Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T22:01:55.979Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On children's uses of from, by and with in oblique noun phrases*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Eve V. Clark*
Affiliation:
Stanford University
Kathie L. Carpenter
Affiliation:
University of Colorado
*
Department of Linguistics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA.

Abstract

This study was designed to follow up children's early spontaneous uses of from to mark oblique agents by giving 40 children (aged 2; 5–6; 1), and 10 adults, grammatical and ungrammatical sentences containing from, with, and by to imitate and to repair. As predicted, children's imitations and repairs showed that (a) 2–year-olds produce from for agents, and with for instruments in imitation; and (b) as children get older, they shift to by for agents in their repairs, and keep from to mark locative sources. These findings support the hypothesis that when children first try to express oblique agents, before acquisition of conventional by, they choose from for this purpose because agents, as instigators of actions, are conceived of as the source of the action and its result.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported in part by a grant from the National Institute of Child Health & Human Development (5 R01 HD18908) and in part by the Sloan Foundation. We thank the staff and children of the City Kids Nursery School, San Francisco; Buttons 'n Bows Montessori School, Palo Alto; and Bing Nursery School, Stanford, for their help in this study. We are also indebted to Nancy Budwig and Herbert H. Clark for their comments on an earlier version of this paper.

References

REFERENCES

Bowerman, M. (1982). Starting to talk worse: clues to language acquisition from children's late speech errors. In Strauss, S. (ed.), U-shaped behavioral growth. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1983). Hidden meanings: the role of covert conceptual structures in children's development of language. In Rogers, D. R. & Sloboda, J. A. (eds), Acquisition of symbolic skills. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Barron, B. J. S. (1988). A thrower-button or a button-thrower? Children's judgements of grammatical and ungrammatical compound nouns. Linguistics 26. 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Carpenter, K. L. (1989). The notion of source in language acquisition. Language 65. 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
DeLancey, S. (1984). Notes on agentivity and causation. Studies in Language 8. 181213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, G. (1985). Mental spaces: aspects of meaning construction in natural language. Cambridge MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Gruber, J. S. (1976). Lexical structure in syntax and semantics. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Hochberg, J. G. (1986). Children's judgements of transitivity errors. Journal of Child Language 13. 317–34.Google Scholar
Horgan, D. M. (1975). Language development: a cross-methodological study. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Horgan, D. M. (1978). The development of the full passive. Journal of Child Language 5. 6580.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. S. (1983). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. (1978). Children's judgments of grammatical and ungrammatical irregular past-tense verbs. Child Development 49. 319–26.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M. P. & Abramovitch, R. (1975). How children understand full, truncated, and anomalous passives. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 14. 145–57.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1985). The child as linguistic icon-maker. In Haiman, J. (ed.), Iconicity in grammar. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. & Bever, T. G. (1982). Children use canonical sentence schemas: a cross-linguistic study of word order and inflections. Cognition 12. 229–65.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. & Welsh, C. A. (1973). Elicited imitation as a research tool in developmental psycholinguistics. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development. New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Traugott, E. C. (1974). Explorations in linguistic elaboration: language change, language acquisition, and the genesis of spatio-temporal terms. In Anderson, J. M. & Jones, C. (eds), Historical linguistics I. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar