Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-24hb2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-17T13:47:44.178Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social network and temporal discounting

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 July 2017

RONALD S. BURT*
Affiliation:
Booth School of Business, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637, USA (e-mail: ron.burt@chicagobooth.edu)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

For reasons of social influence and social logistics, people in closed networks are expected to experience time compression: The more closed a person's network, the steeper the person's discount function, and the more narrow the expected time horizon within which the person deliberates events and behavior. Consistent with the hypothesis, data on managers at the top of three organizations show network closure associated with a social life compressed into daily contact with colleagues. Further, language in closed networks is predominantly about current activities, ignoring the future. Further still, discount functions employed by executive MBA students show more severe discounting by students in more closed networks. Inattention to the future can be argued to impair achievement, however, I find no evidence across the managers of daily contact diminishing the achievement associated with network advantage. I close with comments on replication and extrapolation to language more generally, within-person variation, and select cognitive patterns (closure bias, end of history, and felt status loss).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

References

Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: A behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin, 82 (4), 463496.Google Scholar
Barndt, R. J., & Johnson, D. M. (1955). Time orientation in delinquents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 51, 343345.Google Scholar
Bartels, D. M., & Rips, L. J. (2010). Psychological connectedness and intertemporal choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 139 (1), 4969.Google Scholar
Bartels, D. M., & Urminsky, O. (2011). On intertemporal selfishness: How the perceived instability of identity underlies impatient consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 38, 182198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., & Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: Delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 447454.Google Scholar
Brands, R. A. (2013). Cognitive social structures in social network research: A review. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34 (S1), S82S103.Google Scholar
Briscoe, F. (2007). From iron cage to iron shield? How bureaucracy enables temporal flexibility for professional service workers. Organization Science, 18 (2), 297314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burt, R. S. (1990). Kinds of relations in American discussion networks. In Calhoun, C., Marshall, M. W., & Scott, W. R. (Eds.), Structures of power and constraint (pp. 411451). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Burt, R. S. (1992). Structural holes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Burt, R. S. (2004). Structural holes and good ideas. American Journal of Sociology, 110 (2), 349399.Google Scholar
Burt, R. S. (2005). Brokerage and closure. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Burt, R. S. (2010). Neighbor networks. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Burt, R. S., Kilduff, M., & Tasselli, S. (2013). Social network analysis, through a focus on network advantage. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 527547.Google Scholar
Carnabuci, G., & Dioszegi, B. (2015). Social networks, cognitive style, and innovative performance: A contingency perspective. Academy of Management Journal, 58 (3), 881905.Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time on human development. Science, 312 (5782), 19131915.Google Scholar
Chen, M. K. (2013). The effect of language on economic behavior: Evidence from savings rates, health behaviors, and retirement assets. American Economic Review, 103 (2), 690731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coser, R. L. (1975). The complexity of roles as a seedbed of individual autonomy. In Coser, L. A. (Eds.), The idea of social structure (pp. 237263). New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Danescu-Niculescu-Mizil, C., Lee, L., Pang, B., & Kleinberg, J. (2012). Echoes of power: Language effects and power differences in social interaction. Proc. 21st International World Wide Web Conference.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davids, A., Kidder, C., & Reich, M. (1962). Time orientation in male and female juvenile delinquents. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64 (3), 239240.Google Scholar
De Volder, M. L., & Lens, W. (1982). Academic achievement and future time perspective as a cognitive-motivational concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42 (3), 566571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feld, S. L. (1981). The focused organization of social ties. American Journal of Sociology, 86 (5), 10151035.Google Scholar
Festinger, L., Schachter, S., & Back, K. W. (1950). Social pressures in informal groups. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Frederick, S., Loewenstein, G., & O'Donoghue, T. (2002). Time discounting and time preference: A critical review. Journal of Economic Literature, 40, 351401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, L. C. (1992). Filling in the blanks: A theory of cognitive categories and the structure of social affiliation. Social Psychology Quarterly, 55 (2), 118127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gavetti, G., & Rivkin, J. W. (2007). On the origin of strategy: Action and cognition over time. Organization Science, 18 (3), 420439.Google Scholar
Gilmore, T. N., & Shea, G. P. (1997). Organizational learning and the leadership skill of time travel. Journal of Management Development, 16 (4), 302311.Google Scholar
Gould, R. V. (2003). Collision of wills. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Granovetter, M. (1992). Problems of explanation in economic sociology. In Nohria, N., & Eccles, R. G. (Eds.), Networks and organizations (pp. 2956). Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.Google Scholar
Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of delayed rewards: A life-span comparison. Psychological Science, 5 (1), 3336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, E. T. (1959). The silent dimension. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Ireland, M. E., Slatcher, R. B., Eastwick, P. W., Scissors, L. W., Finkel, E. J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2011). Language style matching predicts relationship initiation and stability. Psychological Science, 22 (1), 3944.Google Scholar
Janicik, G. A., & Larrick, R. P. (2005). Social network schemas and the learning of incomplete networks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88 (2), 348364.Google Scholar
Jaques, E. ([1989]2006). Requisite organization. (Revised memorial edn). Jacksonville, FL: Cason Hall.Google Scholar
Jaques, E. (1979). Taking time seriously in evaluating jobs. Harvard Business Review, 57 (4), 124132.Google Scholar
Jaques, E. (1990). In praise of hierarchy. Harvard Business Review, 68 (1), 127133.Google Scholar
Kagal, J. H., Green, L., & Caraco, T. (1986). When foragers discount the future: Constraint or adaptation? Animal Behavior, 34, 271283.Google Scholar
Kaplan, S. (2008). Framing contests: Strategy making under uncertainty. Organization Science, 19 (5), 729752.Google Scholar
Kaplan, S., & Orlikowski, W. J. (2013). Temporal work in strategy making. Organization Science, 24 (4), 965995.Google Scholar
Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future: Evidence against normative discounting of delayed rewards. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 126 (1), 5470.Google Scholar
Kirby, K. N., Winston, G. C., & Santiesteban, S. (2005). Impatience and grades: Delay-discount rates correlate negatively with college GPA. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 213222.Google Scholar
Malkoc, S. A., & Zauberman, G. (2006). Deferring versus expediting consumption: The effect of outcome concreteness on sensitivity to time horizon. Journal of Marketing Research, 43 (4), 618627.Google Scholar
Marsden, P. V., & Campbell, K. E. (1984). Measuring tie strength. Social Forces, 63 (2), 482501.Google Scholar
Mehl, M. R. (2012). Naturalistic observation sampling: The electronically activated recorder (EAR). In Mehl, M. R., & Conner, T. S. (Eds.), Handbook of research methods for studying daily life (pp. 176192). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Merton, R. K. ([1957]1968). Continuities in the theory of reference group behavior. In Merton, R. K. (Ed.), Social theory and social structure (pp. 335440). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16 (2), 329337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullainathan, S., & Shafir, E. (2003). Scarcity. New York: Times Books.Google Scholar
O'Connor, K. M., & Gladstone, E. (2015). How social exclusion distorts social network perceptions. Social Networks, 40, 123128.Google Scholar
O'Rand, A., & Ellis, R. A. (1974). Social class and social time perspective. Social Forces, 53 (1), 5362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orlikowski, W. J., & Yates, J. (2002). It's about time: Temporal structuring in organizations. Organization Science, 13 (6), 684700.Google Scholar
Padgett, J. F., &. Ansell, C. K. (1993). Robust action and the rise of the Medici, 1400–1434. American Journal of Sociology, 98 (6), 12591319.Google Scholar
Parfit, D. (1984). Reasons and persons. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R J., & Francis, M. E. (2007). Linguistic inquiry and word count (LIWC). Retrieved from http://www.liwc.net.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, J. W., Mehl, M. R., & Niederhoffer, K. G. (2003). Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 547577.Google Scholar
Pentland, A. (2008). Honest signals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pentland, B. T., Recker, J., & Wyner, G. (2017). Rediscovering handoffs. Academy of Management Discoveries, 3, In Press.Google Scholar
Petry, N. M. (2001). Pathological gamblers, with and without substance use disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110 (3), 482487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petry, N. M., Bickel, W. K., & Arnett, A. (1998). Shortened time horizons and insensitivity to future consequences in heroin addicts. Addiction, 93 (5), 729738.Google Scholar
Quoidbach, J., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2013). The end of history illusion. Science, 339, 9698.Google Scholar
Rasmussen, E. B., Lawyer, S. T., & Reilly, W. (2010). Percent body fat is related to delay and probability discounting for food in humans. Behavioural Processes, 83, 2330.Google Scholar
Romero, D. M., Swaab, R. I., Uzzi, B., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). Mimicry is presidential: Linguistic style matching in presidential debates and improved polling numbers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41 (10), 13111319.Google Scholar
Salmon, E. D., Gelfand, M. J., Ting, H., Kraus, S., Gal, Y., & Fulmer, C. A. (2016). When time is not money: Why Americans may lose out at the negotiation table. Academy of Management Discoveries, 2 (4), 349367.Google Scholar
Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26 (6), 978986.Google Scholar
Simmel, G. (1922 [1955]). Conflict and web of group affiliation. Translated by Wolff, K. H., & Bendix, R. (Eds.). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Smith, E. B., Menon, T., & Thompson, L. (2012). Status differences in the cognitive activation of social networks. Organization Science, 23 (1), 6782.Google Scholar
Srivastava, S. B., Goldberg, A., Manian, V. G., & Potts, C. (Forthcoming). Enculturation trajectories: Language, cultural adaptation, and individual outcomes in organizations. Management Science.Google Scholar
Strotz, R. H. (1955–1956). Myopia and inconsistency in dynamic utility maximization. Review of Economic Studies, 23 (3), 165180.Google Scholar
Sudman, S., &. Bradburn, N. M. (1973). Effects of time and memory factors on response in surveys. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 68 (344), 805815.Google Scholar
Teahan, J. E. (1958). Future time perspective, optimism, and academic achievement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 57 (3), 379380.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. (1981). Some empirical evidence on dynamic inconsistency. Economics Letters, 8, 201207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tourangeau, R., Rips, L. J., & Rasinski, K. (2000). The psychology of survey responses. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vuchinich, R. E., & Simpson, C. A. (1998). Hyperbolic temporal discounting in social drinkers and problem drinkers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 6 (6), 292305.Google Scholar
Wallace, M. (1956). Future time perspective in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 52 (2), 240245.Google Scholar
Zauberman, G., Kim, B. K., Malkoc, S. A., & Bettman, J. R. (2009). Discounting time and time discounting: Subjective time perception and intertemporal preferences. Journal of Marketing Research, 66 (4), 543556.Google Scholar
Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77 (6), 12711288.Google Scholar