Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-15T00:38:50.531Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Syntactic bootstrapping with clausal complements of adjectives

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2023

Kristen SYRETT*
Affiliation:
Rutgers The State University of New Jersey – Department of Linguistics 18 Seminary Place, New Brunswick, New Jersey 08901-8554 United States

Extract

Like verbs, adjectives pose a challenge to the young word learner in that some – like red, round, rough, or rectangular – map onto properties that are detectable through the senses, while others – like ready, reasonable, or required – express abstract properties that have no reliable, physical correlate. Even for those adjectives whose properties are observable, how does a child know that one particular property is being highlighted above all others? The physical environment alone will not suffice. Just as with verbs, the child learning adjectives is faced with an inherent indeterminacy of meaning, which can only be resolved through the incorporation of cues originating from multiple sources.

Type
Invited Commentary
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 40, (Serial No. 160).Google Scholar
Braunwald, S. R. (1971). Mother-child communication: the function of maternal-language input. Word, 27(1-3), 2850.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Harrigan, K., Hacquard, V., & Lidz, J. (2019). Hope for syntactic bootstrapping. Language, 95(4), 642682.Google Scholar
Karttunen, L. (1977). Syntax and semantics of questions. Linguistics and Philosophy, 1(1), 344Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Third Edition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent–child discourse. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children’s language , Vol. 4 (pp. 128). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Suppes, P. (1974). The semantics of children’s language. American Psychologist, 29(2), 103114.Google Scholar