Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-xfwgj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-15T19:58:20.390Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Behavioural aspects of neonate survival

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 February 2018

A. B. Lawrence
Affiliation:
Scottish Agricultural College Edinburgh, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG
C. E. O'Connor
Affiliation:
Institute for Ecology and Resource Management, University of Edinburgh, West Mains Road, Edinburgh EH9 3JG
Get access

Abstract

The postnatal period is the most dangerous phase of life and survival depends on the neonate behaving appropriately from birth. Consequently, many aspects of neonate behaviour are often near to being fully formed at birth. However, experience will often be required to shape the expression of the behaviour and improve its effectiveness. The immediate problems facing the neonate will be related to the overall reproductive strategy of the species. In pigs, sibling rivalry will be critical in establishing and maintaining fidelity to a teat, and this early conflict is probably most important for survival. The teat order that succeeds this early rivalry is probably not, however, the result of competition over the most productive teats but because of certain intrinsic advantages of teat fidelity that maximize growth. In sheep the need to quickly follow and solicit sucking only from the dam makes learning to recognise the mother a priority. Recent results suggest that the ability of the neonate lamb to recognize its mother has been under-estimated and that failure of lambs to recognise their mothers can make a significant contribution to mortality rates. Survival in any species is, however, not simply a matter of appropriate neonate behaviour but may depend critically on the quality of the interaction between the mother and offspring. It is difficult to determine the end of the neonate period given the often protracted nature of weaning. As weaning progresses there is a shift to bonding with siblings and peers and to an extent these relationships take over some of the social functions of the mother-infant relationship such as avoidance of predators. Lastly, experiences gained during the neonate period can have long-term effects on behaviour such as diet selection, social preferences and responses to fear with important implications for farmed livestock.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © British Society of Animal Production 1992

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, G. 1960. Maternal behaviour in the Merino ewe. Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production 3: 105114.Google Scholar
Alexander, G. and Williams, D. 1964. Maternal facilitation of sucking drive in newborn lambs. Science 146: 665666.Google Scholar
Algers, B. and Jensen, P. 1985. Communication during suckling in the domestic pig. Effects of continuous noise. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 14: 4961.Google Scholar
Algers, B., Rojanasthien, S. and Uvnas-Moberg, K. 1990. The relationship between teat stimulation, oxytocin release and grunting rate in the sow during nursing. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 26: 267276.Google Scholar
Arnold, G., Wallace, S. R. and Maller, R. A. 1979. Some factors involved in natural weaning processes in sheep. Applied Animal Ethology 5: 4350.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. P. G. 1983. Genes, environment and the development of behaviour. In Animal Behaviour. Volume 3 (ed. Halliday, T. R. and Slater, P. J. B.), pp. 5281. Blackwell, Oxford.Google Scholar
Billing, A. E. and Vince, M. A. 1987a. Teat-seeking behaviour in newborn lambs. 1. Evidence for the influence of maternal skin temperature. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 18: 301313.Google Scholar
Billing, A. E. and Vince, M. A. 1987b. Teat-seeking behaviour in newborn lambs. 2. Evidence for the influence of the dam's surface textures and degree of surface textures and degree of surface yield. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 18: 315325.Google Scholar
Bolhuis, J. J. 1992. Mechanisms of avian imprinting: a review. Biological Reviews. In press.Google Scholar
Bronstein, P. M., Levine, M. J. and Marcus, M. 1975. A rat's first bite: the nongenetic, cross generational transfer of information. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 89: 295298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, J. L. 1975. The evolution of behaviour. W. W. Norton, New York.Google Scholar
Chapple, R. S. and Lynch, J. J. 1986. Behavioural factors modifying acceptance of supplementary foods by sheep. Research and Development in Agriculture 3: 113120.Google Scholar
Coe, C. L., Wiener, S. G., Rosenberg, L. T. and Levine, S. 1985. Endocrine and immune responses to separation and maternal loss in non-human primates. In The psychobiology of attachment and separation (ed. Reite, M. and Field, T.), pp. 163199. Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
Collias, N. E. 1956. The analysis of socialization in sheep and goats. Ecology 37: 228239.Google Scholar
De Passillé, A-M. B., Rushen, J. and Hartsock, T. G. 1988. Ontogeny of teat fidelity in pigs and its relation to competition at suckling. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 68: 325338.Google Scholar
Einon, D. F. and Morgan, M. J. 1976. Habituation of object contact in socially reared and isolated rats. Animal Behaviour 24: 415420.Google Scholar
Ellendorf, F., Forsling, M. L. and Poulain, D. A. 1982. The milk ejection reflex in the pig. Journal of Physiology 333: 577594.Google Scholar
English, P. R., Smith, W. J. and Maclean, A. 1977. The sow — improving her efficiency. Farming Press, Ipswich.Google Scholar
Evans, R. M. 1972. Development of an auditory discrimination in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Animal Behaviour 20: 7787.Google Scholar
Fagen, R. 1981. Animal play behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Ferchim, P. A., Bennett, E. L. and Rosenzweig, M. R. 1975. Direct contact with enriched environment is required to alter cerebral weights in rats, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 88: 360367.Google Scholar
Fraser, D. 1975. The ‘teat order’ of suckling pigs. 2. Fighting during suckling and the effects of clipping the eye teeth. Journal of Agricultural Science, Cambridge 84: 393399.Google Scholar
Fraser, D. 1980. A review of the behavioural mechanisms of milk ejection of the domestic pig. Applied Animal Ethology 6: 247255.Google Scholar
Fraser, D. 1984. The role of behaviour in swine production. Applied Animal Ethology 11: 317339.Google Scholar
Fraser, D. 1990. Behavioural perspectives on piglet survival. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility, suppl. 40, pp. 355370.Google Scholar
Fraser, D. and Jones, R. 1975. The ‘teat order’ of suckling pigs. 1. Relation to birth weight and subsequent growth. Journal of Agricultural Science, Cambridge 84: 387391.Google Scholar
Fraser, D. and Thompson, B. K. 1986. Variation in piglet weights: relationship to suckling behaviour, parity number and farrowing crate design. Canadian Journal of Animal Science 66: 3146.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. and Sherry, D. F. 1973. Mother's milk: a medium for the transmission of cues reflecting the flavour of mother's diet. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 83: 374378.Google Scholar
Geist, V. 1971. Mountain sheep. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
Geist, V. 1981. On the reproductive strategies in ungulates and some problems of adaptation. In Evolution today: proceedings from the second international congress on systematic and evolutionary biology (ed. Scudder, G. G. E. and Reveal, J. L.), pp. 111132. Carnegie University, Pittsburg.Google Scholar
Grubb, P. 1974. The rut and behaviour of Soay rams. In Island survivors: the ecology of the Soay sheep of St Kilda (ed. Jewell, P. A., Milner, C. and Morton-Boyd, J.), pp. 195241. Athlone Press, London.Google Scholar
Hall, W. G. and Rosenblatt, J. S. 1977. Suckling behaviour and intake control in the developing rat pup. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 91: 12321247.Google Scholar
Hall, W. G. and Williams, C. L. 1983. Suckling isn't feeding or is it? A search for developmental continuities. Advances in the Study of Behaviour 13: 219254.Google Scholar
Hartsock, T. G. and Graves, H. B. 1976. Neonatal behaviour and nutrition-related mortality in domestic swine. Journal of Animal Science 42: 235241.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. 1953. Heredity and environment in mammalian behaviour. British Journal of Animal Behaviour 1: 4347.Google Scholar
Hemsworth, P. H., Winfield, C. G. and Mullaney, P. D. 1976. A study of the development of the teat order in piglets. Applied Animal Ethology 2: 225233.Google Scholar
Hennessy, M. B. 1988. Both prevention of physical contact and removal of distal cues mediate Cortisol and vocalisation responses of Guinea pig pups to maternal separation in a novel environment. Physiology and Behaviour 43: 729733.Google Scholar
Hess, E. H. 1959. Two conditions limiting critical age for imprinting. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 52: 513518.Google Scholar
Hinch, G. N., Lynch, J. J., Elwin, R. L. and Green, G. C. 1990. Long-term associations between Merino-ewes and their offspring. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 27: 93103.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. 1970. Animal behaviour. McGraw-Hill, Tokyo.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. 1974. Biological bases of human social behaviour. McGraw-Hill, New York.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. 1973. Development of food recognition in young chicks. 2. Learned associations over long delays. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 83: 367373.Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. 1975. Development of food recognition in young chicks. 3. Discrimination. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 89: 95104.Google Scholar
Horn, G. 1990. Neural bases of recognition memory investigated through an analysis of imprinting. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B 329: 133142.Google Scholar
Jensen, P. 1986. Observations on the maternal behaviour of free-ranging domestic pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 16: 131142 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, P. 1988. Maternal behaviour and mother-young interactions during lactation in free-ranging domestic pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 20: 297308.Google Scholar
Jensen, P. and Redbo, I. 1987. Behaviour during nest leaving in free-ranging domestic pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 18: 355362.Google Scholar
Jeppeson, L. E. 1982. Teat-order in groups of piglets reared on an artificial sow. I. Formation of teat-order and influence of milk yield on teat preference. Applied Animal Ethology 8: 335345.Google Scholar
Jones, R. B. 1989. Fear in poultry. In The detection and control of fear in animals (ed. Gibson, T.), Proceedings of a British Veterinary Association Animal Welfare Foundation symposium, London, pp. 2840.Google Scholar
Joseph, J. R. and Gallagher, R. E. 1980. Gender and early environmental influences on activity, over-responsiveness and exploration. Developmental Psychobiology 29: 649675.Google Scholar
Key, C. and MacIver, R. M. 1980. The effects of maternal influences on sheep: breed differences in grazing, resting and courtship behaviour. Applied Animal Ethology 6: 3348.Google Scholar
Klopfer, P. and Klopfer, M. 1977. Compensatory responses of goat mothers to their impaired young. Animal Behaviour 25: 286291.Google Scholar
Lawrence, A. B. 1986. Consumer demand theory and the assessment of animal welfare. Animal Behaviour 35: 291293.Google Scholar
Lawrence, A. B. 1990. Mother-daughter and peer relationships of Scottish hill sheep. Animal Behaviour 39: 481486.Google Scholar
Lawrence, A. B. 1991. Mother daughter bonds in sheep. Animal Behaviour 42: 683685.Google Scholar
Lawrence, A. B. and Wood-Gush, D. G. M. 1988a. Influence of social behaviour on utilization of supplemental feedblocks by Scottish hill sheep. Animal Production 46: 203212.Google Scholar
Lawrence, A. B. and Wood-Gush, D. G. M. 1988b. Home range behaviour and social organization of Scottish Blackface sheep. Journal of Applied Ecology 25: 2540.Google Scholar
Lent, P. C. 1974. Mother-infant relationships in ungulates. In Behaviour of ungulates and its relation to management (ed. Geist, V. and Walthers, F.), pp. 1453. IUCN, Mordes, Switzerland.Google Scholar
Lobato, J. F. P., Pearce, G. R. and Beilharz, R. G. 1980. Effect of early familiarisation with dietary supplements on the subsequent ingestion of molasses-urea blocks by sheep. Applied Animal Ethology 6: 149161.Google Scholar
Lynch, J. J., Keogh, R. G., Elwin, R. L., Green, G. C. and Mottershead, B. E. 1983. Effects of early experience on the post-weaning acceptance of whole grain wheat by fine-woolled Merino lambs. Animal Production 36: 175183.Google Scholar
Lytle, L. D., Moorcroft, W. H. and Campbell, B. A. 1971. Ontogeny of amphetamine anorexia and insulin hyperphagia in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 77: 388393.Google Scholar
McBride, G. 1963. The ‘teat order’ and communication in young pigs. Animal Behaviour 11: 5356.Google Scholar
McBride, G., James, J. W. and Wyeth, G. S. F. 1965. Social behaviour of domestic animals. VII. Variation in weaning weight in pigs. Animal Production 7: 6774.Google Scholar
McFarland, D. 1985. Animal behaviour. Pitman, London.Google Scholar
Martin, P. 1984. The meaning of weaning. Animal behaviour 32: 12571259.Google Scholar
Meat and Livestock Commission. 1991. Quarterly report. Pig Improvement Services, MLC, Milton Keynes.Google Scholar
Morrill, J. L. and Dayton, A. D. 1977. Effect of feed flavour in milk and calf starter on feed consumption and growth. Journal of Dairy Science 61: 229232.Google Scholar
Newberry, R. C. and Wood-Gush, D. G. M. 1986. Social relationships of piglets in a semi-natural environment. Animal Behaviour 34: 13111318.Google Scholar
Nielsen, N. C., Christensen, K., Bille, N. and Larsen, J. L. 1974. Preweaning mortality in pigs. 1. Herd investigations. Nordic Veterinary Medicine 26: 137150.Google ScholarPubMed
Nowak, R. 1988. Relationship between neonatal mortality and mother recognition by 12 hour old Merino lambs. Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production 17: 449.Google Scholar
Nowak, R. 1990. Lamb's bleats: important for establishment of the mother-young bond? Behaviour 115: 1429.Google Scholar
Nowak, R. and Lindsay, D. R. 1990. Effect of genotype and litter size on discrimination of mothers by their twelve-hour-old lambs. Behaviour 115: 113.Google Scholar
Nowak, R., Poindron, P. and Putu, I. G. 1989. Development of mother discrimination by single and multiple newborn lambs. Developmental Psychobiology 22: 833845.Google Scholar
O'Connor, C. E. 1990. Mother-offspring relationships in Scottish Blackface sheep. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
O'Connor, C. E., Jay, N. P., Nicol, A. M. and Beatson, P. R. 1985. Ewe maternal behaviour score and lamb survival. Proceedings of the New Zealand Society of Animal Production 45: 159162.Google Scholar
O'Connor, C. E., and Lawrence, A. B. 1990. Relationships between lamb vigour and ewe behaviour on sucking success at parturition. Animal Production 50: 554555.Google Scholar
O'Connor, C. E., Lawrence, A. B. and Wood-Gush, D. G. M. 1989. Influence of parity and litter size on maternal behaviour in sheep at parturition. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 24: 86.Google Scholar
Oppenheim, R. W. 1982. Preformation and epigenesis in the origins of the nervous system and behaviour: issues, concepts and their history. In Perspectives in ethology. Volume 5. Ontogeny (ed. Bateson, P. P. G. and Klopfer, P. H.). Plenum Press, New York.Google Scholar
Poindron, P. and Le Neindre, P. 1980. Endocrine and sensory regulation of maternal behaviour in the ewe. Advances in the Study of Behaviour 11: 75119.Google Scholar
Poindron, P. and Schmidt, P. 1985. Distance recognition in ewes and lambs kept permanently indoors or at pasture. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 13: 267273.Google Scholar
Reite, M., Short, R., Kaufman, I. C., Stynes, A. J. and Pauley, J. D. 1978. Heart rate and body temperature in separated monkey infants. Biology of Psychiatry 13: 91105.Google Scholar
Renner, M. J. and Rosenzweig, M. R. 1986. Object interaction in juvenile rats: effects of different experiential histories. Journal of Comparative Psychology 100: 229239.Google Scholar
Rosillon-Warnier, A. and Paquay, R. 1984. Development and consequences of teat-order in piglets. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 12: 3758.Google Scholar
Rowell, T. E. 1991. Till death us do part: long lasting bonds between ewes and their daughters. Animal Behaviour 42. In press.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. 1980. The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm-calling behaviour: a preliminary report. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 54: 3746.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. 1986. Vocal development in vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour 34: 16401658.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. and Marler, P. 1980. Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a freeranging primate. Animal Behaviour 28: 10701094.Google Scholar
Shapiro, L. E. and Insel, T. R. 1990. Infant's response to social separation reflects adult differences in affiliative behaviour: a comparative developmental study in Prairie and Montane voles. Developmental Psychobiology 23: 375393.Google Scholar
Shillito-Walser, E. E., Hague, P. and Yeomans, M. 1983. Preferences for sibling or mother in Dalesbred and Jacob twin lambs. Applied Animal Ethology 9: 289297.Google Scholar
Shillito-Walser, E. E., Willandsen, S. and Hague, P. 1982. Maternal vocal recognition in lambs born to Jacob and Dalesbred ewes after embryo transplantation between breeds. Applied Animal Ethology 8: 479486.Google Scholar
Shillito-Walser, E. E. and Williams, T. 1986. Pairassociations in twin lambs before and after weaning. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15: 241245.Google Scholar
Slee, J. and Springbett, A. 1986. Early post-natal behaviour in lambs of ten breeds. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15: 229240.Google Scholar
Smith, F. V., van Toller, C. and Boyes, T. 1966. The ‘critical period’ in the attachment of lambs and ewes. Animal Behaviour 14: 120125.Google Scholar
Spitz, F. 1986. Current state of knowledge of wild boar. Pig News and Information 7: 171175.Google Scholar
Stanton, M. E., Gutierrez, Y. R. and Levine, S. 1988. Maternal deprivation potentiates pituitary-adrenal responses in infant rats. Behavioural Neurosciences 102: 692700.Google Scholar
Stevens, D., Alexander, G. and Lynch, J. J. 1982. Lamb mortality due to inadequate care of twins by Merino ewes. Applied Animal Ethology 8: 243252.Google Scholar
Vince, M. A. and Ward, T. M. 1984. The responsiveness of newly born Clun Forest lambs to odour sources in the ewe. Behaviour 89: 117127.Google Scholar
Vince, M. A., Ward, T. M. and Reader, M. 1984. Tactile stimulation and teat-seeking behaviour in newly born lambs. Animal Behaviour 32: 11791184.Google Scholar
Wakerley, J. B. and Drewett, R. F. 1975. The pattern of sucking in the infant rat during spontaneous milk ejection. Physiology and Behaviour 15: 227281.Google Scholar
Welch, A. R. and Baxter, M. R. 1986. Responses of newborn piglets to thermal and tactile properties of their environment. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 15: 203215.Google Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F. 1991. Animal boredom: do animals miss being alert and active?. In Applied animal behaviour: past, present and future (ed. Appleby, M. C., Horrell, R. I., Petherick, J. C. and Rutter, S. M.), pp. 120123. Universities Federation for Animal Welfare, Potters Bar, London.Google Scholar