Skip to main content Accesibility Help
×
×
Home

On the pragmatics of contrast*

  • Eve V. Clark (a1)
Abstract

In this paper, I review properties and consequences of the PRINCIPLE OF CONTRAST. This principle, which I have argued from the beginning has a pragmatic basis, captures facts about the inferences speakers and addressees make for both conventional and novel words. Along with a PRINCIPLE OF CONVENTIONALITY, it accounts for the pre-emption of novel words by well-established ones. And it holds just as much for morphology as it does for words and larger expressions. In short, Contrast has the major properties Gathercole (1989) proposed as characteristic of her alternative to Contrast.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Department of Linguistics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA.
Footnotes
Hide All
*

Preparation of this paper was supported in part by the Sloan Foundation. I would like to thank Dwight Bolinger, Willem J. Levelt, Brian MacWhinney and Dan I. Slobin for raising some of the issues I address here, and in particular Herbert H. Clark for discussion of and comments on an earlier draft.

This article concludes a discussion initiated in JCL by Clark (1988) and continued by Gathercole (1989). Ed.

Footnotes
References
Hide All
Aimard, P. (1975). Les jeux de mots des enfants. Villeurbanne: Simép Editions.
Aksu-Koc, A. & Slobin, D. I. (1985). The acquisition of Turkish. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.) The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. 1. The data. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
American Heritage (1970). The American heritage dictionary of the English language. New York: American Heritage Publishing Co. & Houghton Mifflin.
Anisfeld, M. & Gordon, M. (1968). On the psychophonological structure of English inflectional rules. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior 7. 973–9.
Au, Terry K.-F. & Markman, E. M. (1987). Acquiring new words via linguistic contrast. Cognitive Development 2. 217–36.
Berman, R. A. & Clark, E. V. (1989). Learning to use compounds for contrast: data from Hebrew. First Language 9. 247270.
Berman, R. A. & Sagi, Y. (1981). al darxey tetsurat hamilim ve xidushan bagil hats'ir [Word-formation processes and lexical innovations of young children]. Hebrew Computational Linguistics Bulletin 18. 3662.
Bolinger, D. L. (1956). Subjunctive -ra and -se: free variation? Hispania 39. 345–9.
Bolinger, D. L. (1967). Adjectives in English: attribution and predication. Lingua 18. 134.
Bolinger, D. L. (1968). Entailment and the meaning of structures. Glossa 2. 119–27.
Bolinger, D. L. (1971). The phrasal verb in English. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bolinger, D. L. (1972). That's that. The Hague: Mouton.
Bolinger, D. L. (1977). Meaning and form. London: Longman.
Borkin, A. (1984). Problems in form and function. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Bowerman, M. (1982). Evaluating competing linguistic models with language acquisition data: implications of developmental errors with causative verbs. Quaderni di Semantica 3. 566.
Bréal, M. (1897). Essai de sémantique. Paris: Hachette.
Budwig, N. (1986). Agentivity and control in early child language. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.
Budwig, N. (1989). The linguistic marking of agentivity and control in child language. Journal of Child Language 16. 263–84.
Bybee, J. L. & Slobin, D. I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58. 265–89.
Carey, S. (1978). The child as word-learner. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J. & Miller, G. A. (eds), Linguistic theory and psychological reality. Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
Carey, S. & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 15, 1729.
Cassell, . (1971). Cassell's modern guide to synonyms and related words. London: Cassell.
Cazden, C. B. (1968). The acquisition of noun and verb inflections. Child Development 39. 433–48.
Clark, E. V. (1980). Convention and contrast in acquiring the lexicon. Keynote Address, 12th Annual Child Language Research Forum, Stanford University (April). Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 19. 120.
Clark, E. V. (1981). Lexical innovations: how children learn to create new words. In Werner, Deutsch (ed.), The child's construction of language. London: Academic Press.
Clark, E. V. (1982). The young word-maker: a case study of innovation in the child's lexicon. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language acquisition: the state of the art. Cambridge: C.U.P.
Clark, E. V. (1983). Meanings and concepts. In Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (eds), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Cognitive development. New York: Wiley.
Clark, E. V. (1987). The Principle of Contrast: a constraint on acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Clark, E. V. (1988). On the logic of Contrast. Journal of Child Language 15. 317–35.
Clark, E. V. & Berman, R. A. (1984). Structure and use in the acquisition of word formation. Language 60. 542–90.
Clark, E. V. (1987). Types of linguistic knowledge: interpreting and producing compound nouns. Journal of Child Language 14. 547–67.
Clark, E. V. & Clark, H. H. (1979). When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55. 767811.
Clark, E. V. & Cohen, S. A. (1984). Productivity and memory for newly formed words. Journal of Child Language 11. 611–25.
Clark, E. V., Gelman, S. A. & Lane, N. M. (1985). Noun compounds and category structure in young children. Child Development 56. 8494.
Clark, E. V. & Hecht, B. F. (1982). Learning to coin agent and instrument nouns. Cognition 12. 124.
Clark, E. V., Hecht, B. F. & Mulford, R. C. (1986). Coining complex compounds in English: affixes and word order in acquisition. Linguistics 24. 729.
Clark, H. H. (1983). Making sense of nonce sense. In d'Arcais, G. Flores & Jarvella, R. (eds), The process of understanding language. New York: Wiley.
Clark, H. H. & Carlson, T. B. (1982). Speech acts and hearers' beliefs. In Smith, N. V. (ed.), Mutual knowledge. London: Academic Press.
Clark, H. H. & Marshall, C. R. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In Joshi, A., Webber, B. & Sag, I. (eds), Elements of discourse understanding. Cambridge: C.U.P.
Dockrell, J. E. (1981). The child's acquisition of unfamiliar words: an experimental study. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Stirling, Scotland.
Gathercole, V. C. (1989). Contrast: a semantic constraint? Journal of Child Language 16. 685702.
Gerhardt, J. (1988). From discourse to semantics: the development of verb morphology and forms of self-reference in the speech of a two-year-old. Journal of Child Language 15. 337–93
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Baduini, C. & Lavallee, A. (1985). What's in a word? The young child's predisposition to use lexical contrast. Paper presented at the Boston University Conference on Child Language, Boston, MA.
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. L. (eds), Syntax and semantics. Vol. 3. Speech acts. New York: Academic Press.
Gruber, J. S. (1976). Lexical structures in syntax and semantics. Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Haiman, J. (1980). The iconicity of grammar: isomorphism and motivation. Language 56. 515–40.
Horn, L. R. (1989). A natural history of negation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kuczaj, S. A. II (1977). The acqusition of regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16. 589600.
Lewis, David K. (1969). Convention: a philosophical study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lo Duca, M. G. (1988). Acquisizione del linguaggio e formazione dei ‘nomina agentis’ in italiano. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Padua, Italy.
Lyamina, G. M. (1960). Razvitie ponimaniya rechi u deteǐ vtorogo goda zhizni. Voprosy Psikhologii 3. 106–21.
McCawley, J. D. (1978). Conversational implicature and the lexicon. In Cole, P. (ed.), Syntax and semantics, vol. 9. Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.
MacWhinney, B. (1973). How Hungarian children learn to speak. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.
Markman, E. M. & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology 20. 121–57.
Mulford, R. C. (1983). On the acquisition of derivational morphology in Icelandic: learning about -ari. Islensk mál og almenn málfraedi 5. 104–25.
Paul, H. (1898). Principien der Sprachgeschichte (3rd ed.). Halle: Max Niemeyer Verlag.
Room, A. (1981). Room's dictionary of distinguishables. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Saussure, Ferdinand de (1919/1968). Cours de linguistique générale (publié par Charles Bally & Albert Sechehaye). Paris: Payot.
Slobin, Dan I. (1985 a). Developmental paths between form and meaning: crosslinguistic and diachronic perspectives. Keynote Address, 10th Annual Boston University Conference on Child Language, Boston, MA.
Slobin, Dan I. (ed.) (1985 b). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Stern, C. & Stern, W. (1928). Die Kindersprache: eine psychologische und sprachtheoretische Untersuchung (2nd ed.). Leipzig: Barth.
Toivainen, J. (1980). Inflectional affixes used by Finnish-speaking children aged 1–3 years. Helsinki: Suomilaisen Kirjallisuuden Seura.
Zipf, G. K. (1949). Human behavior and the principle of least effort. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Journal of Child Language
  • ISSN: 0305-0009
  • EISSN: 1469-7602
  • URL: /core/journals/journal-of-child-language
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed