Skip to main content
×
Home
    • Aa
    • Aa

Six-year-olds’ learning of novel words through addressed and overheard speech*

  • ANNELEEN BODERÉ (a1) and KOEN JASPAERT (a1)
Abstract
Abstract

Recent research indicates that infants can learn novel words equally well through addressed speech as through overhearing two adult experimenters. The current study examined to which extent six-year-old children learn from overhearing opportunities in regular kindergarten classroom practices. Fifty-three children (M age = 5;6) were exposed to a story with twelve novel words in three different conditions. In the Addressed condition, children were directly addressed to listen to the story. In the Overhearing Classroom, the children were assigned to a task within earshot of the children of the Addressed condition. In the Overhearing Two Adults condition, the experimenter told the story to another adult. The results showed that the Addressees learned equally well as the Overhearers of Two Adults. However, in the Overhearing Classroom condition children learned significantly fewer words compared to the two other conditions. Different hypotheses are offered to explain the relative success of overhearing two adults compared to overhearing classroom interactions.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Address for correspondence: Anneleen Boderé, Department of Linguistics, KU Leuven, Blijde-Inkomststraat 21, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. e-mail: anneleen.bodere@gmail.com
Footnotes
Hide All
[*]

The authors would like to thank all the school directors, teachers, parents, and children who participated in this study. Special thanks go to An Carbonez and Eline Zenner for their additional feedback on design and statistical analysis, and Ditte Kimps, Eline Zenner, Freek Van de Velde, Goedele Vandommele, Lies Strobbe, and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on earlier versions of this paper. This research was supported by a grant from Research Foundation Flanders (FWO) awarded to the first author.

Footnotes
References
Hide All
AkhtarN. (2005). The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science 8, 199209.
AkhtarN., JipsonJ. & CallananM. A. (2001). Learning words through overhearing. Child Development 72, 416–30.
BloomL. (1998). Language acquisition in its developmental context. In Kuhn D., Siegler R. S. & Damon W. (eds), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception, and language, 309–70. New York: Wiley.
BruckM. & CeciS. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children's memory. Annual Review of Psychology 50, 419–39.
CazdenB. C. (2001). Classroom discourse: the language of teaching and learning, 2nd ed. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
ChavajayP. & RogoffB. (2002). Schooling and traditional collaborative social organization of problem solving by Mayan mothers and children. Developmental Psychology 38, 5566.
ClarkH. & SchaeferE. (1992). Dealing with overhearers. In Clark H. (ed.), Arenas of language use, 248–74. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
CucchiariniC. & JaspaertK. (1996). Taalvaardigheidstoets Aanvang Laatste Kleuterklas. Leuven: Steunpunt Nederlands als Tweede Taal.
EatonW. O. & EnnsL. R. (1986). Sex differences in human motor activity level. Psychological Bulletin 100, 1928.
Else-questN. M., HydeJ. S., GoldsmithH. H. & HulleC. A. V. (2006). Gender differences in temperament: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 132, 3372.
FloorP. & AkhtarN. (2006). Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations? Infancy 9, 327–39.
FosterE. K. & HundA. M. (2012). The impact of scaffolding and overhearing on young children's use of the spatial terms between and middle . Journal of Child Language 39, 338–64.
Fox TreeJ. E. (1999). Listening in on monologues and dialogues. Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal 27(1), 3553.
GampeA., LiebalK. & TomaselloM. (2012). Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearing. First Language 32, 385–97.
HuttenlocherJ., HaightW., BrykA., SeltzerM. & LyonsT. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.
JaswalV. & NeelyL. (2006). Adults don't always know best: preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science 17(9), 757–8.
KaushanskayaM., GrossM. & BuacM. (2013). Gender differences in child word learning. Learning and Individual Differences 27, 82–9.
KaushanskayaM., MarianV. & YooJ. (2011). Gender differences in adult word learning. Acta Psychologica 137, 2435.
KramerJ. H., DelisD. C., KaplanE., O'DonnellL. & PrifiteraA. (1997). Developmental sex differences in verbal learning. Neuropsychology 11, 577–84.
LampinenJ. M. & SmithV. L. (1995). The incredible (and sometimes incredulous) child witness: child eyewitnesses’ sensitivity to source credibility cues. Journal of Applied Psychology 80(5), 621–7.
MaccobyE. E. & JacklinC. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Martinez-SussmannC., AkhtarN., DiesendruckG. & MarksonL. (2011). Orienting to third-party conversations. Journal of Child Language 38, 273–96.
MehanH. (1979). Learning lessons: social organization in the classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
NippoldM. (2006). Later language development: school-age children, adolescents, and young adults, 3rd ed. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Oshima-TakaneY. (1988). Children learn from speech not addressed to them: the case of personal pronouns. Journal of Child Language 15, 95108.
Oshima-TakaneY., GoodzE. & DerevenskyJ. L. (1996). Birth order effects on early language development: Do secondborn children learn from overheard speech? Child Development 67, 621–34.
PhilipsS. (1972). Participant structures and communicative competence: Warm Springs children in community and classroom. In Cazden C., John V. & Hymes D. (eds), Functions of language in the classroom (pp. 370394). New York, NY: Teacher College Press.
PhilipsS. (1983). The invisible culture: communication in classroom and community on the Warm Springs Indian reservation. New York, NY: Longman.
RojasR. & IglesiasA. (2013). The language growth of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Child Development 84, 630–46.
ShneidmanL. A., BureshJ. S., ShimpiP. M., Knight-SchwarzJ. & WoodwardA. L. (2009). Social experience, social attention and word learning in an overhearing paradigm. Language Learning and Development 5, 266–81.
TaylorM., CartwrightB. S. & BowdenT. (1991). Perspective taking and theory of mind: Do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers’ knowledge? Child Development 62(6), 1334–51.
UllmanM. T., MirandaR. A. & TraversM. L. (2008). Sex differences in the neurocognition of language. In Becker J. B., Berkley K. J., Geary N., Hampson E. Herman J. P. & Young E.. (eds), Sex on the brain: from genes to behavior, 291309. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Van HoutteM. (2011). So where's the teacher in school effects research? The impact of teachers’ beliefs, culture, and behavior on equity and excellence in education. In Van den Branden K., Van Avermaet P. & Van Houtte M. (eds), Equity and excellence in education: towards maximal learning opportunities for all students, 7595. New York, NY: Routledge.
VoyerD. & VoyerS. D. (2014). Gender differences in scholastic achievement: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 140 1174–204.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Journal of Child Language
  • ISSN: 0305-0009
  • EISSN: 1469-7602
  • URL: /core/journals/journal-of-child-language
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 16
Total number of PDF views: 116 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 290 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between 17th October 2016 - 22nd October 2017. This data will be updated every 24 hours.