Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-559fc8cf4f-rz424 Total loading time: 3.821 Render date: 2021-03-06T02:16:23.944Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "shouldUseShareProductTool": true, "shouldUseHypothesis": true, "isUnsiloEnabled": true, "metricsAbstractViews": false, "figures": false, "newCiteModal": false, "newCitedByModal": true }

The Role of Sketching Activities and Outcomes in Conceptual Design Phase

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 July 2019

Marija Nikolić
Affiliation:
University of Zagreb;
Stanko Škec
Affiliation:
University of Zagreb; Technical University of Denmark (DTU)
Tomislav Martinec
Affiliation:
University of Zagreb;
Nikola Horvat
Affiliation:
University of Zagreb;
Corresponding
E-mail address:

Abstract

Sketching-related activities are considered as an essential form of communication in the early phases of a design process. In the presented study, it is argued that both the sketching and the sketch-related verbalisations are reflected in the level of elaboration of the sketching outputs. Hence, a protocol study was conducted to analyse the frequencies of different sketching-related activities during team conceptual design sessions and the associated levels of elaboration for each of the sketching outputs in the form of concept drawings. The results show that although teams generate sketches of various number, complexity and clarity, there exist commonalities across the studied experiment sessions. For example, teams share a pattern of developing solutions without transformations or using lateral transformations within the first part of the sessions and using vertical transformations to produce final concepts towards the end of the sessions. Moreover, teams used associated sketch elements to start drawing new sketches and then alternated to other activities, most of all verbal explanation, for the sake of elaboration and better understanding.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

References

Agogino, A., Song, S. and Hey, J. (2006), “Triangulation of Indicators of Successful Student Design Teams”, International Journal of Engineering Education, Vol. 22 No. 3, pp. 617625.Google Scholar
Besemer, S.P. (1998), “Creative product analysis matrix: Testing the model structure and a comparison among products - Three novel chairs”, Creativity Research Journal, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 333346. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1104_7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, H.-H., You, M. and Lee, C.-F. (2003), “The sketch in industrial design process”, Proceedings of the 6th Asian Design Conference: Integration of Knowledge, Kansei, and Industrial Power, Tsukuba, Japan, October 14-17, 2003.Google Scholar
Dean, D.L., Hender, J.M., Rodgers, T.L. and Santanen, E. (2006), “Identifying good ideas: constructs and scales for idea evaluation”, Journal of Association for Information Systems, Vol. 7 No. 10, pp. 646699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorst, K. and Dijkhuis, J. (1995), “Comparing paradigms for describing design activity”, Design Studies, Vol. 16 No. 2, pp. 261274. https://doi.org/10.1016/0142-694X(94)00012-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eris, O., Martelaro, N. and Badke-Schaub, P. (2014), “A comparative analysis of multimodal communication during design sketching in co-located and distributed environments”, Design Studies, Vol. 35 No. 6, pp. 559592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.destud.2014.04.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fish, J. and Scrivener, S. (1990), “Amplifying the mind's eye: Sketching and visual cognition”, Leonardo, Vol. 23 No. 1, pp. 117126. https://doi.org/10.2307/1578475CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gero, J.S. (1990), “Design Prototypes: A Knowledge Representation Schema for Design”, AI Magazine, Vol. 11 No. 4, pp. 2636. https://doi.org/10.1609/aimag.v11i4.854Google Scholar
Goel, V. (1995), Sketches of Thought, MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldschmidt, G. (1991), “The Dialectics of Sketching”, Creativity Research Journal, Vol. 4 No. 2, pp. 123143. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419109534381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heiser, J., Tversky, B. and Silverman, M.I.A. (2004), “Sketches for and from collaboration”, Preprints of the 3rd International Conference on Visual and Spatial Reasoning in Design, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, USA, July 22-23, 2004, pp. 6978.Google Scholar
Kudrowitz, B., Te, P. and Wallace, D. (2012), “The influence of sketch quality on perception of product-idea creativity”, Artificial Intelligence for Engineering Design, Analysis and Manufacturing, Vol. 26 No. 3, pp. 267279. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0890060412000145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Lugt, R. (2002), “Brainsketching and how it differs from brainstorming”, Creativity and Innovation Management, Vol. 11 No. 1, pp. 4354. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8691.00235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martinec, T., Horvat, N., Škec, S. and Štorga, M. (2018), “Verbal engagement in teams solving a conceptual design task”, Proceedings of the DESIGN 2018 15th International Design Conference, pp. 20752086. https://doi.org/10.21278/idc.2018.0540Google Scholar
Neill, T.M., Gero, J.S. and Warren, J. (1998), “Understanding conceptual electronic design using protocol analysis”, Research in Engineering Design, Vol. 10 No. 3, pp. 129140. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01607155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nik Ahmad Ariff, N.S., Badke-Schaub, P., Eris, O. and Suib, S.S.S. (2012), “A framework for reaching common understanding during sketching in design teams”, Proceedings of the 12th International Design Conference (DESIGN 2012), Dubrovnik, Croatia, May 21-24, 2012, pp. 15251533.Google Scholar
Purcell, A.T. and Gero, J.S. (1998), “Drawings and the design process: A review of protocol studies in design and other disciplines and related research in cognitive psychology”, Design Studies, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 389430. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(98)00015-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rodgers, P.A., Green, G. and McGown, A. (2000), “Using concept sketches to track design progress”, Design Studies, Vol. 21 No. 5, pp. 451464. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(00)00018-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachse, P., Hacker, W. and Leinert, S. (2004), “External thought - Does sketching assist problem analysis?”, Applied Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 18 No. 4, pp. 415425. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.992CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scrivener, S.A.R., Ball, L.J. and Tseng, W. (2000), “Uncertainty and sketching behaviour”, Design Studies, Vol. 21 No. 5, pp. 465481. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(00)00019-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sevier, D.C., Jablokow, K., McKilligan, S., Daly, S.R., Baker, I.N. and Silk, E.M. (2017), “Towards the Development of an Elaboration Metric for Concept Sketches”, Proceedings of the ASME 2017 IDETC-CIE Conference, Ohio, USA, August 6–9, 2017, p. V003T04A006.Google Scholar
Suwa, M., Purcell, T. and Gero, J. (1998), “Macroscopic analysis of design processes based on a scheme for coding designers’ cognitive actions”, Design Studies, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 455483. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0142-694X(98)00016-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ullman, D.G., Wood, S. and Craig, D. (1990), “The importance of drawing in the mechanical design process”, Computers and Graphics, Vol. 14 No. 2, pp. 263274. https://doi.org/10.1016/0097-8493(90)90037-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuan, P., Li, Y., Chen, J., Xiong, Y. and Liu, L. (2018), “The importance of drawing in the mechanical design process”, Journal of Mechanical Design, Vol. 140 No. 10, p. 101102. https://doi.org/10.1115/1.4040627CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Full text views

Full text views reflects PDF downloads, PDFs sent to Google Drive, Dropbox and Kindle and HTML full text views.

Total number of HTML views: 0
Total number of PDF views: 198 *
View data table for this chart

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between 26th July 2019 - 6th March 2021. This data will be updated every 24 hours.

Access
Open access

Send article to Kindle

To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

The Role of Sketching Activities and Outcomes in Conceptual Design Phase
Available formats
×

Send article to Dropbox

To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

The Role of Sketching Activities and Outcomes in Conceptual Design Phase
Available formats
×

Send article to Google Drive

To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

The Role of Sketching Activities and Outcomes in Conceptual Design Phase
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response


Your details


Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *