Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-17T17:27:28.966Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Case marking and word order in Greek heritage children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 January 2020

Vasiliki CHONDROGIANNI*
Affiliation:
School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, UK
Richard G. SCHWARTZ
Affiliation:
Graduate Center, City University of New York, USA
*
*Corresponding author: School of Philosophy, Psychology and Language Sciences, University of Edinburgh, 3 Charles Street, EdinburghEH8 9AD. Tel.: (+44)(0)131-650-4020; E-mail: v.chondrogianni@ed.ac.uk

Abstract

This study examined the linguistic and individual-level factors that render case marking a vulnerable domain in English-dominant Greek heritage children. We also investigated whether heritage language (HL) children can use case-marking cues to interpret (non-)canonical sentences in Greek similarly to their monolingual peers. A group of six- to twelve-year-old Greek heritage children in New York City and a control group of age-matched monolingual children living in Greece participated in a production and a picture verification task targeting case marking and (non-)canonical word order in Greek. HL children produced syncretic inflectional errors, also found in preschool monolingual children. In the comprehension task, HL children showed variable performance on the non-canonical OVS but ceiling performance on the SVO conditions, which suggests influence from English. Linguistic factors such as case transparency affected comprehension, whereas child-level factors such as proficiency and degree of (early) use of Greek influenced performance on both modalities.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexiadou, A. (1996). Subject positions in Modern Greek. Studies in Greek Linguistics, 16, 242353.Google Scholar
Alexiadou, A., & Anagnostopoulou, E. (1998). Parametrizing AGR: word order, V-movement and EPP-checking. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 16(3), 491539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexopoulou, D. (1999). The syntax of discourse functions in Greek: a non configurational approach. University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S., Walters, J., & Gagarina, N. (2011). The impact of internal and external factors on linguistic performance in the home language and in L2 among Russian–Hebrew and Russian–German preschool children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 291317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benmamoun, E., Montrul, S., & Polinsky, M. (2013). Heritage languages and their speakers: opportunities and challenges for linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics, 39(3/4), 129–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blom, E., & Unsworth, S. (2010). Experimental methods in language acquisition research. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohman, T. M., Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Mendez-Perez, A., & Gillam, R. B. (2010). What you hear and what you say: language performance in Spanish–English bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13(3), 325–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chatterjee, S., & Hadi, A. S. (2006). Logistic regression. In Regression Analysis by Example. Online <https://doi.org/doi:10.1002/0470055464.ch1>.CrossRef.>Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V. (2018). Child L2 acquisition. In Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L. (Eds.), Studies in bilingualism (Vol. 54, pp. 103–26). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T. (2011). Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, tense morphology and morpho-syntax in successive bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 318–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chung, E. S. (2018). Second and heritage language acquisition of Korean case drop*. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(1), 6379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daskalaki, E., Chondrogianni, V., Blom, E., Argyri, F., & Paradis, J. (2019). Input effects across domains: the case of Greek subjects in child heritage language. Second Language Research, 35(3), 421–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition (PPVTTM-4). London: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
Flores, C. (2015). Losing a language in childhood: a longitudinal case study on language attrition. Journal of Child Language, 42(3), 562–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flores, C., Santos, A. L., Jesus, A., & Marques, R. (2017). Age and input effects in the acquisition of mood in Heritage Portuguese. Journal of Child Language, 44(4), 795828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J. & R Core Team (2019). Effect displays for linear, generalized linear, and other models. Online <https://www.r-project.org> (last accessed 17 December 2019).+(last+accessed+17+December+2019).>Google Scholar
Gagarina, N., & Klassert, A. (2018). Input dominance and development of home language in Russian–German bilinguals. Frontiers in Communication, e00040.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism, 12(2), 213–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guasti, M. T., Stavrakaki, S., & Arosio, F. (2012). Cross-linguistic differences and similarities in the acquisition of relative clauses: evidence from Greek and Italian. Lingua, 122(6), 700–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haman, E., Wodniecka, Z., Marecka, M., Szewczyk, J., Białecka-Pikul, M., Otwinowska, A., … Foryś-Nogala, M. (2017). How does L1 and L2 exposure impact L1 performance in bilingual children? Evidence from Polish–English migrants to the United Kingdom. Frontiers in Psychology, e01444.Google ScholarPubMed
Harrell, F. E., Lee, K. L., & Mark, D. B. (1996). Multivariable prognostic models: Issues in developing models, evaluating assumptions and adequacy, and measuring and reducing errors. Statistics in Medicine, 15, 361–87.3.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hendriks, P., & Koster, C. (2010). Production/comprehension asymmetries in language acquisition. Lingua, 120(8), 1887–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., & Core, C. (2013). Input and language development in bilingually developing children. Seminars in Speech and Language, 34(4), 215–26.Google ScholarPubMed
Hopp, H., & Putnam, M. T. (2015). Syntactic restructuring in heritage grammars: word order variation in Moundridge Schweitzer German. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 5(2), 180214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janssen, B. (2017). The acquisition of gender and case in Polish and Russian: a study of monolingual and bilingual children. Amsterdam: Uitgeverij Pegasus.Google Scholar
Janssen, B., Meir, N., Baker, A., & Armon-lotem, S. (2014). On-line comprehension of Russian case cues in monolingual Russian and bilingual Russian–Dutch and Russian–Hebrew children. In BUCLD 39: proceedings of the 39th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (1113) (pp. 266–78).Google Scholar
Kapetangianni, K. (2011). Variable word order in child Greek. In Anderssen, M., Bentzen, K., & Westergaard, M. (Eds.), Variation in the input (Vol. 39, pp. 179205). Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, F., & Alexopoulou, D. (2001). Phonology competes with syntax: experimental evidence for the interaction of word order and accent placement in the realisation of Information Structure. Cognition, 79, 301–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K., O'Grady, W., & Schwartz, B. D. (2017). Case in Heritage Korean. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 8(2), 252–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kupisch, T., & Rothman, J. (2018). Terminology matters! Why difference is not incompleteness and how early child bilinguals are heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22(5), 564–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laleko, O., & Polinsky, M. (2016). Between syntax and discourse: Topic and case marking in heritage speakers and L2 learners of Japanese and Korean. Linguistic Approaches To Bilingualism, 6(4), 396439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lascaratou, C. (1989). A functional approach to constituent order with particular reference to Modern Greek: implications for language learning and language teaching. Athens: Parousia.Google Scholar
Łyskawa, P., & Nagy, N. (2019). Case marking variation in Heritage Slavic Languages in Toronto: not so different. Language Learning, e12348.Google Scholar
Marinis, T. (2003). The acquisition of the DP in Modern Greek. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marinis, T., Chondrogianni, V., Vasić, N., Weerman, F., & Blom, E. (2017). The impact of transparency and morpho-phonological cues in the acquisition of grammatical gender in sequential bilingual children and children with Specific Language Impairment. In E. Blom, L. Cornips, & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence in bilingualism: in honor of Aafke Hulk (pp. 153–80). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Meisel, J. M. (2007). The weaker language in early child bilingualism: Acquiring a first language as a second language? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28(3), 495514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2015). The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S., Bhatia, A., Bhatt, R., & Puri, V. (2019). Case marking in Hindi as the weaker language. Frontiers in Psychology, e00461.Google ScholarPubMed
Montrul, S., Bhatt, R., & Bhatia, A. (2012). Erosion of case and agreement in Hindi heritage speakers. Linguistic Approaches To Bilingualism, 2(2), 141–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omaki, A., Davidson White, I., Goro, T., Lidz, J., & Phillips, C. (2014). No fear of commitment: children's incremental interpretation in English and Japanese wh-questions. Language Learning and Development, 10(3), 206–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2011). Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches To Bilingualism, 1(3), 213–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S. C., Lewedeg, V., & Oller, D. K. (1997). The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 4158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philippaki-Warbarton, I. (1985). Word order in Modern Greek. Transactions of the Philoslogical Society, 83(1), 114–43.Google Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2016). Effects and noneffects of input in bilingual environments on dual language skills in 2½-year-olds. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(5), 1023–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polinsky, M. (2006). Incomplete acquisition: American Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 14(2), 191262.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Scontras, G. (2019). Understanding heritage languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 117. doi:10.1017/S1366728919000245Google Scholar
R Core Team (2013). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Ralli, A. (2002). The role of morphology in gender determination: evidence from Modern Greek. Linguistics, 40(3), 519–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reinhart, T. (2006). Interface strategies. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodina, Y., & Westergaard, M. (2017). Grammatical gender in bilingual Norwegian–Russian acquisition: the role of input and transparency. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(1), 197214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roesch, A. D., & Chondrogianni, V. (2016). ‘Which mouse kissed the frog?’ Effects of age of onset, length of exposure, and knowledge of case marking on the comprehension of wh-questions in German-speaking simultaneous and early sequential bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 43(3), 635–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roussou, A., & Tsimpli, I. M. (2006). On Greek VSO again! Journal of Linguistics, 42(2), 317–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sauerland, U., Grohmann, K. K., Guasti, M. T., Anđelković, D., Argus, R., Armon-Lotem, S., … Yatsushiro, K. (2016). How do 5-year-olds understand questions? Differences in languages across Europe. First Language, 36(3), 169202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skopeteas, S. (2016). Information structure in Modern Greek. In Féry, C. & Ishihara, S. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of information structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199642670.013.15Google Scholar
Song, M., O'Grady, W., Cho, S., & Lee, M. (1997). The learning and teaching of Korean in community schools. In Kim, Y.-H. (Ed.), Korean language in America (Vol. 2, pp. 111–27). Los Angeles, CA: American Association of Teachers of Korean.Google Scholar
Stavrakaki, S., Tasioudi, M., & Guasti, T. (2015). Morphological cues in the comprehension of relative clauses by Greek children with specific language impairment and typical development: a comparative study. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17(6), 617–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephany, U. (1997). The acquisition of Greek. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (pp. 183333). Mahwah, NJ/London: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thordardottir, E. (2015). The relationship between bilingual exposure and morphosyntactic development. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17(2), 97114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsimpli, I. M. (1995). Focusing in modern Greek. In Kiss, K. E. (Ed.), Discourse configurational languages (pp. 176206). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M. (2005). Peripheral positions in early Greek. In Stavrou, M. and Terzi, A. (Eds.), Advances in Greek generative syntax (Linguistics Today, pp. 179216). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Hulk, A. (2013). Grammatical gender and the notion of default: insights from language acquisition. Lingua, 137, 128–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Stavrakaki, S. (1999). The effects of a morphosyntactic deficit in the determiner system: the case of a Greek SLI child. Lingua, 108(1), 3185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsimpli, I. M., & Stavrakaki, S. (2001). Diagnostic Verbal IQ test. University of Thessaloniki.Google Scholar
Unsworth, S. (2013). Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: the case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(1), 86110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, S., Chondrogianni, V., & Skarabela, B. (2018). Experiential measures can be used as a proxy for language dominance in bilingual language acquisition research. Frontiers in Psychology, e01809.Google ScholarPubMed
Varlokosta, S., Nerantzini, M., & Papadopoulou, D. (2015). Comprehension asymmetries in language acquisition: a test for Relativized Minimality. Journal of Child Language, 42(3), 618–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogindroukas, I., Proropapas, A., & Sideridis, G. (2009). Expressive Vocabulary Task [Δοκιμασία Εκφραστικού Λεξιλογίου]. Chania: Glafki.Google Scholar
Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xanthos, A., Laaha, S., Gillis, S., Stephany, U., Aksu-Koç, A., Christofidou, A., … Dressler, W. U. (2011). On the role of morphological richness in the early development of noun and verb inflection. First Language, 31(4), 461–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yager, L., Hellmold, N., Joo, H. A., Putnam, M. T., Rossi, E., Stafford, C., & Salmons, J. (2015). New structural patterns in moribund grammar: case marking in heritage German. Frontiers in Psychology, e01716.Google ScholarPubMed
Zombolou, K. (2011). Attrition in Greek Diaspora: Grammars in contact or incomplete acquisition?. In Ihemere, I. (Ed.), Language contact and language shift: grammatical and sociolinguistic perspectives (Studies in Language Typology) (pp. 133). Munich: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar