Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-25wd4 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T15:08:17.165Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Use of pointing in parent-child interactions by hearing children of deaf and hearing parents: A follow-up from 1- to 3-years of age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 June 2023

Laura KANTO*
Affiliation:
Department of Language and Communication studies, University of Jyväskylä Niilo Mäki Institute, Jyväskylä
Minna LAAKSO
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Logopedics, University of Helsinki
Kerttu HUTTUNEN
Affiliation:
Faculty of Humanities/Research Unit of Logopedics, University of Oulu Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, University Hospital of Oulu Medical Research Center Oulu
*
*Corresponding author. Laura Kanto, Department of Language and Communication studies, University of Jyväskylä. Email: laura.kanto@jyu.fi

Abstract

Pointing plays a significant role in communication and language development. However, in spoken languages pointing has been viewed as a non-verbal gesture, whereas in sign languages, pointing is regarded to represent a linguistic unit of language. This study compared the use of pointing between seven bilingual hearing children of deaf parents (Kids of Deaf Adults [KODAs]) interacting with their deaf parents and five hearing children interacting with their hearing parents. Data were collected in 6-month intervals from the age of 1;0 to 3;0. Pointing frequency among the deaf parents and KODAs was significantly higher than among the hearing parents and their children. In signing dyads pointing frequency remained stable, whereas in spoken dyads it decreased during the follow-up. These findings suggested that pointing is a fundamental element of parent-child interaction, regardless of the language, but is guided by the modality, gestural and linguistic features of the language in question.

Type
Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bonvillian, J. D., Orlansky, M. D., & Folven, R. J. (1994). Early sign language acquisition: Implications for theories of language acquisition. In Volterra, V., & Erting, C. J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 219232). Washington DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Bosworth, R. G., & Stone, A. (2021). Rapid development of perceptual gaze control in hearing native signing infants and children. Developmental Science, 24(4), e13086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, R., Singleton, J. L., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2020). Enhanced gaze‐following behavior in Deaf infants of Deaf parents. Developmental Science, 23(2), e12900. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12900CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Butcher, C., Mylander, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1991). Displaced communication in a self-styled gesture system: Pointing at the non-present. Cognitive Development, 6(3), 315342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartmill, E. A., Hunsicker, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Pointing and naming are not redundant: children use gesture to modify nouns before they modify nouns in speech. Developmental Psychology, 50(6), 1660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd Ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta‐analysis. Developmental Review, 30(4), 352366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooperrider, K., Fenlon, J., Keane, J., Brentari, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2021). How pointing is integrated into language: Evidence from speakers and signers. Frontiers in Communication, 6, 567774. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2021.567774CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cormier, K., Schembri, A., & Woll, B. (2013). Pronouns and pointing in sign languages. Lingua, 137, 230247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimitrova, N., & Moro, C. (2013). Common ground on object use associates with caregivers’ gestures. Infant Behavior and Development, 36(4), 618626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, S., Fletcher, P., Garman, M., Hughes, A., Letts, C., & Sinka, I. (1997). Reynell Developmental Language Scales III . Third Edition. In Kortesmaa, M., Heimonen, K., Merikoski, H., Warma, M-L, & Varpela, V. (Eds.), Reynellin kielellisen kehityksen testi. Helsinki: Psykologien Kustannus.Google Scholar
Ege, B. (1998). Språklig Test I, Revision 1998. Herning: Special pedagogisk forlag A/S. Transl. Pirkanmaan puheterapeuttien testiryhmä (corrected version 1998). Sanaston käytön testi. Helsinki: Early Learning.Google Scholar
Fenlon, J., Cooperrider, K., Keane, J., Brentari, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2019). Comparing sign language and gestures: Insights from pointing. Glossa, 4, 126. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl499CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrara, L. (2020). Some interactional functions of finger pointing in signed language conversations. Glossa, 5, 88, 126. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.993CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fieldsteel, Z., Bottoms, A., & Lieberman, A. M. (2020). Nouns and verbs in parent input in American Sign Language during interaction among deaf dyads, Language Learning and Development, 16 ( 4), 351363, DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2020.1784737CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franco, F., Perucchini, P., & March, B. (2009). Is infant initiation of joint attention by pointing affected by type of interaction? Social Development, 18(1), 5176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., & Iverson, J. M. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10, 778785. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00636.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwyn, S. W., & Acredolo, L. P. (1998). Encouraging symbolic gestures: A new perspective on the relationship between gesture and speech. In Iverson, J., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (Eds.), The balance between gesture and speech in childhood (pp. 6173). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Harris, M. (2001). It’s all a matter of timing: Sign visibility and sign reference in deaf and hearing mothers of 18-month-old children. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 6(3), 177185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, M., & Mohay, H. (1997). Learning to look in the right place: A comparison of attentional behavior in deaf children with deaf and hearing mothers. The Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2(2), 95103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatzopoulou, M. (2008). The acquisition if reference to self and others in Greek Sign Language: From pointing gesture to pronominal pointing signs (Doctoral dissertation). University of Stockholm, Stockholm.Google Scholar
Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E., & Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother–child interaction. Cognitive Development, 14, 5775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jantunen, T. (2017). Fixed and NOT free: Revisiting the order of the main clausal constituents in Finnish Sign Language from a corpus perspective. SKY Journal of Linguistics, 30, 137149.Google Scholar
Jantunen, T., & Pippuri, O., & Wainio, T., & Puupponen, A., & Laaksonen, J. (2016). Annotated video corpus of FinSL with Kinect and computer-vision data. In Proceedings of the 7th Workshop on the Representation and Processing of Sign Languages: Corpus Mining, 93100. Paris: ELRA.Google Scholar
Johnston, T. (2013a). Towards a comparative semiotics of pointing actions in signed and spoken languages. Gesture 13(2), 109142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, T. (2013b). Formational and functional characteristics of pointing signs in a corpus of Auslan (Australian sign language): are the data sufficient to posit a grammatical class of ‘pronouns’ in Auslan? Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 9(1), 109159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanto, L., Laakso, M., & Huttunen, K. (2015). Differentiation in language and gesture use during early bilingual development of hearing children of deaf parents. Bilingualism, 18(4), 769788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanto, L., Laakso, M. L., & Huttunen, K. (2017). Use of code-mixing by young hearing children of Deaf parents. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 20(5), 947964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., Weintraub, S., & Segal, O. (2001). Boston Naming Test. Second Edition. transl. Laine, M., Koivuselkä-Sallinen, P., Hänninen, R., & Niemi, J. (1997). Bostonin nimentätesti. Helsinki: Psykologien Kustannus.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (2008). Some reflections on the relationship between ‘gesture’ and ‘sign’. Gesture 8(3). 348366. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.8.3.05kenCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (2016). Gesture and sign. In Allan, K. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of linguistics (pp. 3346). Accessed on 7 Oct 2021. https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9781315718453.ch3Google Scholar
Kita, S. (2003). Pointing: A foundational building block of human communication. In Kita, S. (Ed.). Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 916). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatter-Folmer, J., van Hout, R., Kolen, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2006). Language development in deaf children’s interactions with deaf and hearing adult: A Dutch longitudinal study. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 11(2), 238251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koulidobrova, E., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2016). A ‘point’of inquiry: The case of the (non-) pronominal ix in asl. The Impact of Pronominal Form on Interpretation, 125, 221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laakso, M., Helasvuo, M.-L., & Savinainen-Makkonen, T. (2010). Children’s early actions in learning language: A study of proto-words and pointing gestures in interaction between one-year-old child and parent. SKY Journal of Linguistics, 23, 199226. http://www.linguistics.fi/julkaisut/SKY2010/Laakso_Helasvuo_Savinainen-Makkonen_n.pdfGoogle Scholar
Lausberg, H., & Sloetjes, H. (2009). Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES-ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 41, 841849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddell, S. (2003). Grammar, gesture, and meaning in American Sign Language. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieberman, A. M., Hatrak, M., & Mayberry, R. I. (2014). Learning to look for language: Development of joint attention in young deaf children. Language Learning and Development, 10(1), 1935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lillo-Martin, D., de Quadros, R. M., Chen Pichler, D., & Fieldsteel, Z. (2014). Language choice in bimodal bilingual development. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing. Cognitive Development, 26(1), 1629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lüke, C., Grimminger, A., Rohlfing, K. J., Liszkowski, U., & Ritterfeld, U. (2017). In infants’ hands: Identification of preverbal infants at risk for primary language delay. Child Development, 88(2), 484492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyytinen, P. (1999). Varhaisten kommunikaation ja keilen kehityksen arviointimenetelmä [Assessment method of hearly communication and language development]. Jyväskylä: Niilo Mäki Instituutti, & Jyväskylän yliopiston lapsitutkimuskeskus.Google Scholar
Meier, R. P., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2013). The points of language. Humana. mente, 24, Journal of Philosophical Studies, 24, 151176.Google ScholarPubMed
Meyer, M., & Baldwin, D. A. (2013). Pointing as a socio-pragmatic cue to particular vs. generic reference, Language Learning and Development, 9(3), 245265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgenstern, A. (2014). Children’s multimodal language development. In Fräncke, C. (Ed.), Manual of language acquisition (pp. 123142). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgenstern, A., Caët, S., Collombel-Leroy, M., Limousin, F., & Blondel, M. (2010). From gesture to sign and from gesture to word: Pointing in deaf and hearing children. Gesture, 10(2–3), 172202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Mayberry, R., & Genesee, F. (1999). Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 514526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norlund, S. (2019). Agent defocusing in two-participant clauses in Finnish Sign Language. Glossa: A journal of general linguistics 4(1). 127. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.801Google Scholar
Özçalıskan, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognition Process, 24(2), 190217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petitto, L. A. (1988). “Language” in the pre-linguistic child. In Kessel, F. (ed.), The development of language and language researchers: Essays in honor of Roger Brown (pp. 187221). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A. (1994). The transition from gesture to symbol in American Sign Language. In Volterra, V., & Erting, C.J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 153161). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Holowka, S., Sergio, L. E., Levy, B., & Ostry, D. J. (2004). Baby hands that move to the rhythm of language: hearing babies acquiring sign languages babble silently on the hands. Cognition, 93(1), 4373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Katerelos, M., Levy, B., Gauna, K., Tétreault, K., & Ferraro, V. (2001). Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: Implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 28(2), 453496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petitto, L. A., & Marentette, P. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251(5000), 14931496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 182187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salonen, J., Wainio, T., Kronqvist, A., & Keränen, J. (2018). Suomen viittomakielten korpusprojektin (CFinSL) annotointiohjeet 1. versio [Annotation Guidelines of the Corpus of Finnish Sign Languages (CFinSL)]. Kieli ja viestintätieteiden laitos Jyväskylä: University of Jyväskylä.Google Scholar
Sarimski, K. (2002). Analysis of intentional communication in severely handicapped children with Cornelia-de-Lange Syndrome. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35(6), 483500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schembri, A., Cormier, K., & Fenlon, J. (2018). Indicating verbs as typologically unique constructions: Reconsidering verb ‘agreement’ in sign languages. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 3(1). 89. 140. DOI: https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.468CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, P. E., & Harris, M. (2006). Patterns and effects of language input to deaf infants and toddlers from deaf and hearing mothers. In Schick, B., Marschark, M., & Spencer, P. E. (Eds.), Advances in the sign language development of deaf children (pp. 71101). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child development, 78(3), 705722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Bogaerde, B. (2000). Input and interaction in deaf families. Doctoral dissertation, University of Amsterdam. Utrecht: LOT. Retrieved October 1, 2020 from: https://www.lotpublications.nl/Documents/035_fulltext.pdfCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Bogaerde, B., & Baker, A. E. (2008). Bimodal language acquisition in Kodas. In Bishop, M., & Hicks, S. L. (Eds.), Hearing, mother father deaf. Hearing people in deaf families (pp. 99131). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
van Herreweghe, M. (2002). Turn-taking mechanisms and active participation in meetings with deaf and hearing participants in Flanders. In Lucas, C. (Ed.), Turn-taking, fingerspelling, and contact in signed languages. Vol. 8 (pp. 73103). Washington D.C.: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Vihman, M. M., & McCune, L. (1994). When is a word a word? Journal of Child Language, 21(3), 517542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshida, H., Cirino, P., Mire, S. S., Burling, J. M., & Lee, S. (2020). Parent’s gesture adaptations to children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Language, 47(1), 205224. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000497CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed