Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home

Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development*

  • JANA M. IVERSON (a1)

Abstract

During the first eighteen months of life, infants acquire and refine a whole set of new motor skills that significantly change the ways in which the body moves in and interacts with the environment. In this review article, I argue that motor acquisitions provide infants with an opportunity to practice skills relevant to language acquisition before they are needed for that purpose; and that the emergence of new motor skills changes infants' experience with objects and people in ways that are relevant for both general communicative development and the acquisition of language. Implications of this perspective for current views of co-occurring language and motor impairments and for methodology in the field of child language research are also considered.

Copyright

Corresponding author

Address for correspondence: Jana M. Iverson, Dept. of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, 3415 Sennott Square, 210 S. Bouquet St., Pittsburgh, PA 15260 USA. tel: +001 (412) 624-6160; fax: +001 (412) 624-4428; email: jiverson@pitt.edu

Footnotes

Hide All
[*]

Preparation of this article was supported by a grant from the National Institutes of Health (R01 HD54979). I am grateful to Erin Koterba, Meg Parladé and Nina Leezenbaum for discussion of many of the ideas presented here, to Robert H. Wozniak for extensive and insightful comments, and to Edith Bavin and two anonymous reviewers for helpful suggestions on previous versions of the manuscript. This article is dedicated to the memory of Michael M. Iverson.

Footnotes

References

Hide All
Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.
Bates, E., Bretherton, I., Snyder, L., Shore, C. & Volterra, V. (1980). Vocal and gestural symbols at 13 months. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 26, 407–23.
Bates, E. & Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology 40, 293310.
Bernardis, P., Bello, A., Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S. & Gentilucci, M. (2008). Manual actions affect the vocalizations of infants. Experimental Brain Research 184, 599603.
Bishop, D. V. M. (2002). Motor immaturity and specific speech and language impairment: Evidence for a common genetic basis. American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 114, 5663.
Bishop, D. V. M. & Edmundson, A. (1987). Specific language impairment as a maturational lag: Evidence from longitudinal data on language and motor development. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 29, 442–59.
Bloom, L. (1993). The transition from infancy to language. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Butterworth, G. & Grover, L. (1990). Joint visual attention, manual pointing and preverbal communication in human infancy. In Jeannerod, M. (ed.), Attention and performance XIII, 605624. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J. & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy 1, 149219.
Campos, J. J., Kermoian, R., Witherington, D., Chen, H. & Dong, Q. (1997). Activity, attention, and developmental transitions in infancy. In Lang, P. J. & Simons, R. F. (eds), Attention and orienting: Sensory and motivational processes, 393415. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V. (2005). From action to language through gesture: A longitudinal perspective. Gesture 5, 155–77.
Clearfield, M. W., Osborne, C. N. & Mullen, M. (2008). Learning by looking: Infants' social looking behavior across the transition from crawling to walking. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 100, 297307.
Corrigan, R. (1979). Language development as related to Stage 6 object permanence development. Journal of Child Language 5, 173–89.
Darrah, J., Hodge, M., Magill-Evans, J. & Kembhavi, G. (2003). Stability of serial assessments of motor and communication abilities in typically developing infants – implications for screening. Early Human Development 72, 97–110.
D'Entremont, B., Hains, S. M. J. & Muir, D. W. (1997). A demonstration of gaze following in 3- to 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development 20, 569–72.
Eilers, R., Oller, D. K., Levine, S., Basinger, D., Lynch, M. P. & Urbano, R. (1993). The role of prematurity and socioeconomic status in the onset of canonical babbling in infants. Infant Behavior and Development 16, 297315.
Ejiri, K. (1998). Relationship between rhythmic behavior and canonical babbling in infant vocal development. Phonetica 55, 226–37.
Ejiri, K. & Masataka, N. (2001). Co-occurrence of preverbal vocal behavior and motor action in early infancy. Developmental Science 4, 4048.
Elbers, L. (1982). Operating principles in repetitive babbling: A cognitive continuity approach. Cognition 12, 4563.
Escalona, S. (1973). Basic modes of social interaction: Their emergence and patterning during the first two years of life. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 19, 205–32.
Fagan, M. K. & Iverson, J. M. (2007). The influence of mouthing on infant vocalization. Infancy 11, 191202.
Fenson, L., Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B. & Zelazo, P. R. (1976). The developmental progression of manipulative play in the first two years. Child Development 47, 232–36.
Gentilucci, M., Santunione, P., Roy, A. C. & Stefanini, S. (2004). Execution and observation of bringing a fruit to the mouth affect syllable pronunciation. European Journal of Neuroscience 19, 190202.
Gentilucci, M., Stefanini, S., Roy, A. C. & Santunione, P. (2004). Action observation and speech production: Study on children and adults. Neuropsychologia 42, 1554–67.
Gesell, A. (1929). Infancy and human growth. New York: The Macmillan Company.
Gogate, L. J. & Bahrick, L. E. (1998). Intersensory redundancy facilitates learning of arbitrary relations between vowel sounds and objects in 7-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 69(2), 133–49.
Gogate, L. J., Bolzani, L. E. & Betancourt, E. (2006). Attention to maternal multimodal naming by 6- to 8-month-old infants and learning of word-object relations. Infancy 9(3), 259–88.
Goldin-Meadow, S. & Iverson, J. M. (in press). Gesturing over the lifespan. In Lerner, R. and Overton, W. (eds), Handbook of lifespan development. Volume 1: Methods, biology, neuroscience, and cognitive development. New York: John Wiley.
Green, J. R. & Wilson, E. M. (2006). Spontaneous facial motility in infancy: A 3D kinematic analysis. Developmental Psychobiology 48, 1628.
Hill, E. L. (1998). A dyspraxic deficit in specific language impairment and developmental coordination disorder? Evidence from hand and arm movements. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 40, 388–95.
Hill, E. L. (2001). Non-specific nature of specific language impairment: A review of the literature with regard to concomitant motor impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 36, 149–71.
Inhelder, B., Lézine, I, Sinclair, H. & Stambak, W. (1971). Les débuts de la fonctionne symbolique. Archives de Psychologie 41, 187243.
Iverson, J. M., Hall, A. J., Nickel, L. & Wozniak, R. H. (2007). The relationship between onset of reduplicated babble and laterality biases in infant rhythmic arm movements. Brain and Language 101, 198207.
Iverson, J. M. & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth, and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6, 1940.
Iverson, J. M. & Wozniak, R. H. (2007). Variation in vocal-motor development in infant siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37, 158–70.
Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Adolph, K. E. (under review). Transition from crawling to walking affects infants' social actions with objects. Child Development.
Kent, R. D. (1984). Psychobiology of speech development: Co-emergence of language and a movement system. American Journal of Physiobiology 246, 888–94.
Koopmans-van Beinum, F. J. & van der Stelt, J. M. (1986). Early stages in the development of speech movements. In Lindbom, B. & Zetterstrom, R. (eds), Precursors of early speech, 3750. New York: Stockton Press.
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological foundations of language. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Le Normand, M.-T., Vaivre-Douret, L. & Delfosse, M. J. (1995). Language and motor development in pre-term children: Some questions. Child: Care, Health, and Development 21, 119–33.
Leonard, L. B. (2000). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lewkowicz, D. J. (2000). Development of intersensory temporal perception: An epigenetic systems/limitations view. Psychological Bulletin 126(2), 281308.
Lifter, K. & Bloom, L. (1989). Object knowledge and the emergence of language. Infant Behavior and Development 12, 395423.
Locke, J. L., Bekken, K. E., McMinn-Larson, L. & Wein, D. (1995). Emergent control of manual and vocal-motor activity in relation to the development of speech. Brain and Language 51, 498508.
MacNeilage, P. F. & Davis, B. L. (2000). On the origin of internal structure of word forms. Science 288, 527–31.
McGraw, M. B. (1943). The neuromuscular maturation of the human infant. New York: Columbia University Press.
McNeill, D. (2006). Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Meier, R. P., Mauk, C. E., Cheek, A. & Moreland, C. J. (2008). The form of children's early signs: Iconic or motoric determinants? Language Learning and Development 4, 6398.
Meier, R. P., McGarvin, L., Zakia, R. A. E. & Willerman, R. (1997). Silent mandibular oscillations in vocal babbling. Phonetica 54, 153–71.
Molfese, V. J. & Betz, J. C. (1987). Language and motor development in infancy: Three views with neuropsychological implications. Developmental Neuropsychology 3, 255–74.
Moore, C. (1999). Gaze following and the control of attention. In Rochat, E. P. (ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life, 241–56. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Society for Research in Child Development Monographs 38, (1–2 Serial No. 149).
Nicolich, L. (1977). Beyond sensorimotor intelligence: Assessment of symbolic maturity through analysis of pretend play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 23, 3999.
Nip, I. S. B., Green, J. R. & Marx, D. B. (2009). Early speech motor development: Cognitive and linguistic considerations. Journal of Communication Disorders 42, 286–98.
Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (1988). The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Development 59, 441–66.
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children, trans. M. Cook. New York: International Universities Press.
Rodgon, M. M., Jankowski, W. & Alenskas, L. (1977). Journal of Child Language 4, 2344.
Rosenbaum, D. (2005). The Cinderella of psychology: The neglect of motor control in the science of mental life and behavior. American Psychologist 60, 308–17.
Ruff, H. A. (1984). Infants' manipulative exploration of objects: Effects of age and object characteristics. Developmental Psychology 20, 9–20.
Ruff, H. A., Saltarelli, L. M., Capozzoli, M. & Dubiner, K. (1992). The differentiation of activity in infants' exploration of objects. Developmental Psychology 28, 851–61.
Snyder, L. (1978). Communicative and cognitive abilities and disabilities during the sensorimotor period. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 24, 161–80.
Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behaviour 27, 699715.
Thelen, E. (1991). Motor aspects of emergent speech: A dynamic approach. In Krasnegor, N. (ed.), Biobehavioral foundations of language, 339–62. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Thelen, E. (2004). The central role of action in typical and atypical development: A dynamic systems perspective. In Stockman, I. J. (ed.), Movement and action in learning and development: Clinical implications for pervasive developmental disorders, 4973. San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press.
Thelen, E. & Smith, L. B. (1993). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Tipps, S. T., Mira, M. P. & Cairns, G. F. (1981). Concurrent tracking of infant motor and speech development. Genetic Psychology Monographs 104, 303–24.
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57, 1454–63.
Viholainen, H., Ahonen, T., Cantell, M., Lyytinen, P. & Lyytinen, H. (2002). Development of early motor skills and language in children at risk for familial dyslexia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 44, 761–69.
Viholainen, H., Ahonen, T., Lyytinen, P., Cantell, M., Tolvanen, A. & Lyytinen, H. (2006). Early motor development and later language and reading skills in children at risk of familial dyslexia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 48, 367–73.
Volterra, V., Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I. & Camaioni, L. (1979). First words in language and action: A qualitative look. In Bates, E. (ed.), The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy, 141222. New York: Academic Press.
Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O. & Pizzuto, E. (2005). Gesture and the emergence and development of language. In Slobin, D. & Tomasello, M. (eds), Beyond nature–nurture: Essays in honor of Elizabeth Bates, 340. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.
Willems, R. M. & Hagoort, P. (2007). Neural evidence for the interplay between language, gesture, and action: A review. Brain and Language 101, 287–98.
Yingling, J. (1981). Temporal features of infant speech: A description of babbling patterns circumscribed by postural achievement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Denver.
Zumbahlen, M. (1997). The role of infant locomotor onset in shaping mother–infant communication. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development*

  • JANA M. IVERSON (a1)

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed