Skip to main content
×
×
Home

Young children's flexible use of semantic cues to word meanings: converging evidence of individual and age differences*

  • GEDEON O. DEÁK (a1) and GAYATHRI NARASIMHAM (a2)
Abstract

A new test of children's flexible use of semantic cues for word learning extended previous results. In Experiment 1, three- to five-year-olds (N=51) completed two tests of interpreting several novel words for the same stimulus arrays. Within-sentence phrasal cues implied different stimulus referent properties. Children's cue-using flexibility in the new Flexible Induction of Meanings [Words for Animates] test (FIM-An) was strongly correlated with an established test (Flexible Induction of Meanings [Words for Objects]; Deák, 2000). Individual children showed between-test consistency in using cues to flexibly assign words to different referent properties. There were large individual differences, as well as limited age differences, in the distribution of flexible and inflexible response patterns. The comprehensibility of specific cues, and perceptual salience of specific properties, explained much of the variance. Proportions of flexible and inflexible patterns shifted with age. Experiment 2 replicated these results in N=36 three- and four-year-olds, using a modified FIM-An with more distinctive cues.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Address for correspondence: Gedeon O. Deák, Dept. of Cognitive Science, 9500 Gilman Dr., University of California, San Diego, La Jolla CA 92093-0515, USA.
Footnotes
Hide All
[*]

This research was supported by a Hellman fellowship and by National Science Foundation award BCS-0092027 to the first author. Thanks to the children who participated, and to Sarah Creel, Anna Holt, and Jenny Nowinski for comments on an earlier draft.

Footnotes
References
Hide All
Akhtar, N., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (1996). The role of discourse novelty in early word learning. Child Development 67, 635–45.
Arnold, J. E., Brown-Schmidt, S. & Trueswell, J. (2007). Children's use of gender and order-of-mention during pronoun comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes 22(4), 527–65.
Arrington, C. M., Logan, G. D. & Schneider, D. W. (2007). Separating cue encoding from target processing in the explicit task-cuing procedure: Are there ‘true’ task switch effects? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33(3), 484502.
Baldwin, D. A. (1989). Priorities in children's expectations about object label reference: form over color. Child Development 60, 1291–306.
Barlow, W. D. & Summers, I. (1979). Barlow's guide to extraterrestrials. New York: Workman.
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G. & McCaslin, E. S. (1983). Vocabulary development: all contexts are not created equal. Elementary School Journal 83(3), 177–81.
Behrend, D. A., Harris, L. L. & Cartwright, K. B. (1995). Morphological cues to verb meaning: verb inflections and the initial mapping of verb meanings. Journal of Child Language 22, 89106.
Brooks, P. J., Hanauer, J. B., Padowska, B. & Rosman, H. (2003). The role of selective attention in preschoolers' rule use in a novel dimensional card sort. Cognitive Development 117, 121.
Cain, K. & Oakhill, J. V. (1999). Inference making ability and its relation to comprehension failure in young children. Reading and Writing 11(5), 489503.
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. V. & Lemmon, K. (2004). Individual differences in the inference of word meanings from context: the influence of reading comprehension, vocabulary knowledge, and memory capacity. Journal of Educational Psychology 96(4), 671–81.
Campbell, D. T. & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin 56(2), 81105.
Campbell, R. N. & Bowe, T. B. (1983). Text and context in early language comprehension. In Donaldson, M., Grieve, R. & Pratt, C. (eds), Early childhood development and education, 115–26. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
Cartwright, K. B. (2002). Cognitive development and reading: the relation of reading-specific multiple classification skill to reading comprehension in elementary school children. Journal of Educational Psychology 94(1), 5663.
Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F. & De Sather, J. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Developmental Psychology 37(5), 715–29.
Chevalier, N. & Blaye, A. (2009). Setting goals to switch between tasks: effects of cue transparency on children's cognitive flexibility. Developmental Psychology 45(3), 782–97.
Corrigan, R., Halpern, E., Aviezer, O. & Goldblatt, A. (1981). The development of three spatial concepts: in, on, under. International Journal of Behavioral Development 4(4), 403–19.
Coulson, S. & Kutas, M. (2001). Getting it: human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders. Neuroscience Letters 316(2), 71–4.
Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C. & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia 44(11), 2037–78.
Deák, G. O. (2000). The growth of flexible problem solving: preschool children use changing verbal cues to infer multiple word meanings. Journal of Cognition and Development 1(2), 157–91.
Deák, G. O. (2003). The development of cognitive flexibility and language abilities. In Kail, R. (ed.), Advances in child development and behavior, vol. 31, 271327. San Diego: Academic Press.
Deák, G. O. & Narasimham, G. (2003). Is perseveration caused by inhibition failure? Evidence from preschool children's inferences about word meanings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 86(3), 194222.
Deák, G. O., Ray, S. D. & Brenneman, K. (2003). Children's perseverative appearance–reality errors are related to emerging language skills. Child Development 74(3), 944–64.
Deák, G. O., Ray, S. D. & Pick, A. D. (2004). Effects of age, reminders, and task difficulty on young children's rule-switching flexibility. Cognitive Development 19, 385400.
DeMarie, D. (2001). A trip to the zoo: children's words and photographs. Early Childhood Research & Practice 3(1), 126.
Dickinson, D. K. (1988). Learning names for materials: factors constraining and limiting hypotheses about word meaning. Cognitive Development 3, 1535.
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. (1981). PPVT-R Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Ellefson, M. R., Shapiro, L. R. & Chater, N. (2006). Asymmetrical switch costs in children. Cognitive Development 21(2), 108–30.
Ellis Weismer, S. & Hesketh, L. (1993). The influence of prosodic and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 1013–25.
Friedman, O. & Neary, K. R. (2008). Determining who owns what: do children infer ownership from first possession? Cognition 107(3), 829–49.
Gelman, S. A. (2003). The essential child: origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gelman, S. A., Coley, J. D., Rosengren, K. S., Hartman, E., Pappas, A. & Keil, F. C. (1998). Beyond labeling: the role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 63(1), 1158.
Gelman, S. A. & Heyman, G. D. (1999). Carrot-eaters and creature-believers: the effects of lexicalization on children's inferences about social categories. Psychological Science 10(6), 489–93.
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J. & Diamond, A. (1994). The relationship between cognition and action: performance of children 31/2 –7 years old on a Stroop-like day–night test. Cognition 53, 129–53.
Goodman, J. C., McDonough, L. & Brown, N. B. (1998). The role of semantic context and memory in the acquisition of novel nouns. Child Development 69(5), 1330–44.
Gray, S. (2004). Word learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: predictors and poor learners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 47(5), 1117–32.
Grieve, R., Hoogenraad, R. & Murray, D. (1977). On the young child's use of lexis and syntax in understanding locative instructions. Cognition 5(3), 235–50.
Hay, D. F. (2006). Yours and mine: toddlers' talk about possessions with familiar peers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 24(1), 3952.
Huxham, M., Welsh, A., Berry, A. & Templeton, S. (2006). Factors influencing primary school children's knowledge of wildlife. Journal of Biological Education 41(1), 912.
Jacques, S. & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). On the possible roots of cognitive flexibility. In Homer, B. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (eds), The development of social cognition and communication, 5381. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Johnson, K. E. & Mervis, C. B. (1994). Microgenetic analysis of first steps in children's acquisition of expertise on shorebirds. Developmental Psychology 30(3), 418–35.
Jones, S. S., Smith, L. B. & Landau, B. (1991). Object properties and knowledge in early lexical learning. Child Development 62(3), 499516.
Kalish, C. W. & Gelman, S. A. (1992). On wooden pillows: multiple classification and children's category-based inductions. Child Development 63(6), 1536–57.
Landau, B., Smith, L. & Jones, S. (1998). Object shape, object function, and object name. Journal of Memory and Language 38(1), 127.
Levy, B. A., Abello, B. & Lysynchuk, L. (1997). Transfer from word training to reading in context: gains in reading fluency and comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly 20(3), 173–88.
Nagy, W. E., Anderson, R. C. & Herman, P. A. (1987). Learning word meanings from context during normal reading. American Educational Research Journal 24(2), 237–70.
Naigles, L. (1996). The use of multiple frames in verb learning via syntactic bootstrapping. Cognition 58, 221–51.
Samuelson, L. K. & Smith, L. B. (1999). Early noun vocabularies: do ontology, category structure and syntax correspond? Cognition 73(1), 133.
Saylor, M. M., Sabbagh, M. A. & Baldwin, D. A. (2002). Children use whole–part juxtaposition as a pragmatic cue to word meaning. Developmental Psychology 38(6), 9931003.
Singer, M., Graesser, A. C. & Trabasso, T. (1994). Minimal or global inference during reading. Journal of Memory and Language 33(4), 421–41.
Smith, L. & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word–referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition 106(3), 1558–68.
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44(6), 1321–37.
Strommen, E. (1995). Lions and tigers and bears, oh my! Children's conceptions of forests and their inhabitants. Journal of Research in Science Teaching 32(7), 683–98.
Thorndyke, P. W. (1976). The role of inferences in discourse comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 15(4), 437–46.
Tomkins, S. & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2007). Nature tables: stimulating children's interest in natural objects. Journal of Biological Education 41(4), 150–55.
Umstead, R. S. & Leonard, L. B. (1983). Children's resolution of pronominal reference in text. First Language 4, 7384.
Yeung, N. & Monsell, S. (2003). Switching between tasks of unequal familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 29, 455–69.
Yu, C. & Ballard, D. H. (2007). A unified model of early word learning: integrating statistical and social cues. Neurocomputing 70(13–15), 2149–65.
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols 1(1), 297301.
Zelazo, P. D., Reznick, J. S. & Piñon, D. E. (1995). Response control and the execution of verbal rules. Developmental Psychology 31(3), 508–17.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Journal of Child Language
  • ISSN: 0305-0009
  • EISSN: 1469-7602
  • URL: /core/journals/journal-of-child-language
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Metrics

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 0 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between <date>. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Usage data cannot currently be displayed