Skip to main content Accesibility Help
×
×
Home

Motherese, affect, and vocabulary development: dyadic communicative interactions in infants and toddlers

  • Shruti DAVE (a1), Ann M. MASTERGEORGE (a2) and Lesley B. OLSWANG (a3)
Abstract

Responsive parental communication during an infant's first year has been positively associated with later language outcomes. This study explores responsivity in mother–infant communication by modeling how change in guiding language between 7 and 11 months influences toddler vocabulary development. In a group of 32 mother–child dyads, change in early maternal guiding language positively predicted child language outcomes measured at 18 and 24 months. In contrast, a number of other linguistic variables – including total utterances and non-guiding language – did not correlate with toddler vocabulary development, suggesting a critical role of responsive change in infant-directed communication. We further assessed whether maternal affect during early communication influenced toddler vocabulary outcomes, finding that dominant affect during early mother–infant communications correlated to lower child language outcomes. These findings provide evidence that responsive parenting should not only be assessed longitudinally, but unique contributions of language and affect should also be concurrently considered in future study.

Copyright
Corresponding author
*Corresponding author. E-mail: sdave@ucdavis.edu
References
Hide All
Akhtar, N., Dunham, F., & Dunham, P. J. (1991). Directive interactions and early vocabulary development: the role of joint attentional focus. Journal of Child Language, 18(1), 41–9.
Albrecht, K., & Miller, L. G. (2001). Innovations: infant and toddler development. New York, NY: Gryphon.
Billingsley, F. F., White, O. R., & Munson, R. (1980). Procedural reliability: a rationale and an example. Behavioral Assessment, 2, 229–41.
Bloom, L., Tinker, E., & Scholnick, E. K. (2001). The intentionality model and language acquisition: engagement, effort, and the essential tension in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1101.
Bortfeld, H. (2004). Which came first: infants learning language or motherese? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 505–6.
Bretherton, I. (1995). A communication perspective on attachment relationships and internal working models. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(2/3), 310–29.
Brodsky, P., Waterfall, H. R., & Edelman, S. (2007). Characterizing motherese: on the computational structure of child-directed language. In McNamara, D. S. & Trafton, J. G. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Cognitive Science Society Conference (pp. 833–8). Cognitive Science Society.
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: a longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35, 207–20.
Carroll, D. W. (2007). Psychology of language. Australia: Wadsworth Publishing Company.
Cohen, S. E., & Beckwith, L. (1976). Maternal language in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 12(4), 371–2.
Dominey, P., & Dodane, C. (2004). Interdeterminancy in language acquisition: the role of child directed speech and joint attention. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17(2/3), 121–45.
Estes, K. G., Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., & Saffran, J. R. (2007). Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological Science, 18(3), 254–60.
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: user's guide and technical manual, 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Development, 64(3), 657–74.
Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behavior and Development, 10(3), 279–93.
Goldin-Meadow, S., Ping, R., Decatur, M. A., Larson, S., & Zinchenko, E. (2012). Gesture predicts readiness-to-learn in adults learning a complex spatial task. Cognitive Processing, 13, S31S34.
Hirotani, M., Stets, M., Striano, T., & Friederici, A. D. (2008). Joint attention helps infants learn new words: event-related potential evidence. NeuroReport, 20(6), 600–5.
Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input influence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11(6), F31F39.
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236–48.
Kitamura, C., & Burnham, D. (2003). Pitch and communicative intent in mother's speech: adjustments for age and sex in the first year. Infancy, 4(1), 85110.
Kitamura, C., & Lam, C. (2009). Age-specific preferences for infant-directed affective intent. Infancy, 14(1), 77100.
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T., & Pruitt, J. (2005). Early speech perception and later language development: implications for the ‘Critical Period’. Language Learning and Development, 1(3/4), 237–64.
Lalonde, C. E., & Werker, J. F. (1995). Cognitive influences on cross-language speech perception in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 18(4), 459–75.
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., & Swank, P. R. (2006). Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 627–42.
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Assel, M. A., & Vellet, S. (2001). Does early responsive parenting have a special importance for children's development or is consistency across early childhood necessary? Developmental Psychology, 37(3), 387403.
Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11(3), F9F16.
Masur, E. F., Flynn, V., & Eichorst, D. L. (2005). Maternal responsive and directive behaviours and utterances as predictors of children's lexical development. Journal of Child Language, 32(1), 6391.
McCathren, R. B., Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (1995). The role of directives in early language intervention. Journal of Early Intervention, 19(2), 91101.
Melby, J. N., Conger, R. D., Book, R., Rueter, M., Lucy, L., Repinski, D., & Scaramella, L. (1993). The Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales. Ames: Institute for Social and Behavioral Research, Iowa State University.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Faces and speech: intermodal processing of biologically relevant signals in infants and adults. In Lewkowicz, D. J. & Lickliter, R., (Eds.), The development of intersensory perception: comparative perspectives (pp. 335–69). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Mumme, D. L., & Fernald, A. (2003). The infant as onlooker: learning from emotional reactions observed in a television scenario. Child Development, 74(1), 221–37.
Mundy, P., & Newell, L. (2007). Attention, joint attention, and social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(5), 269–74.
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant's role in mother–infant communications. Journal of Child Language, 13(1), 1529.
Niwano, K., & Sugai, K. (2002). Intonation contour of Japanese maternal infant-directed speech and infant vocal response. Japanese Journal of Special Education, 39, 5968.
Nozadi, S. S., Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Bolnick, R., Eggum-Wilkens, N. D., Smith, C. L., Gaertner, B., Kupfer, A., & Sallquist, J. (2013). Prediction of toddlers’ expressive language from maternal sensitivity and toddlers’ anger expressions: a developmental perspective. Infant Behavior and Development, 36(4), 650–61.
Page, M., Wilhelm, M. S., Gamble, W. C., & Card, N. A. (2010). A comparison of maternal sensitivity and verbal stimulation as unique predictors of infant social–emotional and cognitive development. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(1), 101–10.
Pine, J. M. (1992). Maternal style at the early one-word stage: re-evaluating the stereotype of the directive mother. First Language, 12, 169–86.
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. London: Oxford University Press.
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 182–7.
Rowe., M. L., Raudenbush, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). The pace of vocabulary growth helps predict later vocabulary skill. Child Development, 83(2), 508–25.
Saint-Georges, C., Chetouani, M., Cassel, R., Apicella, F., Mahdhaoui, A., Muratori, F., & Cohen, D. (2013). Motherese in interaction: at the cross-road of emotion and cognition? (A systematic review). PloS one, 8(10), e78103.
Smith, M. (2010). Good parenting: making a difference. Early Human Development, 86(11), 689–93.
Snow, C. E. (1972). Mothers’ speech to children learning language. Child Development, 43(2), 549–65.
Soderstrom, M., & Morgan, J. L. (2007). Twenty-two-month-olds discriminate fluent from disfluent adult-directed speech. Developmental Science, 10, 641–53.
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development, 72(3), 748–67.
Tomasello, M. (2001). Perceiving intentions and learning words in the second year of life. In Bowerman, M. & Levinson, S. C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 132–58). Cambridge University Press.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675–90.
Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57(6), 1454–63.
Tomasello, M., Mannle, S., & Kruger, A. C. (1986). Linguistic environment of 1- to 2-year-old twins. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 169–76.
Tomasello, M., & Todd, J. (1983). Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4(12), 197211.
Werner, S., & Keller, E. (1994). Prosodic aspects of speech. In Keller, E. (Ed.), Fundamentals of speech synthesis and speech recognition: basic concepts, state of the art, the future challenges, (pp. 2340). Chickester: John Wiley and Sons Ltd.
Wooldridge, M. B., & Shapka, J. (2012). Playing with technology: mother–toddler interaction scores lower during play with electronic toys. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 211–18.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Journal of Child Language
  • ISSN: 0305-0009
  • EISSN: 1469-7602
  • URL: /core/journals/journal-of-child-language
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed