Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home

The role of input frequency and semantic transparency in the acquisition of verb meaning: evidence from placement verbs in Tamil and Dutch*

  • BHUVANA NARASIMHAN (a1) and MARIANNE GULLBERG (a2)
Abstract

We investigate how Tamil- and Dutch-speaking adults and four- to five-year-old children use caused posture verbs (‘lay/stand a bottle on a table’) to label placement events in which objects are oriented vertically or horizontally. Tamil caused posture verbs consist of morphemes that individually label the causal and result subevents (nikka veyyii ‘make stand’; paDka veyyii ‘make lie’), occurring in situational and discourse contexts where object orientation is at issue. Dutch caused posture verbs are less semantically transparent: they are monomorphemic (zetten ‘set/stand’; leggen ‘lay’), often occurring in contexts where factors other than object orientation determine use. Caused posture verbs occur rarely in Tamil input corpora; in Dutch input, they are used frequently. Elicited production data reveal that Tamil four-year-olds use infrequent placement verbs appropriately whereas Dutch children use high-frequency placement verbs inappropriately even at age five. Semantic transparency exerts a stronger influence than input frequency in constraining children's verb meaning acquisition.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Address for correspondence: Bhuvana Narasimhan, Department of Linguistics, Hellems 290, 295 UCB, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO 80309-0295, USA. tel: 303-492-8456; email: Bhuvana.Narasimhan@colorado.edu
Marianne Gullberg, Centre for Languages and Literature, Lund University, PB Box 201, 221 00 Lund, Sweden. tel: +46-46-222 0389; email: marianne.gullberg@ling.lu.se
Footnotes
Hide All
[*]

We gratefully acknowledge the cooperation of the teachers and students of the Ramakrishna Mission Tamil Medium School (Chennai, India) and Kindercentrum Dribbel (Molenhoek, the Netherlands). We are also grateful for funding from the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics. We wish to express our thanks to Judith Bindels, Pauline Chew, Bregje Esmeijer, Anke Jolink, Femke Uijtdewilligen, Arna Van Doorn, Shanmugam Mohan and R. Devi for help with the data collection and analysis, and Melissa Bowerman, Asifa Majid and Leah Roberts for comments on the design and analysis of the study. Our thanks also go to the reviewers of this paper who provided us with valuable suggestions. Any remaining errors are solely ours.

Footnotes
References
Hide All
Asher, R. E. (1985). Tamil. London: Routledge.
Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R. & Gulikers, L. (1995). The CELEX Lexical Database (CD-ROM). Philadelphia, PA: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
Bomba, P. C. (1984). The development of orientation categories between 2 and 4 months of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 37, 609–36.
Bowerman, M. (1978). Systematizing semantic knowledge: Changes over time in the child's organization of word meaning. Child Development 49, 977–87.
Brown, P. (2001). Learning to talk about motion UP and DOWN in Tzeltal: Is there a language-specific bias for verb learning? In Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. C. (eds), Language acquisition and conceptual development, 512–43. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Choi, S. & Bowerman, M. (1991). Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition 41 83–121.
Clark, E. V. (2004) Language builds on cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, 472–78.
Essock, E. A. & Siqueland, E. R. (1981). Discrimination of orientation by human infants. Perception 10, 245–53.
Gathercole, V. (1982). Decrements in children's responses to big and tall: A reconsideration of the potential cognitive and semantic causes. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 34, 156–73.
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In Kuczaj, S. A. (ed.), Language development: vol. 2: Language, thought and culture, 301334. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Howell, D. (2002). Statistical methods for psychology, 5th edn.Pacific Grove, CA: Duxbury.
Huttenlocher, J. (1974). The origins of language comprehension. In Solso, R. L. (ed.), Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola symposium, 331–68. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.
Krauss, R. M. & Glucksberg, S. (1977). Social and nonsocial speech. Scientific American 236, 100105.
Lemmens, M. (2006). Caused posture: Experiential patterns emerging from corpus research. In Stefanowitsch, A. & Gries, S. (eds), Corpora in cognitive linguistics. Corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis, 263–98. Berlin: Mouton.
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, 3rd edn.Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Narasimhan, R. (1981). Modeling language behavior. Berlin: Springer.
Plumert, J. M., Ewert, K. & Spear, S. J. (1995). The early development of children's communication about nested spatial relations. Child Development 66, 959–69.
Quinn, P. C. & Bomba, P. C. (1986). Evidence of a general category of oblique orientations in four-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 42, 345–54.
Senft, G. & Smits, R. (eds) 2000. The Annual Report of the Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, 2000. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.
Slobin, D. I. (1985). Cross-linguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition, vol. 2, 1157–256. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Rowland, C. F. (2002). ‘Going, going, gone’: The acquisition of the verb ‘go’. Journal of Child Language 29, 783811.
Theakston, A. L., Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M. & Rowland, C. F. (2004). Semantic generality, input frequency and the acquisition of syntax. Journal of Child Language 31, 6999.
Thomson, J. R. & Chapman, R. S. (1977). Who is ‘Daddy’ revisited: The status of two-year-olds' over-extended words in use and comprehension. Journal of Child Language 4, 359–75.
Wijnen, F. & Bol, G. (1993). The escape from the optional infinitive stage. In de Boer, A., de Jong, J. & Landeweerd, R. (eds), Language and cognition, vol. 3, 239–48. Groningen: Department of Linguistics, University of Groningen.
Yule, G. (1997). Referential communication tasks. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Journal of Child Language
  • ISSN: 0305-0009
  • EISSN: 1469-7602
  • URL: /core/journals/journal-of-child-language
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed