Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-684899dbb8-p6h7k Total loading time: 0.29 Render date: 2022-05-28T00:21:10.192Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "useRatesEcommerce": false, "useNewApi": true }

The production effect in memory: a prominent mnemonic in children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2014

MICHAL ICHT*
Affiliation:
Ariel University
YANIV MAMA
Affiliation:
Ariel University
*
*Address for Correspondence: Michal Icht, Department of Communication Disorders, Ariel University 40700, ISRAEL. tel: +972-3-9765755; fax: +972-3-9765743; e-mail: lmichal@ariel.ac.il

Abstract

The ‘Production Effect’ (PE) refers to a memory advantage for items studied aloud over items studied silently. Thus, vocalizing may serve as a mnemonic that can be used to assist learners in improving their memory for new concepts. Although many other types of mnemonic have been suggested in the literature, the PE seems especially appropriate for young children, as it does not involve literacy skills. The present study is a first investigation of the PE in children. In two experiments we tested five-year-olds in a PE paradigm using pictures of objects as stimuli. In Experiment 1, pictures of familiar objects were presented to be remembered, and in Experiment 2 we used pictures of unfamiliar objects (evaluating new vocabulary acquisition). In both experiments we showed a memory advantage for vocally produced words (‘look and say’) over other types of learning (‘look’, ‘look and listen’), suggesting the PE as a prominent memory and learning tool.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E. & Pickering, S. J. (2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable? Child Development 77, 1698–716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, R. C. & Nagy, W. E. (1992). The vocabulary conundrum. American Educator, 16(4) 1418, 44–7.Google Scholar
Aureli, D. (2012). The keyword method: a study of vocabulary acquisition in fifth grade. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Rowan University.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N. & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14, 575–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bat-El, O. (2009). Harmonic domains and synchronization in typically and atypically developing Hebrew-speaking children. Language Sciences 31, 117–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, R. H. (1979). Memory, acquisition, and category clustering in learning-disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 27, 365–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G. & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life: robust vocabulary instruction. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Biemiller, A. & Slonim, N. (2001). Estimating root word vocabulary growth in normative and advantaged populations: evidence for a common sequence of vocabulary acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology 93(3), 498520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biran, M. & Friedmann, N. (2005). From phonological paraphasias to the structure of the phonological output lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 20, 589616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodner, G. E., Taikh, A. & Fawcett, J. M. (2014). Assessing the costs and benefits of production in recognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21, 149–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourne, L. E., Dominowski, R. L., Loftus, E. F. & Healy, A. F. (1986). Cognitive processes, 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Bowman, B., Donovan, S. & Burns, M. S. (2000). Eager to learn: educating our preschoolers. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Bowman, M. (1981). The effects of story structure questioning upon reading comprehension. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Education Research Association, Los Angeles, CA.Google Scholar
Camarata, S. M., Nelson, K. E. & Camarata, M. N. (1994). Comparison of conversational-recasting and imitative procedures for training grammatical structures in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 37(6), 1414–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castel, A. D., Rhodes, M. G. & Friedman, M. C. (2013). Predicting memory benefits in the production effect: the use and misuse of self-generated distinctive cues when making judgments of learning. Memory and Cognition 41, 2835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coady, J. (1997). L2 vocabulary acquisition through extensive reading. In Coady, J. & Huckin, T. (eds), Second language vocabulary acquisition (pp. 225237). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Conroy, P. & Collins, P. (2012) Mnemonic devices for braille instruction, Insight 5(1), 4752.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. & Gathercole, S. E. (1987). Modality and longterm memory. Journal of Memory & Language 26, 341–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coyne, M. D., Simmons, D. C., Kame'enui, E. J. & Stoolmiller, M. (2004). Teaching vocabulary during shared storybook readings: an examination of differential effects. Exceptionality 12(3), 145–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunst, C. J., Herter, S. & Shields, H. (2000). Interest-based natural learning opportunities. Young Exceptional Children Monograph Series No. 2: Natural Environments and Inclusion 3748.Google Scholar
Fawcett, J. M. (2013). The production effect benefits performance in between-subject designs: a meta-analysis. Acta Psycologica 142, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fawcett, J. M., Quinlan, C. K. & Taylor, T. L. (2012). Interplay of the production and picture superiority effects: a signal detection analysis. Memory 20(7), 655–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Beach, D. R. & Chinsky, J. M. (1966). Spontaneous verbal rehearsal in a memory task as a function of age. Child Development 37, 283–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flood, J. (1977). Parental styles in reading episodes with young children. Reading Teacher 30, 864–7.Google Scholar
Forrin, N. D., MacLeod, C. M. & Ozubko, J. D. (2012). Widening the boundaries of the production effect. Memory and Cognition 40(7), 1046–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frost, R. & Plaut, D. C. (2005). The word-frequency database for printed Hebrew, online <http://word-freq.mscc.huji.ac.il/index.htm> (last accessed 3 June 2013).+(last+accessed+3+June+2013).>Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. (1998). The development of memory. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 39(1), 327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gathercole, S. E. & Conway, M. A. (1988). Exploring long-term modality effects: vocalization leads to best retention. Memory & Cognition 16(2), 110–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, C. & Braun, C. (1982). Story schemata: metatextual aid to reading and writing. In Niles, J. & Harris, L. (eds), New inquiries in reading research and instruction (Thirty-first Yearbook of the National Reading Conference) (pp. 262268). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.Google Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, R. T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1980). The functions and uses of literacy. Journal of Communication 30, 123–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hochberg, J. & Brooks, V. (1962). Pictorial recognition as an unlearned ability in a study of one child's performance. American Journal of Psychology 75, 624–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, R. H. & Edwards, R. E. (1972). Pronunciation effects in recognition memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 11, 534–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hourihan, K. L. & MacLeod, C. M. (2008). Directed forgetting meets the production effect: distinctive processing is resistant to intentional forgetting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale 62, 242–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, C. & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development of short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 38, 241–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Icht, M., Mama, Y. & Algom, D. (2014) The production effect in memory: multiple species of distinctiveness. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 886.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinheksel, K. A. & Summy, S. E. (2003). Enhancing student learning and social behavior through mnemonic strategies. Teaching Exceptional Children 36(2), 30–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B., Schwartz, R. G., Folger, M. K., Newhoff, M. & Wilcox, M. J. (1979). Children's imitations of lexical items. Child Development 50(1), 1927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, O. Y. H. & MacLeod, C. M. (2012). Aging and the production effect: a test of the distinctiveness account. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 66(3), 212–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, P. A., Mayfield, J. W. & Reynolds, C. R. (2003). Gender differences in memory test performance among children and adolescents, Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 18(8), 865–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, P. A. & MacLeod, C. M. (1998). The influence of attention at encoding on direct and indirect remembering. Acta Psychologica 98, 291310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, C. M. (2011). I said, you said: the production effect gets personal. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18, 1197–202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, C. M., Gopie, N., Hourihan, K. L., Neary, K. R. & Ozubko, J. D. (2010). The production effect: delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 36(3), 671–85.Google ScholarPubMed
Mama, Y. & Icht, M. (in press). Auditioning the production effect: learning by the auditory modality reveals the superiority of writing over vocalizing. Memory.Google Scholar
McCarthy, M. J. (1990). Vocabulary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Meara, P. (1995). The importance of an early emphasis on L2 vocabulary. Language Teacher 19(2), 810.Google Scholar
Morrow, L. M. (1985). Retelling stories: a strategy for improving children's comprehension, concept of story structure and oral language complexity. Elementary School Journal 85, 647–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, L. M. (1987). Promoting voluntary reading: the effects of an inner city program in summer day care centers. Reading Teacher 41, 266–74.Google Scholar
Morrow, L. M. (1988). Young children's responses to one-to-one readings in school settings. Reading Research Quarterly 23, 89107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, L. M., O'Connor, E. M. & Smith, J. K. (1990). Effects of a story reading program on the literacy development of at-risk kindergarten children. Journal of Literacy Research 22, 255–75.Google Scholar
Nation, P. & Newton, J. (1997). Teaching vocabulary. In Coady, J. & Huckin, T. (eds), Second language vocabulary acquisition (pp. 238254). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nielsen, B. (2002). A review of research into vocabulary learning and acquisition, online <http://www.kushiro-ct.ac.jp/library/kiyo/kiyo36/Brian.pdf> (last accessed 6 October 2014).+(last+accessed+6+October+2014).>Google Scholar
Ninio, A. & Bruner, J. (1978). The achievement and antecedents of labeling. Journal of Child Language 5, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odom, S. L., Favazza, P. C., Brown, W. H. & Horn, E. M. (2000). Approaches to understanding the ecology of early environments for children with disabilities. In Thompson, T., Felce, D. & Symons, F. (eds), Behavioral observation: technology and applications in developmental disabilities (pp. 193214). Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Oxford, R. L. & Scarcella, R. C. (1994). Second language vocabulary learning among adults: state of the art in vocabulary instruction. System 22(2), 231–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ozubko, J. D., Gopie, N. & MacLeod, C. M. (2012). Production benefits both recollection and familiarity. Memory and Cognition 40, 326–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozubko, J. D., Hourihan, K. L. & MacLeod, C. M. (2012). Production benefits learning: the production effect endures and improves memory for text, Memory 20(7), 717–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozubko, J. D. & MacLeod, C. M. (2010). The production effect in memory: evidence that distinctiveness underlies the benefit. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 1543–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paribakht, T. S & Wesche, M. (1997). Vocabulary enhancement activities and reading for meaning in second language vocabulary acquisition. In Coady, J. & Huckin, T. (eds), Second language vocabulary acquisition (pp. 174200). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, M. & Myers, N. A. (1979). Development of recall in two- and four-year-old children. Developmental Psychology 15, 7383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, B. M., Clancy-Menchetti, J. & Lonigan, C. J. (2008). Successful phonological awareness instruction with preschool children lessons from the classroom. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 28(1), 317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pressley, M. (2001). The nature of first-grade instruction that promotes literacy achievement. In Pressley, M., Allington, R. L., Wharton-McDonald, R., Block, C. & Morrow, L. (eds), Learning to read: lessons from exemplary first-grade classrooms, 4869. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Putnam, A. L., Ozubko, J. D., MacLeod, C. M. & Roediger, H. L. III (2014). The production effect in paired-associate learning: benefits for item and associative information. Memory and Cognition 42, 409–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richler, J. J., Palmeri, T. J. & Gauthier, I. (2013). How does using object names influence visual recognition memory? Journal of Memory and Language 68(1), 1025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roser, N. & Martinez, M. (1985). Roles adults play in preschoolers’ response to literature. Language Arts 62, 485–90.Google Scholar
Rosin, D. & Yakir, P. (2000). Pictures for Examining Articulation and Naming of Hebrew-speaking Children in Israel. Israel: Rehovot.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. J. & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Transactional regulation: the developmental ecology of early intervention. In Shonkoff, J. P. & Meisels, S. J. (eds), Handbook of early childhood intervention, 2nd ed. (pp. 135159). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, R. G. & Leonard, L. B. (1985). Lexical imitation and acquisition in language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 50(2), 141–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schweickert, R. & Boruff, B. (1986). Short-term memory capacity: magic number or magic spell? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 12, 419–25.Google ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S. & Griffin, P. (eds) (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Spiegel, D. & Whaley, J. (1980). Elevating comprehension skills by sensitizing students to structural aspects of narratives. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Reading Conference, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K. (1977). The role of nonspecific factors in the task performance of learning disabled children: a theoretical assessment. Journal of Learning Disabilities 10(1), 2734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uberti, H. Z., Scruggs, T. E. & Mastropieri, M. A. (2003). Keywords make the difference! Mnemonic instruction in inclusive classrooms. Teaching Exceptional Children 35(3), 5661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weismer, S. E. & Murray-Branch, J. (1989). Modeling versus modeling plus evoked production training: a comparison of two language intervention methods. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54(2), 269–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M. (1988). The early development of memory strategies. In Weinert, F. E. & Perlmutter, M. (eds), Memory development: universal changes and individual differences (pp. 330). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. & Schlosberg, H. S. (1954). Experimental psychology. New York: Holt.Google ScholarPubMed
Zimmerman, C. B. (1994). Self-selected reading and interactive vocabulary instruction: knowledge and perceptions of word learning among L2 learners. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
24
Cited by

Save article to Kindle

To save this article to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

The production effect in memory: a prominent mnemonic in children*
Available formats
×

Save article to Dropbox

To save this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

The production effect in memory: a prominent mnemonic in children*
Available formats
×

Save article to Google Drive

To save this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

The production effect in memory: a prominent mnemonic in children*
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *