Skip to main content
×
Home

Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill*

  • MEREDITH L. ROWE (a1)
Abstract
ABSTRACT

This study sought to determine why American parents from different socioeconomic backgrounds communicate in different ways with their children. Forty-seven parent–child dyads were videotaped engaging in naturalistic interactions in the home for ninety minutes at child age 2 ; 6. Transcripts of these interactions provided measures of child-directed speech. Children's vocabulary comprehension skills were measured using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test at 2 ; 6 and one year later at 3 ; 6. Results indicate that: (1) child-directed speech with toddlers aged 2 ; 6 predicts child vocabulary skill one year later, controlling for earlier toddler vocabulary skill; (2) child-directed speech relates to socioeconomic status as measured by income and education; and (3) the relation between socioeconomic status and child-directed speech is mediated by parental knowledge of child development. Potential mechanisms through which parental knowledge influences communicative behavior are discussed.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Address for correspondence: Meredith L. Rowe, Department of Psychology, University of Chicago, 5848 S. University Ave, Chicago, IL 60637. e-mail: rowemer@uchicago.edu
References
Hide All
Arriaga R. J., Fenson L., Cronan T. & Pethick S. J. (1998). Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low- and middle-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics 19, 209–23.
Baron R. M. & Kenny D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of personality and social psychology 51 (6), 1173–82.
Benasich A. A. & Brooks-Gunn J. (1996). Maternal attitudes and knowledge of child-rearing: Associations with family and child outcomes. Child Development 67, 1186–205.
Borduin C. M. & Henggeler S. W. (1981). Social class, experimental setting, and task characteristics as determinants of mother-child interaction. Developmental Psychology 17, 209–14.
Bornstein M. H., Haynes M. O. & Painter K. M. (1998). Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language 25, 367–93.
Bronfenbrenner U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Clarke-Stewart K. A. (1978). Popular primers for parents. American Psychologist 33, 359–69.
Donahue M. L., Pearl R. & Herzog A. (1997). Mothers' referential communication with preschoolers: Effects of children's syntax and mothers' beliefs. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 18, 133–47.
Dunn L. M. & Dunn L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 3rd ed.Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Farran D. C. & Haskins R. (1980). Reciprocal influence in the social interaction of mothers and three-year-old children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Development 51, 780–91.
Goodnow J. J. (2002). Parents' knowledge and expectations: Using what we know. In Bornstein M. H. (ed.), Handbook of parenting, 2nd ed., Vol. 3, 439–60. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Goodnow J. J. & Collins W. A. (1990). Development according to parents: The nature, sources, and consequences of parents' ideas. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Harkness S. & Super C. M. (eds) (1996). Parent's cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions, and consequences. New York: The Guilford Press.
Hart B. & Risley T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.
Heath S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Hoff E. (2003 a). The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development 74, 1368–78.
Hoff E. (2003 b). Causes and consequences of SES-related differences in parent-to-child speech. In Bornstein M. H. (ed.), Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development, 147–60. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hoff E., Laursen B. & Tardif T. (2002). Socioeconomic status and parenting. In Bornstein M. H. (ed.), Handbook of parenting, 2nd ed., 231–52. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hoff E. & Naigles L. (2002). How children use input in acquiring a lexicon. Child Development 73, 418–33.
Hoff-Ginsberg E. (1991). Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development 62, 782–96.
Huttenlocher J., Haight W., Bryk A., Seltzer M. & Lyons T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.
Huttenlocher J., Vasilyeva M., Waterfall H. R., Vevea J. L. & Hedges L. V. (in press). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology.
Johnston J. B. & Wong M.-Y. A. (2002). Cultural differences in beliefs and practices concerning talk to children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 45, 916–26.
Lawrence V. & Shipley E. F. (1996). Parental speech to middle and working class children from two racial groups in three settings. Applied Psycholinguistics 17, 233–56.
LeVine R. A. (2004). Challenging expert knowledge: Findings from an African study of infant care and development. In Gielen U. P. & Roopnarine J. L. (eds), Childhood and adolescence: Cross-cultural perspectives and applications, 149–65. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Luster T. & Okagaki L. (1993). Multiple influences on parenting: Ecological and life-course perspectives. In Okagaki L. (ed.), Parenting: An ecological perspective, 227–50. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
MacPhee D. (2002; first published 1981). Knowledge of Infant Development Inventory: Manual. Colorado: Colorado State University.
Malvern D., Richards B. J., Chipere N. & Duran P. (2004). Lexical diversity and language development: Quantification and assessment. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
McGillicuddy-DeLisi A. V. & Sigel I. E. (1995). Parental beliefs. In Bornstein M. H. (ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 3, 333–58. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Miller S. A. (1988). Parents' beliefs about children's cognitive development. Child Development 59, 259–85.
Miller S. A. & Davis T. L. (1992). Beliefs about children: A comparative study of mothers, teachers, peers and self. Child Development 63, 1251–65.
Ninio A. (1980). Picture-book reading in mother-infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. Child Development 51, 587–90.
Ochs E. & Schieffelin B. B. (1984). Language acquisition and socializations: Three developmental stories and their implications. In Shweder R. A. & LeVine R. A. (eds), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion. 276320. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Pan B. A., Rowe M. L., Singer J. D. & Snow C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development 76, 763–82.
Richman A. L., Miller P. M. & LeVine R. A. (1992). Cultural and educational variations in maternal responsiveness. Developmental Psychology 28, 614–21.
Rowe M. L. (2000). Pointing and talk by low-income mothers and their 14-month-old children. First Language 20, 305–30.
Rowe M. L., Pan B. A. & Ayoub C. (2005). Predictors of variation in maternal talk to children: A longitudinal study of low-income families. Parenting: Science and Practice 5, 285310.
Sigel I. E. & McGillicuddy-De Lisi A. V. (2002). Parent beliefs are cognitions: The dynamic belief systems model. In Bornstein M. H. (ed.), Handbook of parenting. 2nd ed., Vol. 3, 485508. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Sigel I. E. (1985). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Snow C. E. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4, 122.
Vygotsky L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wechsler D. (1981). Manual for Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revisited. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Journal of Child Language
  • ISSN: 0305-0009
  • EISSN: 1469-7602
  • URL: /core/journals/journal-of-child-language
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 178
Total number of PDF views: 1060 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 3765 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between September 2016 - 24th November 2017. This data will be updated every 24 hours.