Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T11:50:57.001Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Association between recognition and help-seeking preferences and stigma towards people with mental illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 December 2016

L. Picco*
Affiliation:
Research Division, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore
E. Abdin
Affiliation:
Research Division, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore
S. Pang
Affiliation:
Research Division, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore
J. A. Vaingankar
Affiliation:
Research Division, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore
A. Jeyagurunathan
Affiliation:
Research Division, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore
S. A. Chong
Affiliation:
Research Division, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore
M. Subramaniam
Affiliation:
Research Division, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore
*
*Address for correspondence: L. Picco, Research Division, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore. (E-mail: Louisa_Picco@imh.com.sg)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims.

The ability to recognise a mental illness has important implications as it can aid in timely and appropriate help-seeking, and ultimately improve outcomes for people with mental illness. This study aims to explore the association between recognition and help-seeking preferences and stigmatising attitudes, for alcohol abuse, dementia, depression, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and schizophrenia, using a vignette-based approach.

Methods.

This was a population-based, cross-sectional survey conducted among Singapore Residents (n = 3006) aged 18–65 years. All respondents were asked what they think is wrong with the person in the vignette and who they should seek help from. Respondents were also administered the Personal and Perceived sub scales of the Depression Stigma Scale and the Social Distance Scale. Weighted frequencies and percentages were calculated for categorical variables. A series of multiple logistic and linear regression models were performed separately by vignette to generate odd ratios and 95% confidence intervals for the relationship between help-seeking preference, and recognition and beta coefficients and 95% confidence intervals for the relationship between stigma and recognition.

Results.

Correct recognition was associated with less preference to seek help from family and friends for depression and schizophrenia. Recognition was also associated with increased odds of endorsing seeking help from a psychiatric hospital for dementia, depression and schizophrenia, while there was also an increased preference to seek help from a psychologist and psychiatrist for depression. Recognition was associated with less personal and perceived stigma for OCD and less personal stigma for schizophrenia, however, increased odds of social distancing for dementia.

Conclusion.

The ability to correctly recognise a mental illness was associated with less preference to seek help from informal sources, whilst increased preference to seek help from mental health professionals and services and less personal and perceived stigma. These findings re-emphasise the need to improve mental health literacy and reinforce the potential benefits recognition can have to individuals and the wider community in Singapore.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016

Introduction

Mental health literacy refers to ‘knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders, which aid their recognition, management or prevention’ (Jorm et al. Reference Jorm, Korten, Jacomb, Christensen, Rodgers and Pollitt1997). It comprises several components: (i) the ability to recognise specific disorders; (ii) knowledge and beliefs about risk factors and causes of mental disorders as well as awareness of professional help available; (iii) attitudes which facilitate recognition and appropriate help-seeking; and (iv) how to access mental health information (Jorm, Reference Jorm2000). The inability to correctly recognise mental disorders can result in inappropriate help-seeking and delays in treatment seeking (Jorm, Reference Jorm2000). Furthermore, studies have shown that delays in seeking appropriate treatment result in negative outcomes, due to longer duration of untreated illness, and consequently, poorer treatment outcomes (Marshall et al. Reference Marshall, Lewis, Lockwood, Drake, Jones and Croudace2005; Altamura et al. Reference Altamura, Dell'osso, D'Urso, Russo, Fumagalli and Mundo2008, Reference Altamura, Dell'Osso, Berlin, Buoli, Bassetti and Mundo2010; de Diego-Adeliño et al. Reference de Diego-Adeliño, Portella, Puigdemont, Pérez-Egea, Álvarez and Pérez2010).

Whilst there are several factors that may determine or influence help-seeking, recognition plays a key role in this process (Biddle et al. Reference Biddle, Donovan, Sharp and Gunnell2007; Gulliver et al. Reference Gulliver, Griffiths and Christensen2010). Despite this, there has been much debate about the positive and negative implications of using psychiatric terms by the public to label someone with a mental disorder (Angermeyer & Matschinger, Reference Angermeyer and Matschinger2005). With regards to the potentially harmful effects, it has been argued that labelling creates expectations of deviant behaviour (Wright et al. Reference Wright, Jorm, Harris and McGorry2007) and may increase stigma (Jorm & Griffiths, Reference Jorm and Griffiths2008). However, being able to identify or recognise the signs and symptoms of mental illness is linked to early help-seeking and can reduce the burden of disease associated with mental disorders (Wang et al. Reference Wang, Berglund, Olfson, Pincus, Wells and Kessler2005).

Several studies amongst young people and adult populations have explored the relationship between recognition or labelling and help-seeking preferences and intentions. Among Australian adolescents, research findings suggest that recognition of mental disorders in vignettes predicted a preference for recommending or intending to seek help from professional sources, whilst also being associated with less preference to seek help from informal sources such as family and friends (Wright et al. Reference Wright, Jorm and Mackinnon2012; Yap et al. Reference Yap, Reavley and Jorm2014). Similar findings have been found in adult populations (Angermeyer et al. Reference O'Keeffe, Turner, Foley, Lawlor, Kinsella, O'Callaghan and Clarke2009; Rusch et al. Reference Rusch, Evans-Lacko and Thornicroft2012); however, the majority of these studies have focused on depression, schizophrenia and related psychoses and social phobia. Hence less is known about other mental illnesses such as obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) or alcohol abuse.

Whilst recognition can influence and affect when and where help is sought, it is also related to stigma. The detrimental effects of stigma are widespread as it can act as a barrier to help-seeking and service use as well as achievement of age-appropriate functional goals (Corrigan et al. Reference Corrigan, Larson and Rusch2009; Rüsch et al. Reference Rüsch, Corrigan, Wassel, Michaels, Larson, Olschewski, Wilkniss and Batia2009; Clement et al. Reference Clement, Schauman, Graham, Maggioni, Evans-Lacko, Bezborodovs, Morgan, Rüsch, Brown and Thornicroft2015). There seems to be little consensus however, regarding the relationship between recognition and stigma. For example, mental health literacy interventions have been found to reduce stigma, with some studies finding those with greater knowledge of mental illness, were less likely to hold stigmatising attitudes (Brockington et al. Reference Brockington, Hall, Levings and Murphy1993; Thornton & Wahl, Reference Thornton and Wahl1996; Holmes et al. Reference Holmes, Corrigan, Williams, Conor and Kubiak1999; Kitchener & Jorm, Reference Kitchener and Jorm2006). Others however, have determined that having knowledge to be able to label someone as mentally ill is in fact associated with increased stigma (Martin et al. Reference Martin, Pescosolido and Tuch2000; Angermeyer & Matschinger, Reference Angermeyer and Matschinger2004; Peluso & Blay, Reference Peluso and Blay2009; Hengartner et al. Reference Hengartner, Loch, Lawson, Guarniero, Wang, Rössler and Gattaz2013).

Link and Phelan (Reference Link, Phelan, Scheid and Brown2010), however, argue that recognition or labelling has both positive and negative aspects and that whilst labelling a person as ‘mentally ill’ can be stigmatising, labelling the problem or the illness itself, can be beneficial, as it facilitates treatment and ultimately amelioration of symptoms. Given the ambiguity and lack of clarity surrounding recognition and stigma, there is a need to further examine mental health literacy and more specifically recognition and stigma together, as the negative stereotypes implicit in stigma involve distortions of knowledge and understanding (Holman, Reference Holman2015).

Very little is known about the relationships between recognition, help-seeking and stigma in non-Western settings, and more specifically in Singapore, a multi-ethnic city–state situated in South East Asia. Singapore has an urban population of approximately 5.5 million, comprising predominantly of three main ethnic groups: Chinese, Malays and Indians (Statistics Singapore, 2015). The recently completed Mind Matters Study was the first national mental health literacy study of its kind in Singapore, which found significant differences in terms of recognition across five mental disorders. The most well-recognised disorders were dementia (66.3%), alcohol abuse (57.1%) and depression (55.2%), while only 28.7 and 11.5% could recognise OCD and schizophrenia, respectively (Chong et al. Reference Chong, Abdin, Picco, Pang, Jeyagurunathan, Vaingankar, Kwok and Subramaniam2016). Previous mental health literacy interventions often follow the premise that recognition of a problem is the first step prior to appropriate help-seeking (Wright et al. Reference Wright, Jorm, Harris and McGorry2007). Given that recognition can aid in timely and appropriate help-seeking, and ultimately improve outcomes for people with mental illness, the relationship between recognition and other outcomes warrants further investigation. The aim of the current study was to explore the association between recognition and how this may predict help-seeking preferences and stigmatising attitudes, for alcohol abuse, dementia, depression, OCD and schizophrenia, using a vignette-based approach, among Singapore Residents aged 18–65 years. Based on existing literature, we hypothesised that being able to recognise mental disorders would be associated with: (i) greater preference to seek help from mental health professionals; (ii) less preference to seek help from informal sources such as family and friends; and (iii) would be associated with less personal stigma towards people with mental illness.

Methods

Participants and procedure

The Mind Matters Study was a comprehensive, population-based, cross-sectional mental health literacy survey conducted among Singapore Residents (Citizens and Permanent Residents) aged 18–65 years, who were living in Singapore between March 2014 and April 2015 (Chong et al. Reference Chong, Abdin, Picco, Pang, Jeyagurunathan, Vaingankar, Kwok and Subramaniam2016). The sample was derived using the sampling frame from an administrative database in Singapore that maintains data on age, gender, ethnicity and residential address of all those residing in Singapore. Residents living outside of Singapore, those who were unable to be contacted due to incomplete or incorrect addresses and those who were unable to complete the interview in one of the specified languages were excluded from the survey. Trained interviewers administered the survey in English, Mandarin, Malay or Tamil, based on the respondent's preference and on average the interview took 45 min to complete. The study was approved by the institutional ethics review board (National Healthcare Group Domain Specific Review Board). All respondents provided written informed consent and in the case of a respondent who was aged below 21 years, the written informed consent was also obtained from a legally acceptable representative, parent or guardian. A total of 3006 people completed the face-to-face interview, resulting in an overall response rate of 71%.

Survey interview

The interview was based on a vignette describing a person with one of five mental disorders; alcohol abuse, dementia, depression, OCD or schizophrenia, and followed a similar protocol to those used in previous research (Jorm et al. Reference Jorm, Korten, Jacomb, Christensen, Rodgers and Pollitt1997). Respondents were randomly allocated one of the five vignettes and were matched by gender and ethnicity, whereby they were read a vignette about someone of the same gender and ethnicity as them. All vignettes were written to reflect the diagnostic criteria for the five disorders according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV) and International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th Edition (ICD-10). The vignettes and related questions, along with the other measures included in the questionnaire, were pre-tested to ensure an adequate level of understanding among the local population and adaptations were made where needed, prior to the commencement of the survey.

The interview began by collecting socio-demographic information relating to the respondents including their age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, highest education-level attained, employment status and monthly income, using a structured questionnaire. Following this they were read one of the five vignettes and were asked the following open text questions: ‘What do you think the person in the vignette is suffering from?’ and ‘Who do you think the person in the vignette should seek help from?’ Respondents were also asked if they feel these symptoms were a cause for concern (yes/no) and whether they or someone close to them had experienced problems similar to those in the vignette.

Respondents were administered two stigma measures. The first was the Depression Stigma Scale which consists of two subscales, one which measures personal stigma and the other, perceived stigma (Griffiths et al. Reference Griffiths, Christensen, Jorm, Evans and Groves2004). The personal stigma subscale measures stigma in the respondent's own attitudes towards the person in the vignette, while the perceived stigma subscale assesses the respondent's beliefs about the attitudes of others towards the person in the vignette. Both subscales consist of nine items, however one item (‘I would not vote for a politician if I knew they had a mental illness’) was excluded. Responses to each item are measured on a five-point scale, ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’. Whilst the scale was originally intended to measure stigma related to depression, it can also be administered in relation to other disorders (Griffiths et al. Reference Griffiths, Nakane, Christensen, Yoshioka, Jorm and Nakane2006). Scores for each subscale were calculated by summing the respective item scores, with higher scores indicating greater personal or perceived stigma. The second stigma measure was the Social Distance Scale which measures self- reported willingness to have contact with, or the desire to socially distance oneself from the person described in the vignette (Link et al. Reference Link, Phelan, Bresnahan, Stueve and Pescosolido1999). The scale consisted of five items ranked on a four-point scale from definitely willing to definitely unwilling, where scores were calculated by summing item scores and higher scores indicated greater social distance.

Coding and content analysis of open-ended questions

Coding of the open text questions relating to recognition and recommended help-seeking sources were conducted by senior members of the study team. Regarding recognition, responses were coded as correct if the respondent was able to correctly label or name the specific condition. Where the response was a near approximation of the correct answer, two senior investigators (M.S. and S.A.C.) would come to a consensus on how that response should be coded (Chong et al. Reference Chong, Abdin, Picco, Pang, Jeyagurunathan, Vaingankar, Kwok and Subramaniam2016). Sources of help-seeking were broadly coded as follows: ‘talk to family or friends,’ ‘seek help from the Institute of Mental Health (IMH)’ (the only tertiary psychiatric hospital dedicated to providing mental health services in Singapore), ‘see a psychologist,’ ‘see a psychiatrist,’ ‘see a doctor or general practitioner (GP),’ and ‘see a counsellor or have counselling,’ (Picco et al. Reference Picco, Abdin, Chong, Pang, Vaingankar, Sagayadevan, Kwok and Subramaniam2016). Those responses endorsed by <3% of respondents were categorised as ‘other’, however given the heterogeneity of these responses, they were not included in this analysis.

Statistical analysis

All estimates were weighted to adjust for over sampling and post-stratified for age and ethnicity distributions between the survey sample and the Singapore resident population in the year 2012. Weighted frequencies and percentages were calculated for categorical variables. A series of multiple logistic and linear regression models were performed separately by vignette to generate odd ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals for the relationship between help-seeking preference (dependent variable) and recognition (main predictor variable) and beta coefficients and 95% confidence intervals for the relationship between stigma (dependent variable) and recognition (main predictor variable), after adjusting for age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, education, employment, income as well as exposure to mental health problems either in themselves and/or family or friends. The six most frequently reported help-seeking sources, which comprised seeking help from family and friends, IMH, psychologist, doctor or GP, counsellor and psychiatrist (Picco et al. Reference Picco, Abdin, Chong, Pang, Vaingankar, Sagayadevan, Kwok and Subramaniam2016), as well as three dimensions of stigma (personal stigma, perceived stigma and social distance) were included as dependent variables. In these regression analyses, all six help-seeking preferences were treated as binary dependent variables and the three dimensions of stigma were treated as continuous dependent variables. Standard errors (s.e.) and significance tests for survey data analysis procedures were estimated using the Taylor series’ linearisation method to adjust for the weighting. Multivariate significance was evaluated using Wald χ 2 tests based on design corrected coefficient variance–covariance matrices. Statistical significance was evaluated at the 0.05 level using two-sided tests. Data analysis was conducted using the SAS Version 9.3.

Results

The socio-demographic characteristics of the respondents are shown in Table 1. The sample comprised almost equal proportions of females (49.9%) and males (50.1%). The majority of the sample was of Chinese ethnicity (74.7%) and married (64%). 95% of people felt the symptoms described in the vignette were a cause for concern or worry, while 43.7% could identify the condition being described in the vignette.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample (n = 3006)

* Equivalent to up to 11 years of education.

Table 2 shows the association between recognition and help-seeking preferences by each vignette. It shows that recognition was associated with less preference to seek help from family and friends for depression and schizophrenia. Recognition was also associated with increased odds of endorsing seeking help from IMH (a tertiary psychiatric hospital in Singapore) for dementia, depression and schizophrenia while there was also an increased preference to seek help from a psychologist and psychiatrist for depression.

Table 2. Association between recognition and help-seeking preference by vignette

Reference = incorrect recognition.

* =Odds ratio (OR) was derived using multiple logistic regression analyses after adjusting for age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, education, employment, income as well as exposure to mental health problems either in themselves and/or family or friends.

Bold figures indicate a significant result at the p < 0.05 level.

Table 3 shows the association between recognition and the three types of stigma; personal stigma, perceived stigma and social distance. Findings revealed that recognition was associated with less personal and perceived stigma for OCD and less personal stigma for schizophrenia. Recognition was also associated with increased odds of social distancing for dementia.

Table 3. Association between recognition and stigma by vignette

Reference = incorrect recognition.

* =Beta coefficient (Beta) was derived using multiple linear regression analyses after adjusting for age, gender, ethnicity, marital status, education, employment, income as well as exposure to mental health problems either in themselves and/or family or friends.

Bold figures indicate a significant result at the p < 0.05 level.

Discussion

The findings from this study have highlighted some important associations between the ability to correctly recognise a mental disorder and help-seeking preferences and stigmatising attitudes. For depression and schizophrenia, recognition was associated with less preference to seek help from family and friends while higher odds of seeking help from professional sources such as a psychiatrist, psychologist or a psychiatric hospital, for depression. While recognition was associated with less personal and perceived stigma for OCD and less personal stigma for schizophrenia, it was related to greater social distancing for dementia.

As hypothesised, the odds of seeking help from informal sources such as friends and family were lower for those who could correctly recognise depression and schizophrenia. In a study among Australians aged 12–25 years, recognition was associated with less preference to seek help from friends (Wright et al. Reference Wright, Jorm and Mackinnon2012). Given that mental health literacy encompasses the ability to be able to recognise and identify mental disorders as well as having an awareness of professional help available (Jorm, Reference Jorm2000) it is not surprising recognition was associated with less preference to seek help from family and friends. Interestingly, among the same sample, the most commonly reported source of help for depression and schizophrenia was family and friends (Picco et al. Reference Picco, Abdin, Chong, Pang, Vaingankar, Sagayadevan, Kwok and Subramaniam2016) and therefore this suggests that the ability to recognise or label a disorder is influential in terms of help-seeking preferences. It is also important to note that the upper limit of the confidence interval for alcohol abuse, dementia and OCD was 1.06, 1.11 and 1.04, respectively (while the lower limits were all greater than zero), which does suggest that, although these findings are not significant, they are borderline and therefore it appears there is a similar trend across all five disorders, where correct recognition is associated with less preference to seek help from informal sources.

Recognition was associated with increased odds in seeking help from IMH for depression, dementia and schizophrenia. IMH is a 2000 bed acute tertiary psychiatric hospital, with outpatient clinics, which offers a comprehensive range of psychiatric, rehabilitative and counselling services for children, adolescents, adults and the elderly. Since its inception nearly 90 years ago, IMH has grown to not only provide clinical services, but also community-based services and support services for patients and their caregivers. It is thus well known both as an Institution that provides care for those with psychiatric illness and for the availability of trained mental health professionals and a wide range of services in Singapore. Therefore it is logical that those who recognised the conditions correctly were able to recommend seeking services from this Institution.

For depression, the preference to seek help from mental health professionals such as psychologists or psychiatrists was strongly associated with recognition. This finding is based on open text responses where respondents indicated who or where they would recommend the person in the vignette should seek help from. In order to articulate and name specific professionals, this requires a level of knowledge and understanding and therefore it is not surprising to find those who can recognise depression are also more likely to suggest help-seeking from mental health professionals. Beliefs about mental illness aetiology may also be an additional influencing factor relating to help-seeking preferences, whereby despite correctly recognising the disorder based on the vignette, respondents had differing beliefs about the causes of these mental illnesses and accordingly this also influenced their help-seeking preferences (Rusch et al. Reference Rusch, Evans-Lacko and Thornicroft2012; Schomerus et al. Reference Schomerus, Schwahn, Holzinger, Corrigan, Grabe, Carta and Angermeyer2012). Aetiology beliefs could also be culturally influenced (Chen & Mak, Reference Chen and Mak2008) and therefore this gives merit to further investigate the impact of such beliefs on help-seeking preferences.

Recognition appears to be one of the influencing factors relating to help-seeking preferences; correct recognition was associated with less odds in recommending help-seeking from informal sources, whilst higher odds in recommending help-seeking from mental health professionals and services such as psychiatrists, psychologists and IMH. These findings are important as they suggest that those who can correctly identify the signs and symptoms for specific mental disorders are more likely to recommend help-seeking from mental health professionals. Despite this, it is also important to acknowledge that the relationship between mental health literacy and help-seeking is complex and multi-factorial and therefore, help-seeking preferences or behaviours may not solely be influenced by mental health literacy or the ability to recognise signs and symptoms, but additional factors such as aetiology beliefs, cultural influences, illness severity or stigma.

Mental health literacy studies in various countries have shown that failure to recognise mental disorders is not uncommon (Jorm et al. Reference Jorm, Korten, Jacomb, Christensen, Rodgers and Pollitt1997; Wong & He, Reference Wong and He2011; Chong et al. Reference Chong, Abdin, Picco, Pang, Jeyagurunathan, Vaingankar, Kwok and Subramaniam2016) although, recognition has improved over time, particularly for schizophrenia (Jorm et al. Reference Jorm, Barney, Christensen, Highet, Kelly and Kitchener2006b ; Angermeyer et al. Reference Angermeyer, Holzinger and Matschinger2009; Reavley & Jorm, Reference Reavley and Jorm2011). Despite this, social rejection and stigmatising attitudes towards people with mental illness has remained fairly stagnant over the past 20 years. The systematic review and meta-analysis by Schomerus et al. (Reference Schomerus, Schwahn, Holzinger, Corrigan, Grabe, Carta and Angermeyer2012) found that whilst mental health literacy has improved, negative attitudes towards people with a mental illness have not and even more concerning was that attitudes towards schizophrenia have in fact worsened over time.

Contrary to this, we found those who were able to identify OCD and schizophrenia displayed less personal stigma, as well as less perceived stigma towards OCD. It is possible that recognition or greater mental health literacy in terms of awareness and understanding, would therefore result in people not only being more aware of the available treatment options, but also the improved outcomes that are associated with treatment seeking, causing people to hold less stigmatising views. A previous study among the same population found that recognition was associated with higher levels of education (Chong et al. Reference Chong, Abdin, Picco, Pang, Jeyagurunathan, Vaingankar, Kwok and Subramaniam2016), while research has shown that people with increased knowledge about mental illness are less likely to endorse stigmatising attitudes (Holmes et al. Reference Holmes, Corrigan, Williams, Conor and Kubiak1999; Subramaniam et al. Reference Subramaniam, Abdin, Picco, Pang, Shafie, Vaingankar, Kwok, Verma and Chong2016) and this may further explain this finding.

With regards to social distance, contrasting results were found; recognition was associated with greater social distancing towards dementia. Social distance was assessed by asking the respondent how willing they would be to spend an evening, make friends, and work closely with the person in the vignette as well as have this person move next door and have them marry into their family. Given the person in the dementia vignette was aged 75, it is not surprising that the majority of people were unwilling to work with this person and have them marry into their family. Upon further analysis we actually found that if these items were removed, social distancing was no longer significantly associated with correct recognition and this may therefore explain why we observed greater social distancing towards dementia. To date, the majority of research surrounding recognition and social distancing has related to schizophrenia or depression, and results have been mixed (Jorm & Griffiths, Reference Jorm and Griffiths2008; Angermeyer et al. Reference Angermeyer, Holzinger and Matschinger2009). Further research exploring the impacts of recognition and stigma on other mental disorders is therefore needed.

Our findings should be viewed in light of the following limitations. The cross sectional nature of the study precludes any causal inferences and therefore associations could be bidirectional. Respondents were asked to identify what the person in the vignette was suffering from and recommend where they should seek help from; however, recognition of symptoms and help-seeking intentions may not necessarily equate to help-seeking behaviour (Jorm et al. Reference Jorm, Medway, Christensen, Korten, Jacomb and Rodgers2000; Barney et al. Reference Barney, Griffiths, Jorm and Christensen2006). Thus questionnaires that examine intended behaviours should be considered in future surveys. It is possible that respondents may have provided socially desirable responses rather than their preferred responses. Finally, whilst a representative sample was used, non-responders may have differing views, beliefs and knowledge relating to mental disorders.

In summary, findings from the study suggest that recognition was associated with less preference to seek help from informal sources such as family and friends and increased preference to seek help from mental health professionals and services including psychiatrists, psychologists and IMH. Given that less than half of the general population could recognise mental disorders based on vignettes (Chong et al. Reference Chong, Abdin, Picco, Pang, Jeyagurunathan, Vaingankar, Kwok and Subramaniam2016), this re-emphasises the need to improve mental health literacy amongst the local population in Singapore. Furthermore, given that recognition was associated with less personal and perceived stigma, this reinforces the potential benefits recognition can have to individuals and the wider community. It has been shown that even small increases in knowledge can have a significant effect on overall attitudes and behaviour (Wolff et al. Reference Wolff, Pathare, Craig and Leff1996), and therefore educating the public about mental illness, the signs and symptoms and the importance of appropriate and timely help-seeking is crucial.

Moving forward, recognition of mental disorders needs to be a focus of mental health community awareness initiatives designed to facilitate help-seeking (Dumesnil & Verger, Reference Dumesnil and Verger2009), especially given that recognition is generally poorer when compared with Western countries (Jorm et al. Reference Jorm, Christensen and Griffiths2006a ; Angermeyer et al. Reference Angermeyer, Holzinger and Matschinger2009; O'Keeffe et al. Reference O'Keeffe, Turner, Foley, Lawlor, Kinsella, O'Callaghan and Clarke2016). Going beyond this, as knowledge is not the only influencing factor relating to help-seeking, interventions could also address issues relating to stigma and implications of delaying help-seeking (Klineberg et al. Reference Klineberg, Biddle, Donovan and Gunnell2011). Mental health literacy interventions have been shown to have an impact on reducing social distance and stigma (Christensen et al. Reference Christensen, Griffiths and Jorm2004; Kitchener & Jorm, Reference Kitchener and Jorm2006). Given the extent of stigma towards those with mental illness (Subramaniam et al. Reference Subramaniam, Abdin, Picco, Pang, Shafie, Vaingankar, Kwok, Verma and Chong2016), and the lack of such interventions in the local context, this further emphasises the need for initiatives to address mental health literacy and stigma at the population level, in the future.

Given the complex inter-related relationship between recognition, beliefs, help-seeking preferences and stigmatising attitudes, there is a need for more in-depth qualitative studies to delve further into the subjective and inter-personal processes of these intertwined constructs. Furthermore, whilst changing or improving knowledge and beliefs about mental illness can influence behaviour, little is known about whether behaviour changes are actually made and moving forward, there is a need to track and monitor help-seeking behaviour over time.

Acknowledgement

We would like to acknowledge Professor AF Jorm for sharing the relevant vignettes and questionnaire.

Financial Support

This work was supported by the Ministry of Health, Health Services Research Competitive Research Grant (HSRG/0036/2013).

Conflict of Interest

None.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Availability of Data and Materials

Data are not available for online access; however, readers who wish to gain access to the data can write to the senior author Dr Mythily Subramaniam at with their requests. Access can be granted subject to the Institutional Review Board (IRB) and the research collaborative agreement guidelines. This is a requirement mandated for this research study by our IRB and funders.

References

Altamura, AC, Dell'osso, B, D'Urso, N, Russo, M, Fumagalli, S, Mundo, E (2008). Duration of untreated illness as a predictor of treatment response and clinical course in generalized anxiety disorder. CNS Spectrums 13, 415422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altamura, AC, Dell'Osso, B, Berlin, H, Buoli, M, Bassetti, R, Mundo, E (2010). Duration of untreated illness and suicide in bipolar disorder: a naturalistic study. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 260, 385391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, MC, Matschinger, H (2004). The stereotype of schizophrenia and its impact on discrimination against people with schizophrenia: results from a representative survey in Germany. Schizophrenia Bulletin 30, 10491061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, MC, Matschinger, H (2005). Labeling-stereotype-discrimination. An investigation of the stigma process. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 40, 391395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, MC, Holzinger, A, Matschinger, H (2009). Mental health literacy and attitude towards people with mental illness: a trend analysis based on population surveys in the eastern part of Germany. European Psychiatry 24, 225232.Google Scholar
Barney, LJ, Griffiths, KM, Jorm, AF, Christensen, H (2006). Stigma about depression and its impact on help-seeking intentions. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 40, 5154.Google Scholar
Biddle, L, Donovan, J, Sharp, D, Gunnell, D (2007). Explaining non-help-seeking amongst young adults with mental distress: a dynamic interpretive model of illness behaviour. Sociology of Health and Illness 29, 9831002.Google Scholar
Brockington, IF, Hall, P, Levings, J, Murphy, C (1993). The community's tolerance of the mentally ill. British Journal of Psychiatry 162, 9399.Google Scholar
Chen, SX, Mak, WWS (2008). Seeking professional help: etiology beliefs about mental illness across cultures. Journal of Counseling Psychology 55, 442450.Google Scholar
Chong, SA, Abdin, E, Picco, L, Pang, S, Jeyagurunathan, A, Vaingankar, JA, Kwok, KW, Subramaniam, M (2016). Mental Health Literacy among a multiracial population in Southeast Asia. BMC Psychiatry 16, 121.Google Scholar
Christensen, H, Griffiths, KM, Jorm, AF (2004). Delivering interventions for depression by using the Internet: randomised controlled trial. British Medical Journal 328, 265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clement, S, Schauman, O, Graham, T, Maggioni, F, Evans-Lacko, S, Bezborodovs, N, Morgan, C, Rüsch, N, Brown, JS, Thornicroft, G (2015). What is the impact of mental health-related stigma on help-seeking? A systematic review of quantitative and qualitative studies. Psychological Medicine 45, 1127.Google Scholar
Corrigan, PW, Larson, JE, Rusch, N (2009). Self-stigma and the “why try” effect: impact on life goals and evidence based practices. World Psychiatry 8, 7581.Google Scholar
de Diego-Adeliño, J, Portella, MJ, Puigdemont, D, Pérez-Egea, R, Álvarez, E, Pérez, V (2010). A short duration of untreated illness (DUI) improves response outcomes in first-depressive episodes. Journal of Affective Disorders 120, 221225.Google Scholar
Dumesnil, H, Verger, P (2009). Public awareness campaigns about depression and suicide: a review. Psychiatric Services 60, 1203e1213.Google Scholar
Griffiths, KM, Christensen, H, Jorm, AF, Evans, K, Groves, C (2004). Effect of web-based depression literacy and cognitive-behavioural therapy interventions on stigmatising attitudes to depression: randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry 185, 342349.Google Scholar
Griffiths, KM, Nakane, Y, Christensen, H, Yoshioka, K, Jorm, AF, Nakane, H (2006). Stigma in response to mental disorders: a comparison of Australia and Japan. BMC Psychiatry 6, 21.Google Scholar
Gulliver, A, Griffiths, K, Christensen, H (2010). Perceived barriers and facilitators to mental health help-seeking in young people: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 10.Google Scholar
Hengartner, M, Loch, A, Lawson, F, Guarniero, FB, Wang, YP, Rössler, W, Gattaz, WF (2013). Public stigmatization of different mental disorders: a comprehensive attitude survey. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 22, 269274.Google Scholar
Holman, D (2015). Exploring the relationship between social class, mental illness stigma and mental health literacy using British national survey data. Health 19, 413429.Google Scholar
Holmes, P, Corrigan, PW, Williams, P, Conor, J, Kubiak, MA (1999). Changing attitudes about schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 25, 447456.Google Scholar
Jorm, AF (2000). Mental Health Literacy: public knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders. British Journal of Psychiatry 177, 396401.Google Scholar
Jorm, AF, Griffiths, KM (2008). The public's stigmatizing attitudes towards people with mental disorders: how important are biomedical conceptualizations? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 118, 315321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, AF, Korten, AE, Jacomb, PA, Christensen, H, Rodgers, B, Pollitt, P (1997). ‘Mental health literacy’: a survey of the public's ability to recognise mental disorders and their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. Medical Journal of Australia 166, 182186.Google Scholar
Jorm, AF, Medway, J, Christensen, H, Korten, AE, Jacomb, PA, Rodgers, B (2000). Public beliefs about the helpfulness of interventions for depression: effects on actions taken when experiencing anxiety and depression symptoms. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 34, 619626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, AF, Christensen, H, Griffiths, KM (2006 a). The publics ability to recognize mental disorders and their beliefs about treatment: changes in Australia over 8 years. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 40, 3641.Google Scholar
Jorm, AF, Barney, LJ, Christensen, H, Highet, NJ, Kelly, CM, Kitchener, BA (2006 b). Research on mental health literacy: what we know and what we still need to know. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 40, 35.Google Scholar
Kitchener, BA, Jorm, AF (2006). Mental health first aid training: review of evaluation studies. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 40, 68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klineberg, E, Biddle, L, Donovan, J, Gunnell, D (2011). Symptom recognition and help seeking for depression in young adults: a vignette study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 46, 495505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Link, BG, Phelan, JC (2010). Labeling and stigma. In A Handbook for the Study of Mental Health: Social Contexts, Theories and Systems (ed. Scheid, TL and Brown, TN), pp. 571587. Cambridge University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Link, BG, Phelan, JC, Bresnahan, M, Stueve, A, Pescosolido, BA (1999). Public conceptions of mental illness: labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance. American Journal of Public Health 89, 13281333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, M, Lewis, S, Lockwood, A, Drake, R, Jones, P, Croudace, T (2005). Association between duration of untreated psychosis and outcome in cohorts of first-episode patients: a systematic review. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 975983.Google Scholar
Martin, JK, Pescosolido, BA, Tuch, SA (2000). Of fear and loathing: the role of ‘disturbing behavior,’ labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Health Social Behaviour 41, 208223.Google Scholar
O'Keeffe, D, Turner, N, Foley, S, Lawlor, E, Kinsella, A, O'Callaghan, E, Clarke, M (2016). The relationship between mental health literacy regarding schizophrenia and psychiatric stigma in the Republic of Ireland. Journal of Mental Health 25, 100108.Google Scholar
Peluso, ETP, Blay, SL (2009). Public stigma in relation to individuals with depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 115, 201206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picco, L, Abdin, E, Chong, SA, Pang, S, Vaingankar, JA, Sagayadevan, V, Kwok, KW, Subramaniam, M (2016). Beliefs about help-seeking for mental disorders: findings from a Singapore mental health literacy study. Psychiatric Services 67, 12461253.Google Scholar
Reavley, NJ, Jorm, AF (2011). Recognition of mental disorders and beliefs about treatment and outcome: findings from an Australian National Survey of Mental Health Literacy and Stigma. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 45, 947956.Google Scholar
Rüsch, N, Corrigan, PW, Wassel, A, Michaels, P, Larson, JE, Olschewski, M, Wilkniss, S, Batia, K (2009). Self-stigma, group identification, perceived legitimacy of discrimination and mental health service use. British Journal of Psychiatry 195, 551552.Google Scholar
Rusch, N, Evans-Lacko, S, Thornicroft, G (2012). What is a mental illness? Public views and their effects on attitudes and disclosure. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 46, 641650.Google Scholar
Schomerus, G, Schwahn, C, Holzinger, A, Corrigan, PW, Grabe, HJ, Carta, MG, Angermeyer, MC (2012). Evolution of public attitudes about mental illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 125, 440452.Google Scholar
Statistics Singapore (2015). Population and population structure. Retrieved 28 April 2016 from http://www.singstat.gov.sg/statistics/browse-by-theme/population-and-population-structure.Google Scholar
Subramaniam, M, Abdin, E, Picco, L, Pang, S, Shafie, S, Vaingankar, JA, Kwok, KW, Verma, K, Chong, SA (2016). Stigma towards people with mental disorders and its components – a perspective from multi-ethnic Singapore. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 28, 112.Google Scholar
Thornton, J, Wahl, OF (1996). Impact of a newspaper article on attitudes toward mental illness. Journal of Community Psychology 24, 1725.Google Scholar
Wang, PS, Berglund, P, Olfson, M, Pincus, HA, Wells, KB, Kessler, RC (2005). Failure and delay in initial treatment contact after first onset of mental disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Achieves of General Psychiatry 62, 603613.Google Scholar
Wolff, G, Pathare, S, Craig, T, Leff, J (1996). Public education for community care: a new approach. British Journal of Psychiatry 168, 441447.Google Scholar
Wong, DEF, He, X (2011). Schizophrenia literacy among Chinese in Shanghai, China: a comparison with Chinese-speaking Australians in Melbourne and Chinese in Hong Kong. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 45, 524531.Google Scholar
Wright, A, Jorm, AF, Harris, MG, McGorry, PD (2007). What's in a name? Is accurate recognition and labelling of mental disorders by young people associated with better help-seeking and treatment preferences? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 42, 244250.Google Scholar
Wright, A, Jorm, AF, Mackinnon, AJ (2012). Labels used by young people to describe mental disorders: which ones predict effective help-seeking choices? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 47, 917926.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yap, MBH, Reavley, NJ, Jorm, AF (2014). The associations between psychiatric label use and young people's help-seeking preferences: results from an Australian national survey. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 23, 5159.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the sample (n = 3006)

Figure 1

Table 2. Association between recognition and help-seeking preference by vignette

Figure 2

Table 3. Association between recognition and stigma by vignette