuelson2022), and improved academic performance of college students in New Mexico and Nigeria (Chike-Okoli and Okoli, Reference Chike-Okoli and Okoli2018; Daugherty, Reference Daugherty2003).

MI, then, is now widely used in many professions and contexts, nations and languages. However, various critiques of MI have also emerged through its continuing evolution (Miller, Reference Miller2023). Critiques may help in understanding MI and in clarifying the method. This article summarizes and discusses three significant theoretical and methodological criticisms of MI: (1) that MI lacks conceptual stability; (2) that MI lacks a theoretical foundation; and (3) that MI is just common factors in psychotherapy. We plan to discuss further ethical critiques of MI in a subsequent article.

MI lacks conceptual stability

One prominent critique of MI is that it lacks conceptual stability. So, what is MI? How do we know whether what we are testing in studies and teaching practitioners really is MI?

Both Björk (Reference Björk2014) and Atkinson and Woods (Reference Atkinson and Woods2017) fault the conceptual stability of MI, describing how definitions of MI have changed over time. For example, Miller and Rollnick (Reference Miller and Rollnick1991) first set forth five principles of MI: express empathy, develop discrepancy, avoid argumentation, roll with resistance, and support self-efficacy. In their second edition (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2002) this was reduced to four principles, collapsing develop discrepancy and avoid argumentation into ‘roll with resistance’. Next, they vacated the principles altogether, instead describing four processes of MI (engaging, focusing, evoking, and planning) and deconstructing their previously used concept of resistance (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2013). Atkinson and Woods also note other changes in the definition of MI, including that from 2003 onwards there was increased focus on client language by eliciting change talk and avoiding elaboration on the client’s reasons for maintaining status quo behaviors (sustain talk), and later increased emphasis on avoiding MI-inconsistent responses. There also appeared a new description of the underlying ‘spirit’ with which MI is to be practised (Rollnick and Miller, Reference Rollnick and Miller1995) that has received increasing emphasis through subsequent editions of the principal text (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2023). Whereas subtitles of the 1991 and 2002 texts described preparing people for change, subsequent versions removed this preparatory emphasis, focusing instead more broadly on helping people change.

Some of the changes made in how MI is defined and described have been carefully explained with reference to data from emerging research. One such example is how client speech came to feature more prominently in descriptions of MI after 2003, particularly influenced by the psycholinguistic research of Paul Amrhein (Amrhein, Reference Amrhein2004; Amrhein et al., Reference Amrhein, Miller, Yahne, Palmer and Fulcher2003). Different categories of client change talk (and sustain talk) were recognized, and MI practitioners were advised how to respond to client speech as a result of emerging research showing that client in-session speech and how MI practitioners respond to it predict whether behaviour change will happen. It became clear from correlational, sequential, and experimental studies that counsellors influence the balance of clients’ change talk and sustain talk (Apodaca and Longabaugh, Reference Apodaca and Longabaugh2009; Gaume et al., Reference Gaume, Bertholet, Faouzi, Gmel and Daeppen2010; Glynn and Moyers, Reference Glynn and Moyers2010; Walthers et al., Reference Walthers, Janssen, Mastroleo, Hoadley, Barnett, Colby and Magill2019) which in turn predicts subsequent client behaviour change (Gaume et al., Reference Gaume, Bertholet, Faouzi, Gmel and Daeppen2013; Lindqvist et al., Reference Lindqvist, Forsberg, Enebrink, Andersson and Rosendahl2017; Magill et al., Reference Magill, Apodaca, Borsari, Gaume, Hoadley, Gordon, Tonigan and Moyers2018).

Other changes to MI’s definition or description, however, have not been explained in relation to research findings. Atkinson and Woods note, for example, that no clear explanation was given for the transition from describing MI in terms of principles to describing it in terms of processes. Another example of an unexplained theoretical shift is the varying relationship between MI and the transtheoretical model of change (TTM; Prochaska and DiClemente, Reference Prochaska and DiClemente1984) that seemed central in early descriptions of MI (Miller, Reference Miller1983; Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick1991), then more distant in later editions. Frustration with such conceptual changes is understandable. They lead to uncertainty regarding what is in fact being tested in studies and what is being taught in MI training. TTM (Prochaska and DiClemente, Reference Prochaska and DiClemente1984) supports clinicians’ understanding of different types of ambivalence and responding to them appropriately. TTM’s formulation of change as a multi-faceted process including many types of ambivalence, rather than seeing change as a dichotomous process may have contributed to MI’s spread to areas where more complicated change is the focus, such as smoking, alcohol use, etc. Different types of ambivalence require different MI skills. However, TTM’s theoretical foundation in MI has been toned down and the reasoning for doing so has been unclear.

Atkinson and Woods also point out that available treatment manuals tend to describe not MI on its own – or ‘pure’ MI – but rather how MI can be combined with other interventions as in motivational enhancement therapy (MET; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Zweben, DiClemente and Rychtarik1992) or a combined behavioural intervention (Miller, Reference Miller2004). Similarly, other researchers have described how MI should be done in concert with another study intervention (e.g. Naar et al., Reference Naar, Pennar, Wang, Brogan-Hartlieb and Fortenberry2021), often referred to as adaptations of MI (Burke et al., Reference Burke, Arkowitz and Menchola2003). ‘Pure’ MI had not been manualized until recently (Hurlocker et al., Reference Hurlocker, Moyers, Hatch, Curran, McCrady, Venner and Witkiewitz2023). It is indeed the case that by far the most common use of MI in controlled clinical trials has been in combination with other evidence-based treatment, sometimes as a pre-treatment intervention (Westra et al., Reference Westra, Arkowitz and Dozois2009) but more often integrated in less specified ways. In the largest randomized trial of treatments for alcohol use disorders (Project Match, Babor and Del Boca, Reference Babor and Del Boca2003), MI in combination with objective health feedback was associated with behaviour change, with developing discrepancy on the basis of negative objective health feedback being an important ingredient.

In MI, the focus now is on how ambivalence is expressed in language, even if ambivalence can also be expressed in feelings, body language, silence, and behaviour. The concept of ambivalence can be seen as having been given an operational definition in MI in client speech concerning change versus client speech favouring the status quo.

Atkinson and Woods’ criticism that MI lacks a stable definition and a manual for its use is echoed by Björk (Reference Björk2014) who noted that there is often no definition of MI in the scientific studies that have been conducted on its efficacy and effectiveness. Björk, too, notes that when defined in research study manuals, MI tends to be used in combination with another intervention rather than as ‘pure MI’. Both argue that the absence of clear definition gives rise to important methodological and practical problems for the method and its practice, as well as for research on its efficacy and effectiveness. For example, the lack of definition makes it uncertain exactly what has been delivered in different studies and to what extent MI is the same across studies. Changes in how MI was described in the early 1990s versus 20 years later may have caused differences in how MI was done in studies. MI is often described as an evidence-based method for changing behaviour. However, it is likely that the intervention referred to as ‘MI’ has been done differently across the many studies that make up its evidence basis. This concern was also raised by Miller and Rollnick (Reference Miller and Rollnick2014) with regard to MI and behavioural interventions more generally.

Björk also notes, however, that while MI has lacked a clear and consistent definition, the method has been stabilized by other kinds of efforts. One such effort is the development and widespread use of MI fidelity assessment tools, the first of which was the Motivational Interviewing Skills Code (MISC; DeJonge et al., Reference DeJonge, Schippers and Schaap2005; Miller and Mount, Reference Miller and Mount2001). A simplified Motivational Interviewing Treatment Integrity (MITI) code followed, with demonstrated reliability and validity, and which continues to be updated (Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Rowell, Manuel, Ernst and Houck2016), with practice samples more recently subjected to automated machine coding via voice recognition (e.g. Tanana et al., Reference Tanana, Hallgren, Imel, Atkins and Srikumar2016). It remains unclear what levels of proficiency practitioners need on such measures in order to improve clients’ outcomes. It is also clear that in most research and clinical applications of MI to date there has been little or no use of such tools to document fidelity (Lundahl et al., Reference Lundahl, Moleni, Burke, Butters, Tollefson, Butler and Rollnick2013), so their overall effect on stabilizing MI practice may be minimal (Atkinson and Woods, Reference Atkinson and Woods2017).

In psychotherapy, the idea of a standardized treatment manual is a relatively recent by-product of funding for clinical trials that require specification of the interventions to be evaluated. When developing new psychological treatments, it has become common to first develop a step-by-step therapist manual to guide practice and later the dissemination of the intervention. Such homogenization of practice is neither common nor particularly welcome in most clinical service settings, but is it a good idea? Atkinson and Woods note that in the Lundahl et al. (Reference Lundahl, Moleni, Burke, Butters, Tollefson, Butler and Rollnick2013) meta-analysis of trials in health care settings, studies that measured MI fidelity produced lower effect sizes compared with those that did not document fidelity. Another meta-analysis (Hettema et al., Reference Hettema, Steele and Miller2005) found that intervention effect size was significantly lower when the delivery of MI was manual-guided. These two findings indicate that attempts to stabilize the fidelity of MI and minimize its variability may reduce its effectiveness. In a clinical trial for which William Miller personally wrote the standardized treatment manual and personally trained and supervised the therapists, there was no effect of MET on client outcomes (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Yahne and Tonigan2003). One explanation could be that treatment integrity measured in terms of fidelity to a manual or treatment integrity assessment tool might miss important MI skills. Perhaps process studies can shed some light on this issue, highlighting a need to understand MI better. The fidelity issue may be a signal that manuals have lacked a component that is otherwise easier to perform, and that what we measure in MI perhaps does not correlate enough with well-performed MI. Clearly there is more to learn about the content of MI.

In his critique, Björk observes that in contrast to manual-guided treatment, MI has evolved in a manner similar to how technological innovations are often developed and disseminated (cf. Rogers, Reference Rogers2003). Such innovations are often co-created by many people using a methodology that seeks to understand how the innovation works and why. Björk notes that the invention of a technology is seldom an isolated event but rather a long-term process whereby people involved in research and in practical applications test applications in new areas. Technological innovations often begin with attempts to measure features that are hypothesized to be active components. MI seems to mirror this in the way in which researchers and practitioners of MI have both been involved in developing the method and applying it in many different fields and contexts.

In MI, this broad diffusion has happened by training people to teach others in how to use the method. A first Training of New Trainers (TNT) was offered by Miller and Rollnick in 1993. In 1995, a loose network of MI trainers was formed that would later be formally organized as the Motivational Interviewing Network of Trainers (MINT). As Björk notes, MINT established ties between researchers, trainers, and practitioners, developing a culture that encourages its members to ‘give more than you take’. Materials and methods were freely shared to facilitate MI practice, training, and research. First on an email listserv and later on a web-based platform, many kinds of MI-related challenges and solutions were discussed among MINT members. Once a year, new MI trainers were trained, and in connection with the TNT there developed an informal MI conference known as the MINT Forum, where members shared updates, views, and innovations. As new research findings emerged these, too, were disseminated via MINT, influencing future training, practice, and research. This organized collaboration between practitioners, trainers and researchers for more than 30 years has stabilized what MI is and supported the practice and training of MI in a unique way for a psychotherapeutic intervention and its development.

In essence, MI has been analogous to open-source software – freely available for those who are interested in trying it. The authors made no attempt to trademark, franchise, copyright, or otherwise control or restrict its use (Miller, Reference Miller2023; Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2023). This may account in part for its widespread adoption across different problems, settings, practitioner groups, nations, and languages (Bjőrk, Reference Björk2014). Atkinson and Woods also note that MI as an intervention is flexible and capable of being applied in many different contexts with a wide range of clients. Like the person-centred approach of Carl Rogers on which it is based (Miller and Moyers, Reference Miller and Moyers2017), MI has been applied in many fields including education (Herman et al., Reference Herman, Reinke, Frey and Shepard2021; Rollnick et al., Reference Rollnick, Kaplan and Rutschman2016), negotiation (Amador, Reference Amador2022), pastoral care (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Giordano, Cashwell and Lewis2013; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Forcehimes, O’Leary and LaNoue2008), leadership and management (Marshall and Nielsen, Reference Marshall and Nielsen2020; Organ, Reference Organ2021), and social work (Forrester et al., Reference Forrester, Wilkins and Whittaker2021; Hohman, Reference Hohman2021).

A necessary consequence of open sourcing is a lack of consistency and quality assurance in MI delivery and training. No permission or certification is required to practise MI or claim to do so. The same is true, of course, for nearly all forms of therapy, counselling, and coaching. Even if it were desirable for a treatment method to be unilaterally defined and unchanging, that is not the reality of psychotherapies. Their processes and outcomes vary with the person providing them (Miller and Moyers, Reference Miller and Moyers2021).

The MINT offers to practitioners and trainers updated training methods and exercises, new research findings, and innovative applications of MI. MINT’s collaborative and supportive culture of sharing and giving back have contributed to the development and dissemination of MI. MINT also promotes interaction among professionals in research, practice, and training. As noted above, a potential downside of such free exchange is a lack of control over how MI is spread and used. In combination with conceptual changes over time, there is understandable concern about what is actually being delivered in practice, taught in training, and tested in studies of MI.

MI lacks a theoretical foundation

A second criticism of MI is that it has no consistent or coherent theoretical basis (Atkinson and Woods, Reference Atkinson and Woods2017). Implicit in this critique is an ideal that a psychotherapy should be deductively derived from and guided by a pre-existing theory of personality or therapy. Proponents of this critique have been less clear about what disadvantages are bestowed by the lack of an a priori theoretical foundation. Are atheoretical therapies inherently more difficult to learn or more variable in practice? Effective medications are sometimes discovered by accident without a theoretical reason for or understanding of mechanisms of their efficacy.

In becoming President of the American Psychological Association in 1947, Carl Rogers argued that clinical psychology should be an empirical science with measurable therapeutic process and outcomes. His work was a nascent clinical science and a forerunner of current research on active ingredients and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy (e.g. Magill et al., Reference Magill, Kiluk, McCrady, Tonigan and Longabaugh2015). His person-centred approach was derived not from a pre-existing theory but abductively through close observation of clinical practice to develop and test hypotheses about what therapeutic factors actually help clients change (Kirschenbaum, Reference Kirschenbaum2009). Theories arose later to explain the results being observed (e.g. Gendlin, Reference Gendlin1961; Rogers, Reference Rogers and Koch1959).

MI similarly began from close examination and discussion of therapeutic practice (Miller, Reference Miller1983) stimulated in part by the incidental finding of an unexpectedly large effect of counsellor empathy on cognitive behaviour therapy outcomes (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Taylor and West1980). There was no predominant theory guiding its development; MI has been described as quintessentially pragmatic (Carr, Reference Carr2023). Early hypotheses were operationalized and tested in clinical trials (Brown and Miller, Reference Brown and Miller1993; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Sovereign and Krege1988; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Benefield and Tonigan1993; Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Miller and Hendrickson2005), eventually integrating Amrhein’s psycholinguistic findings (Amrhein, Reference Amrhein2004; Amrhein et al., Reference Amrhein, Miller, Yahne, Palmer and Fulcher2003). Tentative theories began to emerge (de Almeida Neto, Reference de Almeida Neto2017; Markland et al., Reference Markland, Ryan, Tobin and Rollnick2005; Miller and Rose, Reference Miller and Rose2009) along with the development of causal chain predictions of client outcomes (Magill et al., Reference Magill, Apodaca, Barnett and Monti2010, Reference Magill, Apodaca, Borsari, Gaume, Hoadley, Gordon, Tonigan and Moyers2018; Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Martin, Houck, Christopher and Tonigan2009).

As with programmatic studies of the person-centred approach (Truax and Carkhuff, Reference Truax and Carkhuff1967), the above-described lines of research have provided increasingly clear guidelines for clinical practice of MI. Has the absence of prior theory impaired delivery and learning of MI? Although MI can be simplified conceptually (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2023), the available evidence on training does indicate that MI can be challenging to learn, with large variability in mastery across individuals. One obstacle is that without reliable performance feedback, clinicians can substantially over-estimate their proficiency with MI, undermining motivation to continue learning (Miller and Mount, Reference Miller and Mount2001). Another challenge is difficulty in unlearning prior MI-inconsistent habits of practice (Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Darnell, Atkins, Hallgren, Imel, Bumgardner, Owens and Roy-Byrne2016; Madson et al., Reference Madson, Loignon and Lane2009; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Yahne, Moyers, Martinez and Pirritano2004). These obstacles are not unique to MI, nor is the need for substantial time to develop mastery of a psychotherapy. Regardless of whether these are greater difficulties for MI and whether they have any connection to its lack of a theoretical foundation, the evidence that learning MI is both challenging and variable is reason enough to question whether it is indeed ‘MI’ that has spread widely to so many settings and practitioner groups. It remains unclear what elements of MI have been disseminated, and ‘the efficacy of MI approaches is unclear given the inconsistency of MI descriptions and intervention components’ (Morton et al., Reference Morton, Beauchamp, Prothero, Joyce, Saunders, Spencer-Bowdage, Dancy and Pedlar2015).

MI is just common factors

If MI has lacked theoretical grounding, could it be nothing more than general components of good practice that are sometimes referred to as ‘common’ or ‘non-specific’ factors in psychotherapy? These two terms can themselves be misleading. Allegedly ‘common’ factors are not universal practices found in all therapies or therapists. Neither does ‘non-specific’ mean that these factors are unspecifiable or unmeasurable. The meaning of both ‘non-specific’ and ‘common’ is that these practices are not unique or limited to any particular theoretical orientation in psychotherapy. Perhaps a better term, then, would be therapeutic factors (Kivlighan and Holmes, Reference Kivlighan and Holmes2004).

So, what are these skills of more effective therapists? Miller and Moyers (Reference Miller and Moyers2021) reviewed 70 years of psychotherapy research to identify therapeutic skills that distinguish clinicians whose clients show better outcomes compared with those treated by their peer practitioners working within the same setting, theoretical orientation, or delivering the same specific and even manualized treatment. These therapist factors often have substantially more impact on client outcomes than specific treatment procedures that are being delivered (Imel et al., Reference Imel, Wampold, Miller and Fleming2008; Wampold and Brown, Reference Wampold and Brown2005). Miller and Moyers identified eight such factors empirically associated with more effective therapists: accurate empathy, acceptance, positive regard, genuineness, focus, hope, evocation, and offering information and advice.

Of these eight factors, seven have been explicitly described and taught in MI since its inception. Only genuineness was unmentioned, an omission corrected in the most recent edition of the source text (Miller and Rollnick, Reference Miller and Rollnick2023). In this sense, MI does appear to embody what renders helpers more helpful, operationalizing and combining these common non-specific therapeutic skills. This is consistent with the finding in addiction research that MI can improve client outcomes when added to other evidence-based treatments (Hettema et al., Reference Hettema, Steele and Miller2005). It also suggests testable hypotheses about what is actually being ‘added’ by MI, coming full circle to the aforementioned seminal finding that therapist empathy substantially improved outcomes of cognitive behaviour therapy (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Taylor and West1980). MI calls attention to often ignored therapist factors that can improve client outcomes across a wide array of treatment methods and clinical problems.

Is MI merely a compilation of these non-specific therapeutic skills? This is a testable question. However, MI has something more in its treatment bag. Beyond the person-centred relational element of MI there is also a technical component related to client language known as change talk and sustain talk (Magill et al., Reference Magill, Apodaca, Borsari, Gaume, Hoadley, Gordon, Tonigan and Moyers2018; Miller and Rose, Reference Miller and Rose2009). Specifically training this aspect of MI in addition to the relational skills has been shown to differentially impact clients’ in-session speech that has been linked to subsequent change (Moyers et al., Reference Moyers, Houck, Glynn, Hallgren and Manual2017). The person-centred relational skills of MI can themselves influence change and sustain talk (DeVargas and Stormshak, Reference DeVargas and Stormshak2020). Three experimental trials have compared MI with a non-directive person-centred condition embodying the relational aspect of MI without seeking to evoke change talk. In two of these studies the MI condition (which included differential responding to change and sustain talk) yielded significantly greater (Sellman et al., Reference Sellman, Sullivan, Dore, Adamson and MacEwan2001) or faster change (Morgenstern et al., Reference Morgenstern, Kuerbis, Amrhein, Hail, Lynch and McKay2012) whereas the third found no 8-week difference in outcome between the directive and non-directive conditions (Morgenstern et al., Reference Morgenstern, Kuerbis, Houser, Levak, Amrhein, Shao and McKay2017).

Discussion

We have considered three potential methodological and theoretical critiques of MI in order to understand more of what we don’t know. The first is that MI has lacked conceptual consistency and stability. Definitions and descriptions of MI have indeed evolved across four decades as is common with technological innovations. It is also true that what is claimed to be MI in clinical trials and in practice has often been undefined and poorly described. A saving grace here is that there are well-developed MI fidelity measures that show improvement with training and do predict client outcomes. In the absence of such measures, it is difficult to know what has actually been delivered in research and practice.

A second critique is that MI has no theoretical moorings. This is also true in that MI was not derived deductively from a pre-existing theory but abductively from close examination of clinical practice to generate testable hypotheses. Various theoretical explanations have subsequently emerged for the observed processes and outcomes of MI, but the practice of MI has not been grounded in or limited to a particular theory of personality or psychotherapy. It is unclear whether and how this atheoretical nature of MI disadvantages research and practice. A good theoretical foundation would give the method a context that helps us understand when it should be used and how it can be taught in a pedagogical manner. The relevance of findings from MI research to other theories and methods might be clearer and promote new knowledge. However, in practice it is common to try things out in practice to see what works without an a priori theory.

A third potential critique is that MI is nothing more than general therapeutic skills that can be found in many different human services and theoretical orientations. Again, it is true that the relational components of MI do correspond closely to ‘non-specific’ but measurable skills that characterize more effective psychotherapists. It is surely not the case that all therapists are skillful in or practise these therapeutic attributes, and the extent to which they do can significantly affects their clients’ outcomes. There are also specific technical aspects of MI related to client language that do appear to improve outcomes above and beyond its person-centred relational components.

There are other critiques related to specific theories that we have not discussed in this article. For example, Mylvaganam (Reference Mylvaganam2009) criticizes how MI is given limited anchoring in ambivalence theory and cognitive dissonance theory (cf. Draycott and Dabbs, Reference Draycott and Dabbs1998). Similarly, there have been attempts to link MI with other theoretical frameworks such as self-determination theory (Markland et al., Reference Markland, Ryan, Tobin and Rollnick2005). Exploring such junctions may inform future research and developments of MI. Our aim in this article has been to summarize some of the main theoretical and methodological critiques of MI that have emerged to date and what they teach us about the prospects of the method.

Data availability statement

Not applicable to this article.

Acknowledgements

None.

Author contributions

Lars Forsberg: Conceptualization (equal), Formal analysis (equal), Investigation (equal), Methodology (equal), Writing - original draft (equal), Writing - review & editing (equal); Lisa Forsberg: Conceptualization (equal), Formal analysis (equal), Investigation (equal), Methodology (equal), Writing - original draft (equal), Writing - review & editing (equal); William R. Miller: Conceptualization (equal), Formal analysis (equal), Investigation (equal), Methodology (equal), Writing - original draft (equal), Writing - review & editing (equal).

Financial support

L.G.F. and W.R.M. received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors for this research. L.F. received funding from the Journal of Moral Education Trust, via the British Academy/Leverhulme Trust Small Research Grants scheme (award SRG2324\241695).

Competing interests

L.G.F. is one of the owners of MIC Lab, a company providing treatment integrity assessment and feedback on MI practice. L.F. has no competing interests. W.R.M. receives royalties from Guilford Press, Psychwire.com, and also from The Change Companies where he serves as a senior consultant.

Ethical standards

We have abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS. No ethical approvals were needed for this research.

References

Amador, X. (2022). I’m right, you’re wrong, now what? Break the impasse and get what you need. Vida Press.Google Scholar
Amrhein, P. C. (2004). How does motivational interviewing work? What client talk reveals. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 18, 323336.10.1891/jcop.18.4.323.64001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amrhein, P. C., Miller, W. R., Yahne, C. E., Palmer, M., & Fulcher, L. (2003). Client commitment language during motivational interviewing predicts drug use outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 862878.10.1037/0022-006X.71.5.862CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apodaca, T. R., & Longabaugh, R. (2009). Mechanisms of change in motivational interviewing: a review and preliminary evaluation of the evidence. Addiction, 104, 705715.10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02527.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, C., & Woods, K. (2017). Establishing theoretical stability and treatment integrity for motivational interviewing. Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapy, 45, 337350.10.1017/S1352465817000145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babor, T.F., & Del Boca, F.K. (eds) (2003). Treatment Matching in Alcoholism. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Björk, A. (2014). Stabilizing a fluid intervention: the development of motivational interviewing 1983–2013. Addiction Research and Theory, 22, 313324.10.3109/16066359.2013.845174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borrelli, B., Tooley, E. M., & Scott-Sheldon, L. A. (2015). Motivational interviewing for parent-child health interventions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatric Dentistry, 37, 254265.Google Scholar
Brown, J., & Miller, W. R. (1993). Impact of motivational interviewing on participation and outcome in residential alcoholism treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 7, 211218.10.1037/0893-164X.7.4.211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, B. L., Arkowitz, H., & Menchola, M. (2003). The efficacy of motivational interviewing: a meta-analysis of controlled clinical trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 843861.10.1037/0022-006X.71.5.843CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calomarde-Gómez, C., Jiménez-Fernández, B., Balcells-Oliveró, M., Gual, A., & López-Pelayo, H. (2021). Motivational interviewing for cannabis use disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. European Addiction Research, 27, 413427.10.1159/000515667CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, E. S. (2023). Working the Difference: Science, Spirit, and the Spread of Motivational Interviewing. University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226827612.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chike-Okoli, A., & Okoli, T. (2018). Investigating the effectiveness of motivational interviewing on the academic performance of senior secondary students in private schools in Niger State, Nigeria. International Journal of Innovative Research & Development, 7, 107114.Google Scholar
Clarke, P. B., Giordano, A. L., Cashwell, C. S., & Lewis, T. F. (2013). The straight path to healing: using motivational interviewing to address spiritual bypass. Journal of Counseling and Development, 91, 8794.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daugherty, M. D. (2003). A randomized trial of motivational interviewing with college students for academic success. PhD dissertation, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque.Google Scholar
de Almeida Neto, A. C. (2017). Understanding motivational interviewing: an evolutionary perspective. Evolutionary Psychological Science, 3, 379389.10.1007/s40806-017-0096-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeJonge, J. J. M., Schippers, G. M., & Schaap, C. P. D. R. (2005). The Motivational Interviewing Skill Code: reliability and a critical appraisal. Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33, 114.Google Scholar
DeVargas, E. C., & Stormshak, E. A. (2020). Motivational interviewing skills as predictors of change in emerging adult risk behavior. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 51, 1624.10.1037/pro0000270CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Draycott, S., & Dabbs, A. (1998) Cognitive dissonance 2: a theoretical grounding of motivational interviewing. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 355.10.1111/j.2044-8260.1998.tb01391.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, C., Darnell, D., Atkins, D. C., Hallgren, K. A., Imel, Z. E., Bumgardner, K., Owens, M., & Roy-Byrne, P. (2016). Within-provider variability in motivational interviewing integrity for three years after MI training: does time heal? Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 65, 7482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forrester, D., Wilkins, D., & Whittaker, C. (2021). Motivational Interviewing for Working with Children and Families: A Practical Guide for Early Intervention and Child Protection. Jessica Kingsley Publications.Google Scholar
Gaume, J., Bertholet, N., Faouzi, M., Gmel, G., & Daeppen, J. B. (2010). Counselor motivational interviewing skills and young adult change talk articulation during brief motivational interventions. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 39, 272281.10.1016/j.jsat.2010.06.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaume, J., Bertholet, N., Faouzi, M., Gmel, G., & Daeppen, J. B. (2013). Does change talk during brief motivational interventions with young men predict change in alcohol use? Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 44, 177185.10.1016/j.jsat.2012.04.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gendlin, E. T. (1961). Experiencing: a variable in the process of therapeutic change. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 15, 233245.10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.1961.15.2.233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glynn, L. H., & Moyers, T. B. (2010). Chasing change talk: the clinician’s role in evoking client language about change. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 39, 6570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, K. C., Reinke, W. M., Frey, A. J., & Shepard, S. A. (2021). Motivational Interviewing in Schools: Stretegies for Engaging Parents, Teachers, and Students. Springer.Google Scholar
Hettema, J., Steele, J., & Miller, W. R. (2005). Motivational interviewing. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 91111.10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143833CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohman, M. (2021). Motivational Interviewing in Social Work Practice (2nd edn). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hurlocker, M. C., Moyers, T. B., Hatch, M., Curran, G., McCrady, B., Venner, K. L., & Witkiewitz, K. (2023). Effectiveness and feasibility of a motivational interviewing intake (MII) intervention for increasing client engagement in outpatient addiction treatment: an effectiveness-implementation hybrid design protocol. Addiction Science & Clinical Practice, 18, 63.10.1186/s13722-023-00412-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imel, Z. E., Wampold, B. E., Miller, S. D., & Fleming, R. R. (2008). Distinctions without a difference: direct comparisons of psychotherapies for alcohol use disorders. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 22, 533543.10.1037/a0013171CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschenbaum, H. (2009). The Life and Work of Carl Rogers. American Counseling Association.Google Scholar
Kivlighan, D. M. Jr, & Holmes, S. E. (2004). The importance of therapeutic factors: a typology of therapeutic factors studies. In J. L. DeLucia-Waack, D. A. Gerrity, C. R. Calodner, & M. T. RIva (eds), Handbook of Group Counseling and Psychotherapy (pp. 2336). Sage.10.4135/9781452229683.n2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, P., Fulbrook, P., Somerset, S., & Schulz, P. (2017). Motivational interviewing to enhance treatment attendance in mental health settings: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing, 24, 699718.10.1111/jpm.12420CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindqvist, H., Forsberg, L., Enebrink, P., Andersson, G., & Rosendahl, I. (2017). Relational skills and client language predict outcome in smoking cessation treatment. Substance Use & Misuse, 52, 3342.10.1080/10826084.2016.1212892CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundahl, B., Moleni, T., Burke, B. L., Butters, R., Tollefson, D., Butler, C., & Rollnick, S. (2013). Motivational interviewing in medical care settings: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Patient Education and Counseling, 93, 157168.10.1016/j.pec.2013.07.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundahl, B. W., Kunz, C., Brownell, C., Tollefson, D., & Burke, B. L. (2010). A meta-analysis of motivational interviewing: twenty-five years of empirical studies. Research on Social Work Practice, 20, 137160.10.1177/1049731509347850CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madson, M. B., Loignon, A. C., & Lane, C. (2009). Training in motivational interviewing: a systematic review. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 36, 101109.10.1016/j.jsat.2008.05.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magill, M., Apodaca, T. R., Barnett, N. P., & Monti, P. M. (2010). The route to change: within-session predictors of change plan completion in a motivational interview. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 38, 299305.10.1016/j.jsat.2009.12.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magill, M., Apodaca, T. R., Borsari, B., Gaume, J., Hoadley, A., Gordon, R. E. F., Tonigan, J. S., & Moyers, T. (2018). A meta-analysis of motivational interviewing process: technical, relational, and conditional process models of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 86, 140157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magill, M., Kiluk, B. D., McCrady, B. S., Tonigan, J. S., & Longabaugh, R. (2015). Active ingredients of treatment and client mechanisms of change in behavioral treatments for alcohol use disorders: progress 10 years later. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 39, 18521862.10.1111/acer.12848CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markland, D., Ryan, R. M., Tobin, V., & Rollnick, S. (2005). Motivational interviewing and self-determination theory. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 811831.10.1521/jscp.2005.24.6.811CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, C., & Nielsen, A. S. (2020). Motivational Interviewing for Leaders in the Helping Professions. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R. (1983). Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 11, 147172.10.1017/S0141347300006583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R. (ed) (2004). Combined Behavioral Intervention manual: A Clinical Research Guide for Therapists Treating People with Alcohol Abuse and Dependence (Vol. 1). National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R. (2023). The evolution of motivational interviewing. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 51, 616632.10.1017/S1352465822000431CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., Benefield, R. G., & Tonigan, J. S. (1993). Enhancing motivation for change in problem drinking: a controlled comparison of two therapist styles. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 455461.10.1037/0022-006X.61.3.455CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., Forcehimes, A., O’Leary, M., & LaNoue, M. (2008). Spiritual direction in addiction treatment: two clinical trials. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 35, 434442.10.1016/j.jsat.2008.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., & Mount, K. A. (2001). A small study of training in motivational interviewing: does one workshop change clinician and client behavior? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 457471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Moyers, T. B. (2017). Motivational interviewing and the clinical science of Carl Rogers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 85, 757766.10.1037/ccp0000179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., & Moyers, T. B. (2021). Effective Psychotherapists: Clinical Skills That Improve Client Outcomes. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (1991). Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People to Change Addictive Behavior. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational Interviewing: Preparing People for Change. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2013). Motivational Interviewing: Helping People Change (3rd edn). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2014). The effectiveness and ineffectiveness of complex behavioral interventions: impact of treatment fidelity. Contemporary Clinical Trials, 37, 234241.10.1016/j.cct.2014.01.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2023). Motivational Interviewing: Helping People Change and Grow (4th edn). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Miller, W. R., & Rose, G. S. (2009). Toward a theory of motivational interviewing. American Psychologist, 64, 527537.10.1037/a0016830CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., Sovereign, R. G., & Krege, B. (1988). Motivational interviewing with problem drinkers: II. The Drinker’s Check-up as a preventive intervention. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 16, 251268.10.1017/S0141347300014129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, W. R., Taylor, C. A., & West, J. (1980). Focused versus broad-spectrum behavior therapy for problem drinkers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 590601.10.1037/0022-006X.48.5.590CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., Yahne, C. E., Moyers, T. B., Martinez, J., & Pirritano, M. (2004). A randomized trial of methods to help clinicians learn motivational interviewing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 10501062.10.1037/0022-006X.72.6.1050CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., Yahne, C. E., & Tonigan, J. S. (2003). Motivational interviewing in drug abuse services: a randomized trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 754763.10.1037/0022-006X.71.4.754CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, W. R., Zweben, A., DiClemente, C., & Rychtarik, R. (1992). Motivational Enhancement Therapy Manual: A Clinical Research Guide for Therapist Treating Individuals with Alcohol Abuse and Dependence (Vol. 2). National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, J., Kuerbis, A., Amrhein, P., Hail, L., Lynch, K., & McKay, J. R. (2012). Motivational interviewing: a pilot test of active ingredients and mechanisms of change. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 26, 859869.10.1037/a0029674CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgenstern, J., Kuerbis, A., Houser, J., Levak, S., Amrhein, P., Shao, S., & McKay, J. R. (2017). Dismantling motivational interviewing: effects on initiation of behavior change among problem drinkers seeking treatment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 31, 751762.10.1037/adb0000317CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, K., Beauchamp, M., Prothero, A., Joyce, L., Saunders, L., Spencer-Bowdage, S., Dancy, B., & Pedlar, C. (2015). The effectiveness of motivational interviewing for health behaviour change in primary care settings: a systematic review. Health Psychology Review, 9, 205223.10.1080/17437199.2014.882006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyers, T. B., Houck, J. M., Glynn, L. H., Hallgren, K. A., & Manual, J. K. (2017). A randomized controlled trial to influence client language in substance use disorder treatment. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 172, 4350.10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.11.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyers, T. B., Martin, T., Houck, J. M., Christopher, P. J., & Tonigan, J. S. (2009). From in-session behaviors to drinking outcomes: a causal chain for motivational interviewing. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 11131124.10.1037/a0017189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyers, T. B., Miller, W. R., & Hendrickson, S. M. L. (2005). How does motivational interviewing work? Therapist interpersonal skill predicts client involvement within motivational interviewing sessions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 590598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyers, T. B., Rowell, L. N., Manuel, J. K., Ernst, D., & Houck, J. M. (2016). The Motivational Interviewing Treatment Integrity code (MITI 4): rationale, preliminary reliability and validity. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 65, 3642.10.1016/j.jsat.2016.01.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mylvaganam, K. (2009). The role of ambivalence and cognitive dissonance in motivational interviewing for alcohol problems. D.Clin.Psychol thesis, University of Leeds.Google Scholar
Naar, S., Pennar, A. L., Wang, B., Brogan-Hartlieb, K., & Fortenberry, J. D. (2021). Tailored motivational interviewing (TMI): translating basic science in skills acquisition into a behavioral intervention to improve community health worker motivational interviewing competence for youth living with HIV. Health Psychology, 40, 920927.10.1037/hea0001071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Organ, J. N. (2021). Motivational interviewing: a tool for servant leadership. International Journal of Servant Leadership, 15, 209234.10.33972/ijsl.13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palacio, A., Garay, D., Langer, B., Taylor, J., Wood, B. A., & Tamariz, L. (2016). Motivational interviewing improves medication adherence: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 31, 929940.10.1007/s11606-016-3685-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1984). The Transtheoretical Approach: Crossing Traditional Boundaries of Therapy. Dow/Jones Irwin.Google Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and interpersonal relationships as developed in the client-centered framework. In Koch, S. (ed), Psychology: The Study of a Science. Vol. 3. Formulations of the Person and the Social Contexts (pp. 184256). McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Rogers, E. M. (2003). Diffusion of Innovations (5th edn). Free Press.Google Scholar
Rollnick, S., Kaplan, S. G., & Rutschman, R. (2016). Motivational Interviewing in Schools: Conversations to Improve Behavior and Learning. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rollnick, S., & Miller, W. R. (1995). What is motivational interviewing? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 325334.10.1017/S135246580001643XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sellman, J. D., Sullivan, P. F., Dore, G. M., Adamson, S. J., & MacEwan, I. (2001). A randomized controlled trial of motivational enhancement therapy (MET) for mild to moderate alcohol dependence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 62, 389396.10.15288/jsa.2001.62.389CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svensson, C., Wickström, H., Forsberg, L., Betnér, S., von Brömssen, C., Reyher, K. K., Bard, A. M., & Emanuelson, U. (2022). Dairy herd health management activities in relation to training of veterinarians in motivational interviewing. Preventive Veterinary Medicine, 204, 105679.10.1016/j.prevetmed.2022.105679CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanana, M., Hallgren, K. A., Imel, Z. E., Atkins, D. C., & Srikumar, V. (2016). A comparison of natural language processing methods for automated coding of motivational interviewing. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 65, 4350.10.1016/j.jsat.2016.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thevos, A. K., Quick, R. E., & Yanjuli, V. (2000). Motivational interviewing enhances the adoption of water disinfection practices in Zambia. Health Promotion International, 15, 207214.10.1093/heapro/15.3.207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truax, C. B., & Carkhuff, R. R. (1967). Toward Effective Counseling and Psychotherapy. Aldine.Google Scholar
Walthers, J., Janssen, T., Mastroleo, N. R., Hoadley, A., Barnett, N. P., Colby, S. M., & Magill, M. (2019). A sequential analysis of clinician skills and client change statements in a brief motivational intervention for young adult heavy drinking. Behavior Therapy, 50, 732742.10.1016/j.beth.2018.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wampold, B. E., & Brown, G. S. (2005). Estimating variability in outcomes attributable to therapists: a naturalistic study of outcomes in managed care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 914923.10.1037/0022-006X.73.5.914CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westra, H. A., Arkowitz, H., & Dozois, D. J. (2009). Adding a motivational interviewing pretreatment to cognitive behavioral therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: a preliminary randomized controlled trial. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 11061117.10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.07.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong-Anuchit, C., Chantamit-o-pas, C., Schneider, J. K., & Mills, A. C. (2019). Motivational interviewing-based compliance/adherence therapy interventions to improve psychiatric symptoms of people with severe mental illness: meta-analysis. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association, 25, 122133.10.1177/1078390318761790CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.