Skip to main content
×
Home
    • Aa
    • Aa

When Instructional Guidance is Needed

  • Ouhao Chen (a1), Slava Kalyuga (a2) and John Sweller (a2)
Abstract

Studying worked examples providing problem solutions to learners usually leads to better test performance than solving the equivalent problems without guidance, demonstrating the worked-example effect. The generation effect occurs when learners who generate answers without guidance learn better than those who read answers that provide guidance. The contradiction between these results can be hypothesised to be due to differences in the element interactivity of the learning tasks. Primary school students in Year 6 participated in the experiment, which investigated the hypothesis by using geometry materials. A disordinal interaction was obtained between levels of guidance and levels of element interactivity. Higher levels of guidance facilitated learning using high element interactivity information, while lower levels of guidance facilitated learning for low element interactivity information. Cognitive load theory was used to explain these contrasting results. From an educational perspective, it was suggested that when determining levels of guidance, a consideration of element interactivity is essential.

  • View HTML
    • Send article to Kindle

      To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

      Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

      Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

      When Instructional Guidance is Needed
      Available formats
      ×
      Send article to Dropbox

      To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

      When Instructional Guidance is Needed
      Available formats
      ×
      Send article to Google Drive

      To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

      When Instructional Guidance is Needed
      Available formats
      ×
Copyright
Corresponding author
Address for correspondence: Ouhao Chen, School of Education, Southern Cross University, Military Road, East Lismore NSW 2480, Australia. E-mail: chenouhao_lucky@126.com
Linked references
Hide All

This list contains references from the content that can be linked to their source. For a full set of references and notes please see the PDF or HTML where available.

S.J. Atkinson, Derry , A. Renkl , & D. Wortham (2000). Learning from examples: Instructional principles from the worked examples research. Review of Educational Research, 70, 181214.

O. Chen , S. Kalyuga , & J. Sweller (2016a). Relations between the worked example and generation effects on immediate and delayed tests. Learning and Instruction, 45, 2030.

N. Cowan (2001). Metatheory of storage capacity limits. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 154176.

S. Kalyuga (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 509539.

S. Kalyuga (2011). Cognitive load theory: How many types of load does it really need? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 119.

J.D. Karpicke , & F.M. Zaromb (2010). Retrieval mode distinguishes the testing effect from the generation effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 227239.

W. Leahy , J. Hanham , & J. Sweller (2015). High element interactivity information during problem solving may lead to failure to obtain the testing effect. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 291304.

G.A. Miller (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 8197.

Z.F. Peynircioğlu (1989). The generation effect with pictures and nonsense figures. Acta Psychologica, 70, 153160.

J. Reisslein , R.K. Atkinson , P. Seeling , & M. Reisslein (2006). Encountering the expertise reversal effect with a computer-based environment on electrical circuit analysis. Learning and Instruction, 16, 92103.

A. Renkl (2014b). The worked-out examples principle in multimedia learning. In R.E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

A. Rourke , & J. Sweller (2009). The worked-example effect using ill-defined problems: Learning to recognise designers’ styles. Learning and Instruction, 19, 185199.

R. Schweickert , M.A. McDaniel , & G. Riegler (1994). Effects of generation on immediate memory span and delayed unexpected free recall. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 781804.

J. Sweller (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 123138.

J. Sweller , P. Ayres , & S. Kalyuga (2011). Cognitive load theory. New York: Springer.

J. Sweller , & G.A. Cooper (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 5989.

Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

The Educational and Developmental Psychologist
  • ISSN: 2059-0776
  • EISSN: 2059-0784
  • URL: /core/journals/educational-and-developmental-psychologist
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords:

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 14
Total number of PDF views: 211 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 532 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between 5th October 2016 - 23rd September 2017. This data will be updated every 24 hours.