Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-20T04:52:01.633Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Conceptual clarity and valid measurement are needed to improve research on depression in dementia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2023

Toni T. Saari*
Affiliation:
Institute for Molecular Medicine Finland (FiMM), HiLIFE, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland Department of Neurology, University of Eastern Finland, Kuopio, Finland
*
Author for correspondence: Toni T. Saari, E-mail: toni.saari@uef.fi, toni.saari@helsinki.fi
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Costello, Roiser, and Howard (Reference Costello, Roiser and Howard2023) examine the nature, mechanisms and treatment of depression in dementia in their timely review. While outlining important avenues potentially leading to better pharmacological treatment of depression in dementia, the authors also show the conceptual challenges inherent in studying neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia, of which depression is a prime example. In this brief commentary, I wish to expand on the authors’ examination of the clinical construct we know as depression in dementia by including both the broader perspective of depression assessment and the results of recent studies in dementia specifically.

Depression in dementia can be considered distinct compared to major depressive disorder in the cognitively unimpaired (Olin, Katz, Meyers, Schneider, & Lebowitz, Reference Olin, Katz, Meyers, Schneider and Lebowitz2002), but there are at least three conceptual and methodological similarities between the two fields. The first is the challenge of valid and reliable measurement of depression (Fried, Flake, & Robinaugh, Reference Fried, Flake and Robinaugh2022). Commonly used measures of depression in dementia can be roughly divided into three groups: those used in screening and diagnosis of depression with no special focus on dementia or ageing, those with a focus on geriatric psychiatry and those specifically designed for dementia. While using dementia-specific rating scales appears to be the best option, the validity evidence for even the most commonly used instruments is relatively scarce (e.g. Perrault, Oremus, Demers, Vida, & Wolfson, Reference Perrault, Oremus, Demers, Vida and Wolfson2000). This lack of evidence calls into question how well we are assessing depression (and not agitation, sleep disturbances, apathy, anxiety, cognitive symptoms, etc.), an observation also raised by the authors. Moreover, many of the earlier neuropsychiatric symptom measures derive their validity evidence from samples that had more severe dementia than individuals coming to memory clinics today.

Aiming for operational criteria of depression is another point of convergence between dementia and the general population. There are numerous diagnostic and research criteria for neuropsychiatric syndromes in dementia, including depression (Olin et al., Reference Olin, Katz, Meyers, Schneider and Lebowitz2002). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) has been considered an example to strive for, and existing criteria for psychiatric disorders have been used as a starting point for neuropsychiatric syndrome criteria in dementia (Saari, Reference Saari2023). Relatively absent, however, is the critical discussion of diagnostic criteria in neuropsychiatric syndromes (including depression) in light of the substantive criticism levelled at diagnostic criteria of psychiatric syndromes (e.g. Andreasen, Reference Andreasen2007; Frances & Widiger, Reference Frances and Widiger2012; Haslam, McGrath, Viechtbauer, & Kuppens, Reference Haslam, McGrath, Viechtbauer and Kuppens2020). The critical discussion in neuropsychiatric syndromes of dementia has largely focused on incremental changes in existing criteria to accommodate research advances rather than more fundamental aspects of the process, such as how transparently the criteria are formulated and how possible concerns from the scientific community or the general public are handled (Frances & Widiger, Reference Frances and Widiger2012; Saari, Reference Saari2023).

Owing to the pathological and clinical heterogeneity in dementia, it appears that there is no a priori reason to assume that the diagnostic and research criteria of neuropsychiatric syndromes are working significantly better than they are in psychiatry. For depression specifically, there are data indicating that the National Institute of Mental Health-Depression in Alzheimer's disease (NIMH-dAD) criteria (Olin et al., Reference Olin, Katz, Meyers, Schneider and Lebowitz2002) identify more individuals as depressed compared to the DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) in line with the requirement of less symptoms and a shorter persistence than the DSM criteria (Sepehry et al., Reference Sepehry, Lee, Hsiung, Beattie, Feldman and Jacova2017). However, Sepehry et al. (Reference Sepehry, Lee, Hsiung, Beattie, Feldman and Jacova2017) concluded that the reliability of the NIMH-dAD criteria still needs to be established. Furthermore, a certain circularity cannot be avoided in comparing the diagnostic properties of the NIMH-dAD to the DSM criteria of major or minor depressive disorder that served as the foundation for the NIMH-dAD criteria.

The third area of overlap, which relates to the preceding observations, is the debate on the nature of depression as continuous or categorical. As yet, there appears to be no conceptual consensus on whether depression in dementia is a distinct categorical entity (present or not), or continuous in the same sense as cognitive symptoms. Based on extensive empirical research, most diagnostic constructs in psychiatry, including mood disorder, tend to be better represented as continuous dimensions (Haslam et al., Reference Haslam, McGrath, Viechtbauer and Kuppens2020). It seems at least plausible that depression in dementia could be continuous in nature owing to progressive neurodegeneration and various psychosocial factors. If depression in dementia was continuous, it would benefit clinical trials as continuous outcomes provide an increase in statistical power (Markon, Chmielewski, & Miller, Reference Markon, Chmielewski and Miller2011).

A possibly fruitful way forward, as also suggested by the authors, is to step outside the traditional thinking in diagnostic categories and sum-scores by examining how depressive symptoms connect to another as networks. The authors propose viewing depression as ‘a complex system of interacting depressive symptoms, which differs across dementia subtypes and stages of disease’ (Costello et al., Reference Costello, Roiser and Howard2023). There are some studies that, taken together, may elucidate the dynamics of depressive symptoms from normal cognitive ageing to dementia.

A network analytic study found that the symptoms of lack of happiness and worthlessness were among the most central depressive symptoms in cognitively unimpaired older adults, and that apathy and depressive symptoms were highly interwoven (van Wanrooij, Borsboom, Moll van Charante, Richard, & van Gool, Reference van Wanrooij, Borsboom, Moll van Charante, Richard and van Gool2019). In our own study, we found a highly similar network structure to that of van Wanrooij et al. in patients with (mostly very mild or mild) AD, with helplessness and lack of happiness again as the most interconnected symptoms, with worthlessness also emerging as one of the more central features (Saari, Hallikainen, Hintsa, & Koivisto, Reference Saari, Hallikainen, Hintsa and Koivisto2020). The network structure was also highly similar after 1 year of follow-up. An earlier study by Masters, Morris, and Roe (Reference Masters, Morris and Roe2015) found that individual depressive symptoms did not appreciably differ between individuals who remained cognitively unimpaired and those with cognitive decline in follow-up. The only depression symptom that predicted cognitive decline was, unsurprisingly, ‘having more memory problems than most’ (as was also found in van Wanrooij et al. Reference van Wanrooij, Borsboom, Moll van Charante, Richard and van Gool2019).

Taken together, the three studies show tentative evidence that there are no marked qualitative changes in the dynamics of individual depressive symptoms between normal cognitive ageing and mild AD; evidence to the contrary in AD may partly be explained by the use of total scores of depression rating instruments. However, some caveats should be kept in mind. First, the participants in these studies did not need to meet criteria for depression and different network structures might emerge in clinically depressed individuals without cognitive impairment compared to those with dementia. Next, the authors (Costello et al., Reference Costello, Roiser and Howard2023) cite two studies in Parkinson's disease, where depression symptom profiles differed between depressed older adults with and without Parkinson's disease and individuals with Parkinson's disease with and without dementia, respectively (Ehrt, Brønnick, Leentjens, Larsen, & Aarsland, Reference Ehrt, Brønnick, Leentjens, Larsen and Aarsland2006; Riedel, Heuser, Klotsche, Dodel, & Wittchen, Reference Riedel, Heuser, Klotsche, Dodel and Wittchen2010). It appears that different neurodegenerative diseases may result in different depressive symptom profiles and larger deviations from depressive symptom profiles in those without cognitive impairment. Finally, there is a need to examine the effects of antidepressant treatment on the network structure of depression (Costello et al., Reference Costello, Roiser and Howard2023).

In addition to similarities in conceptualising and assessing depression in dementia and depression in those without cognitive impairment, a key difference is that in dementia, depression is often assessed by an informant. This difference was also noted by the authors, but the implications may deserve further comment as depressive symptom ratings from different raters tend to produce discrepant results in cognitively normal ageing (Georgi, Vlckova, Lukavsky, Kopecek, & Bares, Reference Georgi, Vlckova, Lukavsky, Kopecek and Bares2019) and in individuals with dementia (O'Sullivan et al., Reference O'Sullivan, Schweighart, Lech, Kessler, Tegeler, Teti and Gellert2022; Saari et al., Reference Saari, Hallikainen, Hintsa and Koivisto2020). It is acknowledged that caregiver burden (Pfeifer et al., Reference Pfeifer, Drobetz, Fankhauser, Mortby, Maercker and Forstmeier2013), anosognosia (Verhülsdonk, Quack, Höft, Lange-Asschenfeldt, & Supprian, Reference Verhülsdonk, Quack, Höft, Lange-Asschenfeldt and Supprian2013) and the various neurocognitive changes outlined by the authors (Costello et al., Reference Costello, Roiser and Howard2023) may drive discrepancies between self-ratings and informant-ratings. In addition to these clinical and psychosocial factors of the disease, there may also be more fundamental psychometric and psychological factors at play. For instance, informant-ratings tend to understandably emphasise explicit signs and symptoms of depression which may only represent a part of the depressive symptoms experienced by the patient (Mograbi & Morris, Reference Mograbi and Morris2014). There is also a basic asymmetry in how people view themselves v. how they view others (Pronin, Reference Pronin2008), which may explain why a perfect convergence between rating modalities is unachievable even under the most optimal conditions. Rather than trying to decide which rating modality is the least biased, other fields have attempted to create methods for integrating ratings of multiple modalities, which, in testing, have provided better predictive utility than any single rating (Makol et al., Reference Makol, Youngstrom, Racz, Qasmieh, Glenn and Reyes2020).

In sum, there appears to be much for dementia research to still learn from the trials and errors in conceptualisation and assessment of depression in the cognitively healthy population. It is hoped that research aiming to refine the concept(s) and measurement of depression in dementia would be met with more enthusiasm and urgency. With future treatment studies in mind, more attention should be paid to defining what depression in dementia is and is not and how to best assess depression at various stages of cognitive decline.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of interest

The author declares none.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (2007). DSM and the death of phenomenology in America: An example of unintended consequences. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(1), 108112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, H., Roiser, J. P., & Howard, R. (2023). Antidepressant medications in dementia: Evidence and potential mechanisms of treatment-resistance. Psychological Medicine, 53(3), 654667. Retrieved from https://doi:10.1017/S003329172200397X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehrt, U., Brønnick, K., Leentjens, A. F. G., Larsen, J. P., & Aarsland, D. (2006). Depressive symptom profile in Parkinson's disease: A comparison with depression in elderly patients without Parkinson's disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21(3), 252258. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.1456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frances, A. J., & Widiger, T. (2012). Psychiatric diagnosis: Lessons from the DSM-IV past and cautions for the DSM-5 future. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8(1), 109130. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, E. I., Flake, J. K., & Robinaugh, D. J. (2022). Revisiting the theoretical and methodological foundations of depression measurement. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1, 358368. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00050-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgi, H. S., Vlckova, K. H., Lukavsky, J., Kopecek, M., & Bares, M. (2019). Beck Depression Inventory-II: Self-report or interview-based administrations show different results in older persons. International Psychogeriatrics, 31(5), 735742. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610218001187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, N., McGrath, M. J., Viechtbauer, W., & Kuppens, P. (2020). Dimensions over categories: A meta-analysis of taxometric research. Psychological Medicine, 50(9), 14181432. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172000183X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Makol, B. A., Youngstrom, E. A., Racz, S. J., Qasmieh, N., Glenn, L. E., & Reyes, A. D. L. (2020). Integrating multiple informants’ reports: How conceptual and measurement models may address long-standing problems in clinical decision-making. Clinical Psychological Science, 8(6), 953970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markon, K. E., Chmielewski, M., & Miller, C. J. (2011). The reliability and validity of discrete and continuous measures of psychopathology: A quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 856879. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masters, M. C., Morris, J. C., & Roe, C. M. (2015). ‘Noncognitive’ symptoms of early Alzheimer disease: A longitudinal analysis. Neurology, 84(6), 617622. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000001238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mograbi, D. C., & Morris, R. G. (2014). On the relation among mood, apathy, and anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(1), 27. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617713001276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olin, J. T., Katz, I. R., Meyers, B. S., Schneider, L. S., & Lebowitz, B. D. (2002). Provisional diagnostic criteria for depression of Alzheimer disease: Rationale and background. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 10(2), 129141. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1097/00019442-200203000-00004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Sullivan, J. L., Schweighart, R., Lech, S., Kessler, E.-M., Tegeler, C., Teti, A., … Gellert, P. (2022). Concordance of self- and informant-rated depressive symptoms in nursing home residents with dementia: Cross-sectional findings. BMC Psychiatry, 22(1), 241. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-03876-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perrault, A., Oremus, M., Demers, L., Vida, S., & Wolfson, C. (2000). Review of outcome measurement instruments in Alzheimer's disease drug trials: Psychometric properties of behavior and mood scales. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 13(4), 181196. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177/089198870001300403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pfeifer, L., Drobetz, R., Fankhauser, S., Mortby, M. E., Maercker, A., & Forstmeier, S. (2013). Caregiver rating bias in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease: Impact of caregiver burden and depression on dyadic rating discrepancy across domains. International Psychogeriatrics, 25(8), 13451355. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610213000562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pronin, E. (2008). How we see ourselves and how we see others. Science (New York, N.Y.), 320(5880), 11771180. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1154199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riedel, O., Heuser, I., Klotsche, J., Dodel, R., & Wittchen, H.-U., & GEPAD Study Group. (2010). Occurrence risk and structure of depression in Parkinson disease with and without dementia: Results from the GEPAD study. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 23(1), 2734. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988709351833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saari, T. T. (2023). Empirical and authoritative classification of neuropsychiatric syndromes in neurocognitive disorders. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 35(1), 3947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saari, T. T., Hallikainen, I., Hintsa, T., & Koivisto, A. M. (2020). Network structures and temporal stability of self- and informant-rated affective symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Affective Disorders, 276, 10841092. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.07.100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sepehry, A. A., Lee, P. E., Hsiung, G.-Y. R., Beattie, B. L., Feldman, H. H., & Jacova, C. (2017). The 2002 NIMH provisional diagnostic criteria for depression of Alzheimer's disease (PDC-dAD): Gauging their validity over a decade later. Journal of Alzheimer's Disease, 58(2), 449462. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-161061.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Wanrooij, L. L., Borsboom, D., Moll van Charante, E. P., Richard, E., & van Gool, W. A. (2019). A network approach on the relation between apathy and depression symptoms with dementia and functional disability. International Psychogeriatrics, 31(11), 16551663. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610218002387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verhülsdonk, S., Quack, R., Höft, B., Lange-Asschenfeldt, C., & Supprian, T. (2013). Anosognosia and depression in patients with Alzheimer's dementia. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 57(3), 282287. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2013.03.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed