Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T20:17:14.890Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of previously learned words on the child's acquisition of words for similar referents*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Michael Tomasello*
Affiliation:
Emory University
Sara Mannle*
Affiliation:
Emory University
Lori Werdenschlag*
Affiliation:
Emory University
*
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322, USA.
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322, USA.
Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322, USA.

Abstract

The current study assessed the child's learning of an object label in two conditions: when its referent was highly similar and when its referent was highly dissimilar to the referent of a known child word. Twenty-two 2-year-old children were taught the name for an object. In a session several days subsequent, they were taught names for two new objects: one which was highly similar and one which was highly dissimilar to the referent of the word learned in the first session (two stimulus sets assured counterbalancing). In this second session, children produced the word with the similar referent more often than the word with the dissimilar referent. In third and fourth sessions (several days later), children continued to show better performance with the similar word in comprehension. These results were interpreted as evidence that young children find it easier to learn a new word when they are able to contrast its referent with that of a word they already know.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

The authors would like to thank the children and mothers who made this study possible. Our thanks also go to Larry Barsalou and Robyn Fivush for helpful comments. This research was supported in part by NIH Grant R03 MH38997-01A1 to the first author.

References

REFERENCES

Baker, N. & Nelson, K. E. (1984). Recasting and related conversational techniques for triggering syntactic advances by young children. First Language 5. 322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. (1986). Early semantic representations and early word usage. In Kuczaj, S. & Barrett, M. (eds), The acquisition of word meaning. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Chapman, K., Leonard, L. & Mervis, C. (1986). The effect of feedback on young children's inappropriate word usage. Journal of Child Language 13. 101–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlesworth, W. (1969). In Elkind, D. & Flavell, J. (eds), Studies in cognitive development. New York: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1983). Meanings and concepts. In Flavell, J. & Markman, E. (eds), Handbook of child psychology. Cognitive development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1987). The principle of contrast: a constraint on language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hilldale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Farrar, J. (1986). The acquisition of grammatical morphemes from discourse: a cognitive perspective. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Emory University.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. (1987). The contrastive hypothesis for the acquisition of word meaning: a reconsideration of the theory. Journal of Child Language 14. 493532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, J. (1987). Children's memory for atypical actions in script-based stories. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, Maryland.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The acquisition of morphonology. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 43. No. 174. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Markman, E. (1987). How children constrain the possible meanings of words. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Merriman, W. (1986). Some reasons for the occurrence and eventual correction of children's naming errors. Child Development 57. 942–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merriman, W. (1987 a). Lexical contrast in toddlers: a reanalysis of the diary evidence. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, Maryland.Google Scholar
Merriman, W. (1987 b). Developmental studies of lexical contrast. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, Maryland.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. (1987). Child-basic object categories and early lexical development. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E. (1977). Facilitating children's syntax acquisition. Developmental Psychology 13. 101–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. E. (1981). Toward a rare-event cognitive comparison theory of syntax acquisition. In Dale, P. & Ingram, D. (eds.), Child language: an international perspective. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. E. (in press). Some observations from the perspective of the rare event cognitive comparison theory of language acquisition. In Nelson, K. E. & van Kleeck, A. (eds.), Children's language. Vol. 6. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1984). The equilibration of cognitive structures. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shipley, E. & Kuhn, I. (1983). A constraint on comparisons: equally detailed alternatives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 35. 195222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. & Medin, D. (1981). Categories and concepts. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Herron, C. (in press). Down the garden path: inducing and correcting overgeneralization errors in the foreign language classroom. Applied Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Parrar, J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57. 1454–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed