Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T19:11:50.063Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do general and specific factors of preschool psychopathology predict preadolescent outcomes? A transdiagnostic hierarchical approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 August 2022

Giorgia Michelini*
Affiliation:
Department of Biological & Experimental Psychology, School of Biological & Behavioural Sciences, Queen Mary University of London, London, UK Semel Institute for Neuroscience & Human Behavior, University of California Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Kelly Gair
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Yuan Tian
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Mathematics and Statistics, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Jiaju Miao
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Mathematics and Statistics, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Lea R. Dougherty
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
Brandon L. Goldstein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, CT, USA
Leigha A. MacNeill
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Social Sciences, Feinberg School of Medicine, and Institute for Innovations in Developmental Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA
Deanna M. Barch
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USA Departments of Psychiatry, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USA Departments of Radiology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USA
Joan L. Luby
Affiliation:
Departments of Psychiatry, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USA
Lauren S. Wakschlag
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Social Sciences, Feinberg School of Medicine, and Institute for Innovations in Developmental Sciences, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA
Daniel N. Klein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
Roman Kotov
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Health, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Giorgia Michelini, E-mail: g.michelini@qmul.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Preschool psychiatric symptoms significantly increase the risk for long-term negative outcomes. Transdiagnostic hierarchical approaches that capture general (‘p’) and specific psychopathology dimensions are promising for understanding risk and predicting outcomes, but their predictive utility in young children is not well established. We delineated a hierarchical structure of preschool psychopathology dimensions and tested their ability to predict psychiatric disorders and functional impairment in preadolescence.

Methods

Data for 1253 preschool children (mean age = 4.17, s.d. = 0.81) were drawn from three longitudinal studies using a similar methodology (one community sample, two psychopathology-enriched samples) and followed up into preadolescence, yielding a large and diverse sample. Exploratory factor models derived a hierarchical structure of general and specific factors using symptoms from the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment interview. Longitudinal analyses examined the prospective associations of preschool p and specific factors with preadolescent psychiatric disorders and functional impairment.

Results

A hierarchical dimensional structure with a p factor at the top and up to six specific factors (distress, fear, separation anxiety, social anxiety, inattention-hyperactivity, oppositionality) emerged at preschool age. The p factor predicted all preadolescent disorders (ΔR2 = 0.04–0.15) and functional impairment (ΔR2 = 0.01–0.07) to a significantly greater extent than preschool psychiatric diagnoses and functioning. Specific dimensions provided additional predictive power for the majority of preadolescent outcomes (disorders: ΔR2 = 0.06–0.15; functional impairment: ΔR2 = 0.05–0.12).

Conclusions

Both general and specific dimensions of preschool psychopathology are useful for predicting clinical and functional outcomes almost a decade later. These findings highlight the value of transdiagnostic dimensions for predicting prognosis and as potential targets for early intervention and prevention.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Psychiatric symptoms are as common in preschoolers as they are in school-aged children (Dougherty et al., Reference Dougherty, Leppert, Merwin, Smith, Bufferd and Kushner2015; Egger & Angold, Reference Egger and Angold2006). Emotional and behavioral difficulties in early childhood contribute to a negative cascade with long-lasting effects over development and prospectively predict future psychopathology at school age and beyond (Finsaas, Bufferd, Dougherty, Carlson, & Klein, Reference Finsaas, Bufferd, Dougherty, Carlson and Klein2018; Luby, Gaffrey, Tillman, April, & Belden, Reference Luby, Gaffrey, Tillman, April and Belden2014). Assessing and monitoring the emergence of preschool symptoms is an important step for the design and implementation of prevention and early intervention efforts (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Roberts, Flynn, Smith, Krogh-Jespersen, Kaat and Davis2019). Yet, pragmatic methods for predicting the course of psychopathology from preschool age and informing the prevention of future psychiatric outcomes are still underdeveloped (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Allen, Estabrook, Pine, Rogers, Krogh-Jespersen and Wakschlag2019).

A challenge for clinical practice and research in preschool populations is that traditional psychiatric diagnoses are not tailored to preschool children and do not provide guidelines for diagnosing most conditions before school age (APA, 2013; WHO, 1992). General skepticism toward preschool diagnoses among clinicians often arises from concerns regarding ‘labeling’ young children with psychopathology, given the stigma around mental illness (Heflinger & Hinshaw, Reference Heflinger and Hinshaw2010) and the ‘they'll grow out of it’ myth (Luby, Reference Luby2012). Further, the methodological artifacts induced by binary categorical diagnoses often mean that preschool children may shift between above and below clinical cutoffs despite showing a relatively stable clinical picture (Angold, Costello, Farmer, Burns, & Erkanli, Reference Angold, Costello, Farmer, Burns and Erkanli1999). A promising approach to address these concerns, consistent with the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology (HiTOP; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017) and Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; Cuthbert, Reference Cuthbert2014) frameworks, is to describe psychopathology using transdiagnostic dimensions, rather than binary diagnoses. Transdiagnostic dimensional approaches allow a finer-grained characterization of clinical problems across a continuum of severity (including subthreshold presentations) and better account for the widespread co-occurrence and heterotypic developmental continuity between different forms of psychopathology (Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman, & Kotov, Reference Michelini, Palumbo, DeYoung, Latzman and Kotov2021; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Choi, Carter, Hullsiek, Burns, McCarthy and Briggs-Gowan2012). Moreover, they provide richer information for understanding risk and greater clinical utility than narrow band diagnoses at younger ages. This is in line with current clinical approaches toward the early prevention of psychopathology, which largely focus on self-regulation promotion via parent training (Forbes, Rapee, & Krueger, Reference Forbes, Rapee and Krueger2019; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Roberts, Flynn, Smith, Krogh-Jespersen, Kaat and Davis2019).

Among transdiagnostic approaches, hierarchical dimensional approaches consistent with the HiTOP framework delineate major latent dimensions of psychopathology organized across multiple hierarchical levels, from general to specific (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2014; Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017). Several studies using either exploratory factor analysis (EFA) and bifactor approaches in school-aged children and adults have identified a general psychopathology (‘p’) factor at the apex, which is thought to reflect a general liability to all mental health problems and accounts for their common co-occurrence (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Cheesman, Rimfeld, Selzam, Pingault, Eley and Plomin2020; Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2014; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Applegate, Hakes, Zald, Hariri and Rathouz2012). A number of specific dimensions have also been delineated with these approaches (e.g. fear, distress, antisocial behavior) (Achenbach, Ivanova, & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach, Ivanova and Rescorla2017; Carragher et al., Reference Carragher, Teesson, Sunderland, Newton, Krueger, Conrod and Slade2016; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Hicks, Angstadt, Rutherford, Taxali, Hyde and Sripada2021; Forbes, Tackett, Markon, & Krueger, Reference Forbes, Tackett, Markon and Krueger2016; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019; St Clair et al., Reference St Clair, Neufeld, Jones, Fonagy, Bullmore, Dolan and Goodyer2017), which may differ with age (Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019). Several dimensional measures exist for assessing preschool psychopathology (Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach, Ivanova and Rescorla2017; Essex et al., Reference Essex, Boyce, Goldstein, Armstrong, Kraemer and Kupfer2002; Goodman, Reference Goodman2001), but empirical efforts for characterizing preschool psychopathology through latent transdiagnostic dimensions have been very limited (Sterba, Egger, & Angold, Reference Sterba, Egger and Angold2007; Wichstrøm & Berg-Nielsen, Reference Wichstrøm and Berg-Nielsen2014), and none have characterized it hierarchically in a sufficiently detailed fashion. A few available preschool studies have applied confirmatory bifactor models, including a general p factor alongside specific internalizing and externalizing factors (Olino, Dougherty, Bufferd, Carlson, & Klein, Reference Olino, Dougherty, Bufferd, Carlson and Klein2014; Olino et al., Reference Olino, Bufferd, Dougherty, Dyson, Carlson and Klein2018) or internalizing, externalizing and inattention-hyperactivity factors (McElroy, Belsky, Carragher, Fearon, & Patalay, Reference McElroy, Belsky, Carragher, Fearon and Patalay2018). Yet, because these findings are based on pre-specified confirmatory models, it is unclear whether a greater number of specific dimensions can be identified in preschoolers, in line with EFA studies of older children and adults (Carragher et al., Reference Carragher, Teesson, Sunderland, Newton, Krueger, Conrod and Slade2016; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Tackett, Markon and Krueger2016, Reference Forbes, Greene, Levin-Aspenson, Watts, Hallquist, Lahey and Krueger2021a, Reference Forbes, Sunderland, Rapee, Batterham, Calear, Carragher and Krueger2021b; Kim & Eaton, Reference Kim and Eaton2015; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019).

Furthermore, little is known about the effects of preschool p factor and specific dimensions of psychopathology on future psychiatric disorders and functional impairment. Given the widespread heterotypic continuity commonly observed across development (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Ambler, Danese, Elliott, Hariri and Moffitt2020; Finsaas et al., Reference Finsaas, Bufferd, Dougherty, Carlson and Klein2018), conceptualizing risk for future psychopathology and associated functional outcomes with a p factor at preschool age may be more useful for predicting these outcomes than using traditional diagnostic categories (Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson, & Lichtenstein, Reference Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson and Lichtenstein2018). Further, previous evidence in middle childhood shows that specific dimensions may explain certain clinical and functional outcomes over and above the p factor (Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019). Investigating the ability of preschool general and specific dimensions to predict future outcomes can help establish their clinical utility and potentially inform future prognostic and preventive efforts.

The current study aimed to delineate a hierarchical structure of preschool psychopathology dimensions and test their ability to predict psychiatric and functional outcomes in preadolescence. We aggregated data from three longitudinal samples of preschool children that utilized the same clinical interview of preschool psychopathology and were followed up into preadolescence, yielding a large, socio-demographically and racially diverse sample. We chose a follow-up time point in preadolescence based on the availability of preadolescent assessments in all three samples and because the onset of psychiatric disorders is common in this developmental period (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005). We tested the alternative hypotheses that (a) the preschool p factor alone would significantly predict preadolescent psychiatric disorders and functional impairment, without a further increase in predictive power when also considering specific dimensions; or that (b) specific dimensions would enhance predictive power beyond the p factor.

Methods

Sample

The sample consisted of participants between 3 and 6 years followed up into preadolescence and drawn from three naturalistic longitudinal studies with a number of similar characteristics (Table 1): the Multidimensional Assessment of Preschoolers Study (MAPS, N = 410; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Choi, Carter, Hullsiek, Burns, McCarthy and Briggs-Gowan2012; Wiggins, Briggs-Gowan, Brotman, Leibenluft, & Wakschlag, Reference Wiggins, Briggs-Gowan, Brotman, Leibenluft and Wakschlag2021), the Preschool Depression Study (PDS, N = 302; Gaffrey, Tillman, Barch, & Luby, Reference Gaffrey, Tillman, Barch and Luby2018; Luby, Si, Belden, Tandon, & Spitznagel, Reference Luby, Si, Belden, Tandon and Spitznagel2009), and the Stony Brook Temperament Study (SBTS, N = 541; Klein & Finsaas, Reference Klein and Finsaas2017). In all three samples, parents provided written informed consent and all study procedures were approved by local Institutional Review Boards at Northwestern University, Washington University School of Medicine, and Stony Brook University (Klein & Finsaas, Reference Klein and Finsaas2017; Luby et al., Reference Luby, Si, Belden, Tandon and Spitznagel2009; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Choi, Carter, Hullsiek, Burns, McCarthy and Briggs-Gowan2012).

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and rates of psychiatric diagnoses in each sample

ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; CD, conduct disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; MAPS, Multidimensional Assessment of Preschoolers Study; N, number of participants; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; PDS, Preschool Depression Study; SBTS, Stony Brook Temperament Study; s.d., standard deviation.

Notes: current preadolescent diagnoses were used in analyses of MAPS and PDS. Diagnoses met in the interval between assessments carried out at age 9 and at age 12 (i.e. interval diagnoses) were instead analyzed in SBTS, due to the lower rates of preadolescent diagnoses in this population-based sample. As only a subset of psychiatric diagnoses was measured at follow-up, the rates of diagnoses at follow-up do not reflect the total number of participants who met the criteria for any DSM diagnosis in preadolescence.

MAPS is a sample enriched for psychopathology which was recruited from multiple pediatric clinics within the Chicago, Illinois area (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Choi, Carter, Hullsiek, Burns, McCarthy and Briggs-Gowan2012; Wiggins et al., Reference Wiggins, Briggs-Gowan, Brotman, Leibenluft and Wakschlag2021). Parents with young children from a larger unselected sample were invited to participate, oversampling for child disruptive behavior (above the 80th percentile on the Multidimensional Assessment Profile of Disruptive Behavior) (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Choi, Carter, Hullsiek, Burns, McCarthy and Briggs-Gowan2012) and parental intimate partner violence (mother-reported past-year intimate partner violence). Eligibility criteria were English-speaking biological mother and absence of developmental delays. Of 425 children in this clinical subsample, 49.9% were Black, 29.9% were Hispanic, 18.6% were White, and 1.6% belonged to other racial/ethnical groups. Parental respondents were mainly biological mothers (91%), with 50% of all parents being married. Just under half (49.2%) of the participant families were from a poor background (Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Choi, Carter, Hullsiek, Burns, McCarthy and Briggs-Gowan2012).

PDS is a sample enriched for preschool psychopathology via oversampling of child depressive symptoms. Participants from the PDS were recruited near St. Louis, Missouri, as part of a longitudinal study of preschool depression (Gaffrey et al., Reference Gaffrey, Tillman, Barch and Luby2018; Luby et al., Reference Luby, Si, Belden, Tandon and Spitznagel2009). Recruitment was done through primary care practices, and preschools/daycares that were accessible to the general community in order to increase the socioeconomic and ethnic diversity of the sample. Recruitment sites were chosen at random using a geographically stratified method. Initial recruitment involved 1474 families, with a child between 3 and 6 years old, screened with the Preschool Feelings Checklist to assess depressive symptoms (Luby, Heffelfinger, Mrakotsky, & Hildebrand, Reference Luby, Heffelfinger, Mrakotsky and Hildebrand1999). Preschoolers with endorsed depressive symptoms (scores >3, i.e. above established cut-off) and with no depressive symptoms (scores of 0, i.e. healthy controls) were invited to join the study. A total of 305 preschoolers aged 3–6 years old completed the baseline assessment. The primary reporting caregivers were predominately biological mothers (92%). About half of the children (53.9%) were White and 100 (32.9%) were Black. Among participating parents, 130 (43.4%) had at least a four-year college degree.

SBTS is an unselected community sample of preschool children (Klein & Finsaas, Reference Klein and Finsaas2017). Participants within a 20-mile radius of a suburban community in Stony Brook, New York, were recruited from a commercial mailing list as part of a longitudinal study examining temperament and risk for psychopathology (Klein & Finsaas, Reference Klein and Finsaas2017; Olino et al., Reference Olino, Dougherty, Bufferd, Carlson and Klein2014). Families with a 3-year-old child and an English-speaking biological parent were invited to participate. Exclusion criteria were limited to significant medical conditions or developmental disabilities. Out of the 815 families who were invited to participate, 66.4% agreed to enter the study, leaving a final sample of 559 families. One biological parent per family (98.5% mothers) reported on their child's psychopathology symptoms. Informed consent was obtained from the parent prior to participation. Most children were White (94.5%) and non-Hispanic (90.8%). Most of the parents in the sample were married or cohabiting (96%), just over half of the parents (55% of the mothers, 47.1% of the fathers) had at least a four-year college degree, and the median household income was between $70 000 and 90 000. These socio-demographic characteristics, although not representative of the entire U.S. population, are representative of the community from which the sample was drawn (Bufferd, Dougherty, Carlson, & Klein, Reference Bufferd, Dougherty, Carlson and Klein2011).

The total sample included 1253 children (mean age = 4.24, s.d. = 0.64; Table 1), of which 48% were girls and 60% were White. All three studies included follow-up assessments when participants were aged 9–12 years (mean age = 11.06, s.d. = 0.72). The current analysis focused on 410 participants from MAPS, 302 participants from PDS, and 541 participants from SBTS with data on the PAPA (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Erkanli, Keeler, Potts, Walter and Angold2006) in young childhood. In preadolescence, 298 (70%) of MAPS participants, 235 (77%) of PDS participants, and 434 (80%) of SBTS participants completed assessments of psychiatric diagnoses and functioning. There were no differences in socio-demographic characteristics between participants who did and did not complete preadolescent follow-up assessments in MAPS (age: t = −0.12, p = 0.90; sex: χ2 = 2.03, p = 0.15; race/ethnicity: χ2 = 4.27, p = 0.11; poor background: χ2 = 0.58, p = 0.45) and PDS (Gaffrey et al., Reference Gaffrey, Tillman, Barch and Luby2018). SBTS children who completed follow-up assessments were slightly younger than those who were lost at follow-up, but the two groups did not differ on any other socio-demographic characteristics (Finsaas et al., Reference Finsaas, Bufferd, Dougherty, Carlson and Klein2018). Demographic characteristics and rates of psychiatric diagnoses at preschool and preadolescent assessments are given in Table 1.

Measures

Preschool assessments

Across all three study sites, participating parents were administered the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA) by trained interviewers (Egger et al., Reference Egger, Erkanli, Keeler, Potts, Walter and Angold2006). The PAPA is a structured diagnostic interview designed to assess common parent-reported DSM-IV psychiatric disorders in preschoolers in the 3 months before the interview. It covers a comprehensive set of DSM-IV symptoms using questions eliciting developmentally appropriate examples of symptom presentation. From the PAPA, we used individual symptoms for modeling psychopathology dimensions. Since the full PAPA interview was administered in the three samples with small modifications, we first harmonized the data across the three samples to obtain a common set of symptoms (see online Supplementary Material). We also created a composite of PAPA incapacity ratings (withdrawal and discord ratings) across domains of functioning to obtain a global index of preschool functional impairment.

Preadolescent assessments

DSM-IV psychiatric diagnoses were assessed at the preadolescent follow-up assessments using structured interviews: the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children Present and Lifetime (K-SADS-PL; Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci and Ryan1997) in SBTS and MAPS, and the Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA; Angold et al., Reference Angold, Prendergast, Cox, Harrington, Simonoff and Rutter1995) in PDS. Interviews were conducted with participating parents by trained interviewers at all study sites. In SBTS, interviews were also conducted with preadolescent participants, and reports were combined by the interviewer to assign diagnostic status. Our analyses focused on a subset of preadolescent disorders that were assessed in all three samples, namely depressive disorder (major depressive disorder or dysthymia), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), separation anxiety disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), and conduct disorder (CD) in MAPS and PDS. Current diagnoses at the preadolescent follow-up were used in MAPS and PDS, which allowed greater comparability between diagnoses assessed in these samples. In SBTS, due to the low rates of current diagnoses at preadolescence in this population-based sample, we used diagnoses met in the interval between two assessments carried out at age 9 and at age 12 (i.e. interval diagnoses). This yielded a rate of diagnoses that was more comparable to rates in MAPS and PDS (Table 1).

Functional impairment was measured using the total functioning score from the parent-rated MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire (HBQ; Essex et al., Reference Essex, Boyce, Goldstein, Armstrong, Kraemer and Kupfer2002) in MAPS and PDS, and the interviewer-rated Children's Global Assessment Scale (CGAS; Shaffer et al., Reference Shaffer, Gould, Brasic, Ambrosini, Fisher, Bird and Aluwahlia1983) in SBTS.

Statistical analysis

Analyses involved two steps. First, we explicated the hierarchical structure of preschool psychopathology through a latent dimensional approach based on EFA. To increase statistical power and achieve accurate model estimates (Conway, Forbes, & South, Reference Conway, Forbes and South2022), the three samples were combined beforehand through data harmonization and preparation steps (online Supplementary Material). Second, longitudinal analyses tested the prospective association of preschool dimensions with preadolescent psychiatric diagnoses and functional impairment. MAPS and PDS were combined also for these prospective analyses (MAPS + PDS; total follow-up N = 533), whereas SBTS was analyzed separately (follow-up N = 434), due to the aforementioned differences in the available preadolescent measures (i.e. current diagnoses and HBQ functioning in MAPS + PDS; interval diagnoses and CGAS functioning in SBTS).

Factor analysis on preschool symptoms of psychopathology

We examined the factor structure of the individual PAPA symptoms by extracting factor solutions with an increasing number of factors, using exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM) in Mplus (Muthén and Muthén, Los Angeles, CA). Since we did not include model constraints, the results of this ESEM analysis are identical to those from a standard EFA. This exploratory approach was preferred over a confirmatory factor analysis as the number of dimensions and their composition across hierarchical levels in preschoolers was uncertain due to the paucity of relevant literature. The maximum number of factors was determined with parallel analyses, with extraction stopped when eigenvalues fell within the 95% confidence interval of eigenvalues from simulated data (Floyd & Widaman, Reference Floyd and Widaman1995). Since the parallel analysis has a tendency to over-factor, we also examined the interpretability of factor solutions, defined as the presence of at least 4 primary loadings (highest loading ≥0.35 and at least 0.10 greater than all other loadings) for each factor (Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum, & Strahan, Reference Fabrigar, Wegener, MacCallum and Strahan1999; Velicer & Fava, Reference Velicer and Fava1998). Factors were rotated using an oblique rotation (goemin) to allow the extracted dimensions to correlate as expected (Kim & Eaton, Reference Kim and Eaton2015; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019). All factor structures from one to the maximum number of factors were considered.

To map the hierarchical structure and transitions between increasingly complex factor solutions, we correlated factor scores from models with an increasing number of factors, using Goldberg's bass-ackwards method (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg2006). We chose this approach as the primary method for extracting a hierarchy of psychopathology factors because, to our knowledge, it is the only available method to delineate multiple (i.e. >2) levels of a hierarchical structure from factors derived through exploratory factor models. Furthermore, this approach allowed us to compare our results with previous studies that used this method in older children and adults (Allegrini et al., Reference Allegrini, Cheesman, Rimfeld, Selzam, Pingault, Eley and Plomin2020; Conway, Latzman, & Krueger, Reference Conway, Latzman and Krueger2020; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Kotov, Ruggero, Watson, Zimmerman and Krueger2017, Reference Forbes, Sunderland, Rapee, Batterham, Calear, Carragher and Krueger2021b; Kim & Eaton, Reference Kim and Eaton2015; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019; Tackett, Quilty, Sellbom, Rector, & Bagby, Reference Tackett, Quilty, Sellbom, Rector and Bagby2008; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Thomas, Hopwood, Markon, Pincus and Krueger2012). Factors in the derived hierarchical structure can be interpreted as interconnected across hierarchical levels, from a 1-factor level to narrower and more specific dimensions, allowing the examination of how different aspects of psychopathology shift and reorganize from one level to the next (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg2006). As shown in several previous studies of psychopathology (and other constructs, e.g. personality and intelligence, or ‘g’), the general factor from this analytic approach is virtually identical to the general factor from confirmatory bifactor models (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Hicks, Angstadt, Rutherford, Taxali, Hyde and Sripada2021; Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Greene, Levin-Aspenson, Watts, Hallquist, Lahey and Krueger2021a; Fried, Greene, & Eaton, Reference Fried, Greene and Eaton2021; Kim & Eaton, Reference Kim and Eaton2015; Mansolf & Reise, Reference Mansolf and Reise2017; Morgan, Hodge, Wells, & Watkins, Reference Morgan, Hodge, Wells and Watkins2015; Murray & Johnson, Reference Murray and Johnson2013; Murray, Booth, Eisner, Obsuth, & Ribeaud, Reference Murray, Booth, Eisner, Obsuth and Ribeaud2019; van Bork, Epskamp, Rhemtulla, Borsboom, & van der Maas, Reference van Bork, Epskamp, Rhemtulla, Borsboom and van der Maas2017). Since general and specific factors of psychopathology have also previously been estimated using bifactor models (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2014; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Keenan, Stepp, Loeber and Hipwell2015, Reference Lahey, Zald, Perkins, Villalta-Gil, Werts, Van Hulle and Waldman2018; St Clair et al., Reference St Clair, Neufeld, Jones, Fonagy, Bullmore, Dolan and Goodyer2017), we tested an additional confirmatory bifactor model. Each PAPA item was specified to load onto a general factor and one specific factor, based on its primary loading in the factor solution from exploratory models that we identified to include the maximum number of interpretable factors.

Prospective analyses of preschool psychopathology dimensions and preadolescent outcomes

We entered the following variables as consecutive blocks into hierarchical regressions: covariates (sex, preschool age; block 1), presence of any preschool DSM diagnosis (in models predicting preadolescent disorders) or preschool functioning (in models predicting preadolescent functional impairment) (block 2), p factor (block 3), and specific factors (block 4). Logistic regressions were used for psychiatric diagnoses (binary outcomes), whereas linear regressions were used for functional impairment (continuous outcomes). We examined the incremental predictive effects of each block by testing whether the change in R 2 between blocks was significant, reflecting new information not captured in previous blocks. Area under the curve (AUC) from receiver operating characteristic analysis and AUC change are also reported for binary outcomes, but AUCs should be interpreted cautiously, especially for preadolescent disorders with low rates, such as CD (Table 1). Additional analyses in SBTS tested predictors of first-onset psychiatric disorders after removing children with preschool diagnoses. While our primary analyses focused on the p factor and specific factors from the solution with the maximum number of factors, an additional analysis examined the predictive power of intermediate factor solutions. Finally, we examined the predictive power of each dimension (p and specific factors) on its own, testing their associations with preadolescent outcomes while controlling for sex and preschool age. As a sensitivity analysis, all prospective analyses were also repeated in MAPS and PDS separately. Race and ethnicity were not included as covariates in prospective analyses because these variables reflect complex social constructs and their inclusion in prediction models may perpetrate disparities (MacNeill et al., Reference MacNeill, Allen, Poleon, Vargas, Osborne, Damme and Wakschlag2021; Obermeyer, Powers, Vogeli, & Mullainathan, Reference Obermeyer, Powers, Vogeli and Mullainathan2019).

Results

Hierarchical dimensional structure of preschool psychopathology

A total of 76 symptoms were included in the EFA. After examining the results of parallel analyses (Figure S1) and the interpretability of factor solutions, models including 1 to 6 factors were found acceptable (Fig. 1, online Supplementary Table S1), as more complex models included at least one factor with less than 4 primary loadings and showed poor interpretability.

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of the hierarchical structure of preschool psychopathology derived by extracting and correlating factor solutions with an increasing number of factors.

Note: Arrows depict correlations >0.60 with shift across hierarchical levels of more than 2 primary-loading items, indicating a shift in content from a higher-level factor to a lower-level factor. Analyses were run combining all three samples.

Models from 1-factor to 6-factors were arranged in a hierarchical structure, with paths showing substantial correlations between levels (Fig. 1). All correlations within and across hierarchical levels are given in online Supplementary Table S2. The 1-factor structure reflected a general preschool p factor, with strong loadings on most PAPA symptoms (online Supplementary Table S1). The 2-factor solution showed broad internalizing and externalizing factors. In the 3-factor structure, a distress-oppositionality factor (e.g. irritability, tantrums, depression) emerged from the broad internalizing and externalizing factors, alongside fear and inattention-hyperactivity factors. In the 4-factor solution, the distress-oppositionality factor split into distress (e.g. depression, tearfulness) and oppositionality (e.g. tantrums, arguments with adults) factors. In the 5-factor structure, separation anxiety (e.g. distress in absence of caregiver) split from other fear problems (e.g. fear of blood/injections, social anxiety). Finally, in the 6-factor structure, the social anxiety split from fear. This most differentiated interpretable factor solution was used to measure specific preschool dimensions in subsequent prospective analyses.

The additional confirmatory bifactor model yielded general and specific factors that were highly correlated with the factors from EFA (p factors: r = 0.98; specific factors: r = 0.62–0.81, all p < 0.01; online Supplementary Material, Table S3). Thus, only factors from EFA were used in subsequent analyses.

Preschool dimensions and preadolescent outcomes

Psychiatric disorders

Preadolescent psychiatric outcomes were examined with hierarchical logistic regressions. Preschool psychiatric diagnoses were generally weak and inconsistent predictors of preadolescent disorders in both MAPS + PDS and SBTS (Fig. 2; see also online Supplementary Tables S4 and S5 for analyses separating MAPS and PDS). The preschool p factor (1-factor model) prospectively predicted all preadolescent psychiatric diagnoses over and above preschool diagnoses in either MAPS + PDS or SBTS, with significant increases in R 2 ranging between 4% and 15% after adding the p factor (Fig. 2, online Supplementary Table S4). The exceptions were GAD in MAPS + PDS and separation anxiety disorder and ODD in SBTS, where adding the p factor did not significantly increase the R 2.

Fig. 2. Variance in preadolescence disorders explained by preschool DSM diagnoses, general (p) factor, and specific factors of psychopathology.

Notes: Nagelkerke pseudo-R 2 is plotted for preadolescent psychiatric disorders. Asterisks (**p < 0.01, *p < 0.05) indicate that adding a block yielded a statistically significant change in R 2 from the previous block in hierarchical regression models. Depressive disorders and CD were investigated only in MAPS + PDS as too few SBTS participants met the criteria for these conditions in preadolescence. ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; CD, conduct disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; MAPS, Multidimensional Assessment of Preschoolers Study; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; PDS, Preschool Depression Study; SBTS, Stony Brook Temperament Study.

When adding factors solutions with 2 to 6 factors, we broadly found that the greater the number of factors, the greater the variance explained (online Supplementary Table S6). The 6 specific factors accounted for a significantly larger proportion of variance in preadolescent disorders over the p factor and DSM diagnoses, with R 2 increasing 6–16% after adding specific factors (Fig. 2, online Supplementary Table S4). The only preadolescent disorders not showing a significant improvement in R 2 when adding the 6-factor model were separation anxiety disorder and ADHD across samples, and GAD in SBTS.

Results in SBTS excluding participants with preschool diagnoses were largely consistent (online Supplementary Material, Table S4).

In models that examined each factor individually, higher scores on the preschool p factor were prospectively associated with all preadolescent disorders [odds ratios (ORs) = 1.87–6.04, p < 0.05], except GAD and ODD in SBTS (Table 2; online Supplementary Table S7 for analyses separating MAPS and PDS). Specific preschool dimensions showed few significant effects in SBTS, but numerous effects in MAPS + PDS (Tables 2, online Supplementary Table S7). Higher preschool distress, separation anxiety, inattention-hyperactivity, and oppositionality factors were predictive of most future diagnoses, whereas the fear and social anxiety factors showed more specific associations (Table 2).

Table 2. Bivariate associations of preschool psychopathology dimensions (rows) with preadolescent psychiatric and functional outcomes (columns)

ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; CD, conduct disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; MAPS, Multidimensional Assessment of Preschoolers Study; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; OR, odds ratio; PDS, Preschool Depression Study; SBTS, Stony Brook Temperament Study.

Notes: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. All analyses were run on standardized variables and controlled for sex and preschool age. CGAF functioning scores were reverse-coded, such that higher scores indicate worse functioning, similar to the HBQ scores in MAPS + PDS. Depressive disorders and CD were investigated only in MAPS + PDS as too few SBTS participants met the criteria for these conditions.

Functioning

Functional outcomes were examined in hierarchical linear regressions. The preschool p factor prospectively predicted preadolescent functional impairment over and above preschool functioning (significant R 2 increase of 7% in MAPS + PDS and of 1% in SBTS; Figure 2, online Supplementary Tables S4–S5). Adding the 6-factor solution produced a further significant R 2 increase (4% in MAPS + PDS and 5% in SBTS). R 2 also increased, albeit not always significantly, when a greater number of factors were included beyond the p factor (online Supplementary Table S6). As a control analysis, we ran hierarchical regressions to test whether preschool DSM diagnoses (entered as block 3) predicted preadolescent functional impairments controlling for preschool age and sex (block 1) and preschool functioning (block 2). Preschool diagnoses were significant predictors beyond preschool functioning in SBTS (R 2 increase of 1%), but not in MAPS + PDS (online Supplementary Tables S4–S5). However, these models including DSM diagnoses explained a significantly smaller proportion of variance in preadolescence functioning than models including general and specific factors (MAPS + PDS: Hotelling's t = 4.24, p < 0.001; SBTS: Hotelling's t = 2.29, p = .02).

In analyses examining each factor individually, higher scores on the p factor and all specific dimensions prospectively predicted worse functioning in preadolescence (standardized β = 0.14–0.38, p < 0.05), except for social anxiety in MAPS + PDS and fear in both MAPS + PDS and SBTS (Tables 2, online Supplementary Table S5).

Discussion

The current study represents the most comprehensive examination to date of the structure and predictive validity of transdiagnostic hierarchical dimensions of psychopathology in early childhood. We identified a dimensional hierarchy with the p factor at the apex and six lower-level specific factors, capturing distress, fear, social anxiety, separation anxiety, inattention-hyperactivity, and oppositionality. Higher levels of the p factor and specific factors prospectively predicted new onsets of psychiatric disorders as well as functional impairment in preadolescence – almost a decade later – over and above preschool DSM diagnoses and functioning. These findings highlight the value of conceptualizing preschool psychopathology using data-driven hierarchical models of dimensional phenotypes that capture individual differences in severity for forecasting important clinical and functional outcomes. The identified preschool dimensions of psychopathology may be used to guide future early identification and prevention strategies to reduce the long-lasting impact of preschool psychiatric symptoms.

Our study delineated a multi-level transdiagnostic hierarchical structure of psychopathology, with a p factor at the top (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2014; Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Applegate, Hakes, Zald, Hariri and Rathouz2012; Michelini et al., Reference Michelini, Barch, Tian, Watson, Klein and Kotov2019), which separated into the broad externalizing and internalizing spectra in the 2-factor structure (Achenbach et al., Reference Achenbach, Ivanova and Rescorla2017). The broad externalizing factor progressively split into oppositionality and inattention-hyperactivity factors, whereas the broad internalizing factors split into specific distress, fear, separation anxiety, and social anxiety factors. These findings extend the few prior studies investigating both general and specific dimensions in early childhood, where fewer specific factors were specified a-priori, using a bifactor approach (McElroy et al., Reference McElroy, Belsky, Carragher, Fearon and Patalay2018; Olino et al., Reference Olino, Dougherty, Bufferd, Carlson and Klein2014). Most of the specific dimensions identified by our data-driven approach captured symptoms belonging to specific diagnostic categories, whereas the distress factor aggregated symptoms of both depressive disorders and GAD. These findings in preschoolers are broadly consistent with previous studies of older children and adults, which converged in the HiTOP model (Kotov et al., Reference Kotov, Krueger, Watson, Achenbach, Althoff, Bagby and Zimmerman2017), and support the use of transdiagnostic hierarchical approaches across development (Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Rapee and Krueger2019; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Roberts, Flynn, Smith, Krogh-Jespersen, Kaat and Davis2019; Wilson & Olino, Reference Wilson and Olino2021).

Using preadolescent follow-up assessments, we compared the ability of the preschool p factor and specific psychopathology dimensions to predict future clinical and functional outcomes. We found weak and inconsistent evidence that preschool DSM diagnoses predicted preadolescent disorders, as indicated by the small portion of variance explained beyond covariates. Conversely, the preschool p factor predicted preadolescent disorders over and above preschool diagnoses, with only a few exceptions for GAD in MAPS + PDS and separation anxiety disorder and ODD in SBTS, suggesting that general preschool psychopathology severity may have limited predictive power in these outcomes beyond the presence of a preschool diagnosis. Further, specific dimensions significantly predicted preadolescent disorders beyond p, except for preadolescent separation anxiety disorder and ADHD. A similar pattern of findings emerged in predicting preadolescent functional impairment, where both the preschool p factor and specific dimensions were significant predictors even when controlling for preschool functioning and to a greater extent than preschool DSM diagnoses. These results in early childhood are consistent with initial findings on the predictive validity of the p factor at school age (Pettersson et al., Reference Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson and Lichtenstein2018) and with models conceptualizing the p factor as a common liability to all form of psychopathology (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Moffitt2014; Lahey, Krueger, Rathouz, Waldman, & Zald, Reference Lahey, Krueger, Rathouz, Waldman and Zald2017).

Our novel findings have two important clinical implications. First, assessment of both general and specific dimensions of preschool psychopathology offers greater prognostic utility than binary DSM diagnoses, supporting a shift away from the exclusive use of diagnostic categories for predicting long-term outcomes. This is because these broad dimensions reflect the continuous nature of psychopathology and can accommodate subthreshold presentations. Second, early interventions and prevention strategies targeting preschool general psychopathology may be able to avert the emergence of various psychiatric disorders over the next decade, but targeting specific dimensions may improve certain outcomes further. Future intervention studies should test whether a transdiagnostic stepped-care approach for preschool children, firstly aiming to reduce general psychopathology and secondly targeting more specific problems, may represent an effective strategy for preventing subsequent psychiatric conditions throughout development (Forbes et al., Reference Forbes, Rapee and Krueger2019; Wakschlag et al., Reference Wakschlag, Roberts, Flynn, Smith, Krogh-Jespersen, Kaat and Davis2019).

The present study has the following limitations. First, the preschool dimensions of psychopathology were derived from diagnostic assessments based on one informant (usually mothers). Although this limitation is common to much of the existing literature in early childhood, future research should confirm these findings using additional informants, such as fathers and teachers, or observational assessments. Second, our study aggregated data from three samples recruited with different methods and assessed with partly overlapping measures. Different findings for some preadolescent disorders in SBTS (community sample, predominantly middle-class and White) v. MAPS + PDS (psychopathology-enriched samples, socio-demographically and racially diverse) may be explained by differences in sample composition. Nevertheless, our multi-sample approach yielded a large sample size, necessary for factor analysis of a large number of symptoms, and provided convergent findings across different methodologies and recruitment strategies. Finally, our study only focused on one follow-up point in preadolescence, which was available in all three studies. Future studies examining multiple follow-up points will be helpful for clarifying how the predictive power of preschool dimensions changes for outcomes in different developmental periods.

Conclusion

Our study provides novel findings on the hierarchical dimensional structure of preschool psychopathology in a large and diverse sample of young children. The identified p and specific dimensions show substantial predictive validity with regard to important clinical and functional outcomes in preadolescence, up to almost a decade later. Of note, these dimensions were substantially more predictive of outcomes than DSM diagnoses, although both dimensions and diagnoses were derived from the same preschool interview. An important next step will be to translate these findings into clinical practice, for example through the development of clinical cutoffs and predictive algorithms in independent samples. Future efforts should also test whether targeting the identified hierarchical dimensions through preventive interventions may improve the developmental trajectories of preschool children at risk for persistent psychopathology.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172200246X.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the families who took part in the three cohorts harmonized in this study, without whom this work would not be possible. We also gratefully acknowledge the many students, staff, and collaborators who have contributed to the cohorts harmonized here, particularly Dr Margaret J. Briggs-Gowan.

Financial support

This work was supported by NIMH grants R01MH069942 (PI Klein), R01MH064769 (PI Luby), R01MH090876 (PIs Luby and Barch), R01MH082830 (PI Wakschlag), 2U01MH082830 (PI Wakschlag), R01MH121877 (PI Wakschlag), and U01MH090301 (PI Briggs-Gowan). Dr Michelini was funded by a NARSAD Young Investigator Award from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (28566) and by a Klingenstein Third Generation Foundation Fellowship (20212999).

Conflicts of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Achenbach, T. M., Ivanova, M. Y., & Rescorla, L. A. (2017). Empirically based assessment and taxonomy of psychopathology for ages 1½–90 + years: Developmental, multi-informant, and multicultural findings. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 79, 418. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.03.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allegrini, A. G., Cheesman, R., Rimfeld, K., Selzam, S., Pingault, J.-B., Eley, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2020). The p factor: Genetic analyses support a general dimension of psychopathology in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(1), 3039. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., Farmer, E. M., Burns, B. J., & Erkanli, A. (1999). Impaired but undiagnosed. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(2), 129137. doi:10.1097/00004583-199902000-00011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angold, A., Prendergast, M., Cox, A., Harrington, R., Simonoff, E., & Rutter, M. (1995). The child and adolescent psychiatric assessment (CAPA). Psychological Medicine, 25(4), 739753. doi:10.1017/s003329170003498xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bufferd, S. J., Dougherty, L. R., Carlson, G. A., & Klein, D. N. (2011). Parent-reported mental health in preschoolers: Findings using a diagnostic interview. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 52(4), 359369. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2010.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carragher, N., Teesson, M., Sunderland, M., Newton, N. C., Krueger, R. F., Conrod, P. J., … Slade, T. (2016). The structure of adolescent psychopathology: A symptom-level analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46(5), 981994. (26620582). doi:10.1017/s0033291715002470CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Ambler, A., Danese, A., Elliott, M. L., Hariri, A., … Moffitt, T. E. (2020). Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin birth cohort study. JAMA Network Open, 3(4), e203221. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3221CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., … Moffitt, T. E. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137. (25360393). doi:10.1177/2167702613497473CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, D. A., Hicks, B. M., Angstadt, M., Rutherford, S., Taxali, A., Hyde, L., … Sripada, C. (2021). The general factor of psychopathology in the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: A comparison of alternative modeling approaches. Clinical Psychological Science, 9(2), 169182. doi:10.1177/2167702620959317CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, C. C., Forbes, M. K., & South, S. C. (2022). A hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP) primer for mental health researchers. Clinical Psychological Science, 10(2), 236258. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026211017834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, C. C., Latzman, R. D., & Krueger, R. F. (2020). A meta-structural model of common clinical disorder and personality disorder symptoms. Journal of Personality Disorders, 34(1), 88106. doi:10.1521/pedi_2019_33_383CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N. (2014). The RDoC framework: Facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry, 13(1), 2835. (24497240). doi:10.1002/wps.20087CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougherty, L. R., Leppert, K. A., Merwin, S. M., Smith, V. C., Bufferd, S. J., & Kushner, M. R. (2015). Advances and directions in preschool mental health research. Child Development Perspectives, 9(1), 1419. doi:10.1111/cdep.12099CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2006). Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3–4), 313337. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, H. L., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., Potts, E., Walter, B. K., & Angold, A. (2006). Test-retest reliability of the preschool age psychiatric assessment (PAPA). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(5), 538549. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000205705.71194.b8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Essex, M. J., Boyce, W. T., Goldstein, L. H., Armstrong, J. M., Kraemer, H. C., & Kupfer, D. J., & MacArthur Assessment Battery Working Group. (2002). The confluence of mental, physical, social, and academic difficulties in middle childhood. II: Developing the Macarthur health and behavior questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(5), 588603. doi:10.1097/00004583-200205000-00017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4(3), 272299. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.4.3.272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finsaas, M. C., Bufferd, S. J., Dougherty, L. R., Carlson, G. A., & Klein, D. N. (2018). Preschool psychiatric disorders: Homotypic and heterotypic continuity through middle childhood and early adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 48(13), 21592168. doi:10.1017/S0033291717003646CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floyd, F. J., & Widaman, K. F. (1995). Factor analysis in the development and refinement of clinical assessment instruments. Psychological Assessment, 7(3), 286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, M. K., Greene, A. L., Levin-Aspenson, H. F., Watts, A. L., Hallquist, M., Lahey, B. B., … Krueger, R. F. (2021a). Three recommendations based on a comparison of the reliability and validity of the predominant models used in research on the empirical structure of psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 130(3), 297317. doi:10.1037/abn0000533CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, M. K., Kotov, R., Ruggero, C. J., Watson, D., Zimmerman, M., & Krueger, R. F. (2017). Delineating the joint hierarchical structure of clinical and personality disorders in an outpatient psychiatric sample. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 79, 1930. (28495022). doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2017.04.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, M. K., Rapee, R. M., & Krueger, R. F. (2019). Opportunities for the prevention of mental disorders by reducing general psychopathology in early childhood. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 119, 103411. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2019.103411CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, M. K., Sunderland, M., Rapee, R. M., Batterham, P. J., Calear, A. L., Carragher, N., … Krueger, R. F. (2021b). A detailed hierarchical model of psychopathology: From individual symptoms up to the general factor of psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science, 9(2), 139168. doi:10.1177/2167702620954799CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, M. K., Tackett, J. L., Markon, K. E., & Krueger, R. F. (2016). Beyond comorbidity: Toward a dimensional and hierarchical approach to understanding psychopathology across the life span. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt1), 971986. doi:10.1017/S0954579416000651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, E. I., Greene, A. L., & Eaton, N. R. (2021). The p factor is the sum of its parts, for now. World Psychiatry, 20(1), 6970. doi:10.1002/wps.20814CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaffrey, M. S., Tillman, R., Barch, D. M., & Luby, J. L. (2018). Continuity and stability of preschool depression from childhood through adolescence and following the onset of puberty. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 86, 3946. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.07.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, L. R. (2006). Doing it all bass-ackwards: The development of hierarchical factor structures from the top down. Journal of Research in Personality, 40(4), 347358. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.01.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 13371345. (11699809). doi:10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heflinger, C. A., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2010). Stigma in child and adolescent mental health services research: Understanding professional and institutional stigmatization of youth with mental health problems and their families. Administration and Policy in Mental Health, 37(1–2), 6170. doi:10.1007/s10488-010-0294-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., … Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. (9204677). doi:10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602. (15939837). doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H., & Eaton, N. R. (2015). The hierarchical structure of common mental disorders: Connecting multiple levels of comorbidity, bifactor models, and predictive validity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(4), 10641078. doi:10.1037/abn0000113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, D. N., & Finsaas, M. C. (2017). The stony brook temperament study: Early antecedents and pathways to emotional disorders. Child Development Perspectives, 11(4), 257263. (29151849). doi:10.1111/cdep.12242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Watson, D., Achenbach, T. M., Althoff, R. R., Bagby, R. M., … Zimmerman, M. (2017). The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): A dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(4), 454477. (28333488). doi:10.1037/abn0000258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., Hakes, J. K., Zald, D. H., Hariri, A. R., & Rathouz, P. J. (2012). Is there a general factor of prevalent psychopathology during adulthood? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4), 971977. (22845652). doi:10.1037/a0028355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, B. B., Krueger, R. F., Rathouz, P. J., Waldman, I. D., & Zald, D. H. (2017). A hierarchical causal taxonomy of psychopathology across the life span. Psychological Bulletin, 143(2), 142186. (28004947). doi:10.1037/bul0000069CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., Keenan, K., Stepp, S. D., Loeber, R., & Hipwell, A. E. (2015). Criterion validity of the general factor of psychopathology in a prospective study of girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(4), 415422. (25052460). doi:10.1111/jcpp.12300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lahey, B. B., Zald, D. H., Perkins, S. F., Villalta-Gil, V., Werts, K. B., Van Hulle, C. A., … Waldman, I. D. (2018). Measuring the hierarchical general factor model of psychopathology in young adults. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 27(1), e1593. doi:10.1002/mpr.1593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J., Allen, N., Estabrook, R., Pine, D. S., Rogers, C., Krogh-Jespersen, S., … Wakschlag, L. (2019). Mapping infant neurodevelopmental precursors of mental disorders: How synthetic cohorts & computational approaches can be used to enhance prediction of early childhood psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 123, 103484. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2019.103484CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J., Heffelfinger, A., Mrakotsky, C., & Hildebrand, T. (1999). Preschool feelings checklist. St. Louis, MO: Washington University School of Medicine.Google Scholar
Luby, J. L. (2012). Dispelling the “they'll grow out of it” myth: Implications for intervention. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(11), 11271129. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12081037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Gaffrey, M. S., Tillman, R., April, L. M., & Belden, A. C. (2014). Trajectories of preschool disorders to full DSM depression at school age and early adolescence: Continuity of preschool depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(7), 768776. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13091198CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Si, X., Belden, A. C., Tandon, M., & Spitznagel, E. (2009). Preschool depression: Homotypic continuity and course over 24 months. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(8), 897905. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacNeill, L. A., Allen, N. B., Poleon, R. B., Vargas, T., Osborne, K. J., Damme, K. S. F., … Wakschlag, L. S. (2021). Translating RDoC to real-world impact in developmental psychopathology: A neurodevelopmental framework for application of mental health risk calculators. Development and Psychopathology, 33(5), 16651684. doi:10.1017/s0954579421000651CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mansolf, M., & Reise, S. P. (2017). When and why the second-order and bifactor models are distinguishable. Intelligence, 61, 120129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McElroy, E., Belsky, J., Carragher, N., Fearon, P., & Patalay, P. (2018). Developmental stability of general and specific factors of psychopathology from early childhood to adolescence: Dynamic mutualism or p-differentiation? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(6), 667675. (29197107). doi:10.1111/jcpp.12849CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G., Barch, D. M., Tian, Y., Watson, D., Klein, D. N., & Kotov, R. (2019). Delineating and validating higher-order dimensions of psychopathology in the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study. Translational Psychiatry, 9(1), 261. (31624235). doi:10.1038/s41398-019-0593-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michelini, G., Palumbo, I. M., DeYoung, C. G., Latzman, R. D., & Kotov, R. (2021). Linking RDoC and HiTOP: A new interface for advancing psychiatric nosology and neuroscience. Clinical Psychology Review, 86, 102025. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, G. B., Hodge, K. J., Wells, K. E., & Watkins, M. W. (2015). Are fit indices biased in favor of bi-factor models in cognitive ability research?: A comparison of fit in correlated factors, higher-order, and bi-factor models via Monte Carlo simulations. Journal of Intelligence, 3(1), 220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, A. L., Booth, T., Eisner, M., Obsuth, I., & Ribeaud, D. (2019). Quantifying the strength of general factors in psychopathology: A comparison of CFA with maximum likelihood estimation, BSEM, and ESEM/EFA bifactor approaches. Journal of Personality Assessment, 101(6), 631643. doi:10.1080/00223891.2018.1468338CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. L., & Johnson, W. (2013). The limitations of model fit in comparing the bi-factor versus higher-order models of human cognitive ability structure. Intelligence, 41(5), 407422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Obermeyer, Z., Powers, B., Vogeli, C., & Mullainathan, S. (2019). Dissecting racial bias in an algorithm used to manage the health of populations. Science, 366(6464), 447453. doi:10.1126/science.aax2342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olino, T. M., Bufferd, S. J., Dougherty, L. R., Dyson, M. W., Carlson, G. A., & Klein, D. N. (2018). The development of latent dimensions of psychopathology across early childhood: Stability of dimensions and moderators of change. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(7), 13731383. doi:10.1007/s10802-018-0398-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olino, T. M., Dougherty, L. R., Bufferd, S. J., Carlson, G. A., & Klein, D. N. (2014). Testing models of psychopathology in preschool-aged children using a structured interview-based assessment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(7), 12011211. (24652485). doi:10.1007/s10802-014-9865-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettersson, E., Lahey, B. B., Larsson, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2018). Criterion validity and utility of the general factor of psychopathology in childhood: Predictive associations with independently measured severe adverse mental health outcomes in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(6), 372383. (29859553). doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2017.12.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaffer, D., Gould, M. S., Brasic, J., Ambrosini, P., Fisher, P., Bird, H., & Aluwahlia, S. (1983). A children's global assessment scale (CGAS). Archives of General Psychiatry, 40(11), 12281231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St Clair, M. C., Neufeld, S., Jones, P. B., Fonagy, P., Bullmore, E. T., Dolan, R. J., … Goodyer, I. M. (2017). Characterising the latent structure and organisation of self-reported thoughts, feelings and behaviours in adolescents and young adults. PLoS ONE, 12(4), e0175381. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0175381CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterba, S., Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2007). Diagnostic specificity and nonspecificity in the dimensions of preschool psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(10), 10051013. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01770.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tackett, J. L., Quilty, L. C., Sellbom, M., Rector, N. A., & Bagby, R. M. (2008). Additional evidence for a quantitative hierarchical model of mood and anxiety disorders for DSM-V: The context of personality structure. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(4), 812825. (19025228). doi:10.1037/a0013795CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Bork, R., Epskamp, S., Rhemtulla, M., Borsboom, D., & van der Maas, H. L. (2017). What is the p-factor of psychopathology? Some risks of general factor modeling. Theory & Psychology, 27(6), 759773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velicer, W. F., & Fava, J. L. (1998). Affects of variable and subject sampling on factor pattern recovery. Psychological Methods, 3(2), 231251. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.3.2.231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wakschlag, L. S., Choi, S. W., Carter, A. S., Hullsiek, H., Burns, J., McCarthy, K., … Briggs-Gowan, M. J. (2012). Defining the developmental parameters of temper loss in early childhood: Implications for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(11), 10991108. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02595.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakschlag, L. S., Roberts, M. Y., Flynn, R. M., Smith, J. D., Krogh-Jespersen, S., Kaat, A. J., … Davis, M. M. (2019). Future directions for early childhood prevention of mental disorders: A roadmap to mental health, earlier. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 48(3), 539554. doi:10.1080/15374416.2018.1561296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wichstrøm, L., & Berg-Nielsen, T. S. (2014). Psychiatric disorders in preschoolers: The structure of DSM-IV symptoms and profiles of comorbidity. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(7), 551562. doi:10.1007/s00787-013-0486-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiggins, J. L., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Brotman, M. A., Leibenluft, E., & Wakschlag, L. S. (2021). Toward a developmental nosology for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder in early childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(3), 388397. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2020.04.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, S., & Olino, T. M. (2021). A developmental perspective on personality and psychopathology across the life span. Journal of Personality, 89(5), 915932. doi:10.1111/jopy.12623CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research (10th ed.). Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Wright, A. G., Thomas, K. M., Hopwood, C. J., Markon, K. E., Pincus, A. L., & Krueger, R. F. (2012). The hierarchical structure of DSM-5 pathological personality traits. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4), 951957. (22448740). doi:10.1037/a0027669CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and rates of psychiatric diagnoses in each sample

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of the hierarchical structure of preschool psychopathology derived by extracting and correlating factor solutions with an increasing number of factors.Note: Arrows depict correlations >0.60 with shift across hierarchical levels of more than 2 primary-loading items, indicating a shift in content from a higher-level factor to a lower-level factor. Analyses were run combining all three samples.

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Variance in preadolescence disorders explained by preschool DSM diagnoses, general (p) factor, and specific factors of psychopathology.Notes: Nagelkerke pseudo-R2 is plotted for preadolescent psychiatric disorders. Asterisks (**p < 0.01, *p < 0.05) indicate that adding a block yielded a statistically significant change in R2 from the previous block in hierarchical regression models. Depressive disorders and CD were investigated only in MAPS + PDS as too few SBTS participants met the criteria for these conditions in preadolescence. ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; CD, conduct disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; MAPS, Multidimensional Assessment of Preschoolers Study; ODD, oppositional defiant disorder; PDS, Preschool Depression Study; SBTS, Stony Brook Temperament Study.

Figure 3

Table 2. Bivariate associations of preschool psychopathology dimensions (rows) with preadolescent psychiatric and functional outcomes (columns)

Supplementary material: File

Michelini et al. supplementary material

Michelini et al. supplementary material

Download Michelini et al. supplementary material(File)
File 170.2 KB