Skip to main content
×
Home

Maternal verbal responsiveness and directiveness: consistency, stability, and relations to child early linguistic development*

  • LEILA PAAVOLA-RUOTSALAINEN (a1), JAANA LEHTOSAARI (a2), JOSEFINA PALOMÄKI (a3) and IMMI TERVO (a4)
Abstract
Abstract

Maternal responsive and directive speech to children at ages 0;10 and 2;0 was investigated by applying a procedure first introduced by Flynn and Masur (2007) to a new language community (Finnish). The issues examined were consistency and stability over time, and also the role of responsiveness and directiveness in child linguistic development at 1;0 and 2;6. The measures of maternal speech from each age were used to predict the results of the subsequent linguistic assessment. Negative correlations between responsive and directive utterances were found at both ages. The frequencies of responsive utterances and supportive directives increased over time. Responsiveness was positively, and intrusive directiveness negatively, related to child early comprehensive skills and the use of symbolic actions and communicative gestures. By contrast, no relations were found between responsiveness and directiveness and children's later linguistic capacities.

Copyright
Corresponding author
*Address for correspondence: Leila Paavola-Ruotsalainen. e-mail: leila.paavola@oulu.fi
References
Hide All
Akhtar N., Dunham F. & Dunham P. (1991). Directive interactions and early vocabulary development: the role of joint attentional focus. Journal of Child Language 18, 41–9.
Arshavsky Y. (2009). Two functions of early language experience. Brain Research Reviews 60, 327–40.
Barrett M., Harris M. & Chasin J. (1991). Early lexical development and maternal speech: a comparison of children's initial and subsequent uses of words. Journal of Child Language 18, 2140.
Bornstein M. H., Tamis-LeMonda C. S. & Haynes O. M. (1999). First words in the second year: continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Infant Behavior and Development 22, 6585.
Brent M. R. & Siskind J. M. (2001). The role of exposure to isolated words in early vocabulary development. Cognition 81, B3344.
Bruner J. S. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Language and Communication 1, 155–78.
Bruner J. S. (1983). Child's talk: learning to use language. New York: Norton.
Ciccone N., Hennessey N. & Stokes S. F. (2012). Community-based early intervention for language delay: a preliminary investigation. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 47, 467–70.
Clark E. (2009). First language acquisition, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Clark R. (1985). The Parent–Child Early Relational Assessment. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Medical School, Department of Psychiatry.
Darrah J., Hodge M., Magill-Evans J. & Kembhavi G. (2003). Stability of serial assessments of motor and communication abilities in typically developing infants – implications for screening. Early Human Development 72, 97110.
Della Corte M., Benedict H. & Klein D. (1983). The relationship of pragmatic dimensions of mothers’ speech to the referential–expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language 10, 3443.
Desmarais C., Sylvestre A., Meyer F., Bairati I. & Rouleau N. (2008). Systematic review of the literature on characteristics of late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Language & Communicative Disorders 43, 361–89.
D'Odorico L., Salerni N., Cassibba R. & Jacob V. (1999) Stability and change of maternal speech to Italian infants from 7 to 21 months of age: a longitudinal study of its influence on early stages of language acquisition. First Language 19, 313–46.
Dunham P. J., Dunham F. & Curwin A. (1993). Joint-attentional states and lexical acquisition at 18 months. Developmental Psychology 29, 827–31.
Edwards S., Fletcher P., Garman M., Hughes A., Letts C. & Sinka I. (1997). Reynell Developmental Language Scales III. The University of Reading edition. Berkshire: NferNelson (Translation and standardization of the Finnish version: Psykologien Kustannus Oy, 2001).
Fenson L., Bates E., Dale P., Goodman J., Reznick S. & Thal D. (2000). Measuring variability in early child language: don't shoot the messenger. Child Development 71, 323–8.
Fenson L., Dale P. S., Reznick J. S., Bates E., Thal D. & Pethick S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59 (5, Serial No. 242).
Ferguson C. A. (2004 [1978]). Talking to children: a search for universals. Reprinted in Lust B. C. & Foley C. (eds), First language acquisition: the essential readings (pp. 176189). Oxford: Blackwell.
Flynn V. & Masur E. (2007). Characteristics of maternal verbal style: responsiveness and directiveness in two natural contexts. Journal of Child Language 34, 519–43.
Furrow D., Nelson K. & Benedict H. (1979). Mothers’ speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. Journal of Child Language 6, 423–42.
Gibbard D., Coglan L. & MacDonald J. (2004). Cost-effectiveness analysis of current practice and parent intervention for children under 3 years presenting with expressive language delay. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 39, 229–44.
Girolametto L., Bonifacio S., Visini C., Weitzman E., Zocconi E. & Pearce P. S. (2002). Mother–child interactions in Canada and Italy: linguistic responsiveness to late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Communication Disorders 37, 153–71.
Girolametto L., Weitzman E., Wiigs M. & Pearce P. (1999). The relationship between maternal language measures and language development in toddlers with expressive vocabulary delays. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 8, 364–74.
Gleitman L., Newport E. & Gleitman H. (1984). The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language 11, 4379.
Goodman J. C., Dale P. S. & Ping L. (2008) Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.
Hampson J. & Nelson K. (1993). The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language 20, 313–42.
Harrison L. J. & McLeod S. (2010). Risk and protective factors associated with speech and language impairment in a nationally representative sample of 4- to 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53, 508–29.
Hoff E. & Naigles L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development 73, 418–33.
Hurtado N., Marchman V. A. & Fernald A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science 11, 31–9.
Huttenlocher J., Waterfall H. R., Vasilyeva M., Vevea J. L. & Hedges L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children's language growth. Cognitive Psychology 61, 343–65.
Kaiser A. P. & Hancock T. B. (2003). Teaching parents new skills to support their young children's development. Infants & Young Children 16, 921.
Landry S. H., Chapienski M. L. & Schmidt M. (1986). Effects of maternal attention-directing strategies on preterms’ response to toys. Infant Behavior and Development 9, 257–69.
Landry S. H, Smith K. E. & Swank P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology 42, 627–42.
Lloyd C. A. & Masur E. F. (2014). Infant behaviors influence mothers’ provision of responsive and directive behaviors. Infant Behavior & Development 37, 276–85.
Lyytinen P. (1999) MCDI-testi: varhaisen kommunikaation ja kielen kehityksen arviointimenetelmä [MCDI test: an assessment method for early communicative and language development] . Jyväskylä: University of Jyväskylä and the Niilo Mäki Institute.
Masur E. F. (1982). Mothers’ responses to infants’ object-related gestures: influences on lexical development. Journal of Child Language 9, 2330.
Masur E. F., Flynn V. & Eichorst D. L. (2005). Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development. Journal of Child Language 23, 6391.
Masur E. F., Flynn V. & Lloyd C. A. (2013). Infants’ behaviors as antecedents and consequents of mothers’ responsive and directive utterances. First Language 33, 354–71.
Mundy P. & Gomes A. (1997). A skills approach to early language development: lessons from research on developmental disabilities. In Adamson L. B. & Romski M. A. (eds), Communication and language acquisition: discoveries from atypical development (pp. 469482). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Pine J. (1992). Maternal style at the early one-word stage: re-evaluating the stereotype of the directive mother. First Language 12, 169–86.
Rescorla L. & Fechnay T. (1996). Mother–child synchrony and communicative reciprocity in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39, 200–8.
Roberts M. Y. & Kaiser A. P. (2011). The effectiveness of parent-implemented language interventions: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 20, 180–99.
Rowe M. L. (2008). Child-directed speech: relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language 35, 185205.
Rowe M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development 83, 1762–74.
Silvén M., Niemi P. & Voeten M. J. M. (2002). Do maternal interaction and early language predict phonological awareness in 3- to 4-year-olds? Cognitive Development 17, 1133–55.
Smolak L. (1987). Child characteristics and maternal speech. Journal of Child Language 14, 481–92.
Snow C. E. (1995). Issues in the study of input: finetuning, universality, individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes. In Fletcher P. & MacWhinney B. (eds), The handbook of child language (pp. 180193). Oxford: Blackwell.
Soderstrom M. (2007). Beyond babytalk: re-evaluating the nature and content of speech input to preverbal infants. Developmental Review 27, 501–32.
Song L., Spier E. T. & Tamis-LeMonda C. S. (2014). Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children's language and cognitive development in low-income families. Journal of Child Language 41, 305–26.
Stoel-Gammon C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language 38, 134.
Stolt S., Haataja L., Lapinleimu H. & Lehtonen L. (2008). Early lexical development of Finnish children – a longitudinal study. First Language 28, 259–79.
Tamis-LeMonda C. S., Bornstein M. H. & Baumwell L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development 72, 748–67.
Tomasello M. & Farrar J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57, 1454–63.
van Balkom H., Verhoeven L. & van Weerdenburg M. (2010). Conversational behaviour of children with developmental language delay and their caretakers. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders 45, 295319.
Vigil D. C., Hodges J. & Klee T. (2005). Quantity and quality of parental language input to late-talking toddlers during play. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 21, 107–22.
Wetherby A. M., Cain D. H., Yonclas D. G. & Walker V. G. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multiword stage. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 31, 240–52.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Journal of Child Language
  • ISSN: 0305-0009
  • EISSN: 1469-7602
  • URL: /core/journals/journal-of-child-language
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 9
Total number of PDF views: 75 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 239 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between 22nd June 2017 - 21st November 2017. This data will be updated every 24 hours.