Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-9knnw Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-04T23:57:02.526Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evidence on antidepressant withdrawal: an appraisal and reanalysis of a recent systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 July 2025

Joanna Moncrieff*
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/02jx3x895 University College London , London, UK Research and Development Department, https://ror.org/023e5m798North East London NHS Foundation Trust (NELFT), Essex, UK
Harriet Hobday
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/02jx3x895 University College London , London, UK
Anders Sørensen
Affiliation:
Copenhagen Trial Unit, Centre for Clinical Intervention Research, The Capital Region, Copenhagen University Hospital – Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen, Denmark
John Read
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology, University of East London, London, UK
Martin Plöderl
Affiliation:
University Clinic of Psychiatry Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Paracelsus Medical University Salzburg, Austria
Michael Hengartner
Affiliation:
Department of Applied Psychology, https://ror.org/049c2kr37Kalaidos University of Applied Sciences Zurich, Switzerland
Caroline Kamp
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark Department of Regional Health Research, The Faculty of Heath Sciences, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
Janus Jakobsen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark Department of Regional Health Research, The Faculty of Heath Sciences, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark
Sophie Juul
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark Department of Regional Health Research, The Faculty of Heath Sciences, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark Stolpegaard Psychotherapy Centre, Mental Health Services in the Capital Region of Denmark, Gentofte, Denmark
James Davies
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Anthropology and Psychology, https://ror.org/043071f54University of Roehampton, London, UK
Mark Horowitz
Affiliation:
Research and Development Department, https://ror.org/023e5m798North East London NHS Foundation Trust (NELFT), Essex, UK Institute of Pharmaceutical Sciences, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Joanna Moncrieff; Email: j.moncrieff@ucl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

There has been debate about the frequency and severity of antidepressant withdrawal effects.

Methods

We set out to appraise and reanalyze an influential systematic review by Henssler and colleagues that concluded that withdrawal effects are not particularly common and rarely severe. We repeated the meta-analysis, including only studies where data were derived from systematic measures of withdrawal symptoms.

Results

Most data in the Henssler review are derived from pharmaceutical industry–sponsored efficacy studies in which withdrawal was a minor consideration. Shortcomings of the review include the use of spontaneously reported adverse events to estimate withdrawal symptoms, potential misclassification of withdrawal symptoms as relapse, inclusion of data from retrospective case-note studies, short duration of prior antidepressant use, short observation periods, the overlooking of differences between placebo and drug withdrawal effects, and the use of questionable proxies for severe withdrawal. There were also discrepancies and uncertainties in some figures used. In our reanalysis, we included only the five studies that used a systematic and relevant method to assess the incidence of any withdrawal symptom. Prior treatment was short-term (12 weeks or less) in all but one of these. The pooled percentage was 55% (95% confidence interval, CI, 31% to 81%; N = 601) without subtracting nocebo effects, with high heterogeneity.

Conclusions

Henssler’s review is based on unreliable data and does not provide an adequate basis for the evaluation of antidepressant withdrawal effects. Further good-quality research on antidepressant withdrawal is required.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Antidepressants are widely prescribed, and their use is increasing across the globe (Alabaku et al., Reference Alabaku, Yang, Tharmarajah, Suda, Vigod and Tadrous2023). Almost a fifth of the population of the UK and the US use an antidepressant each year, and people frequently take them for long periods. Fifty per cent of users in the UK have taken them for more than a year and almost the same proportion in the US for more than five years (Brody & Gu, Reference Brody and Gu2020; NHS Digital, 2023; Public Health England, 2019).

Initially, antidepressant withdrawal symptoms were generally thought to be mild and short-lived (Iacobucci, Reference Iacobucci2019; Sørensen, Jørgensen, & Munkholm, Reference Sørensen, Jørgensen and Munkholm2022a). However, a review published in 2019 suggested withdrawal symptoms occurred in 56% of participants across included studies and that nearly half of those reported the symptoms to be severe (Davies & Read, Reference Davies and Read2019a). Some guidelines were updated to reflect this evidence (Burn, Horowitz, Roycroft, & Taylor, Reference Burn, Horowitz, Roycroft and Taylor2020; Iacobucci, Reference Iacobucci2019; NICE, 2022b).

However, there was debate about the results of this review (Davies & Read, Reference Davies and Read2019b; Jauhar & Hayes, Reference Jauhar and Hayes2019). Subsequently, an influential review by Henssler et al. (Reference Henssler, Schmidt, Schmidt, Schwarzer, Bschor and Baethge2024) appeared to suggest that antidepressant withdrawal effects might be less common and only rarely severe.

On the basis of data from 62 cohorts from randomized trials and other studies, Henssler et al. estimated the incidence of ‘any’ withdrawal symptom to be 31% among those coming off an antidepressant. By deducting the incidence of symptoms following the discontinuation of placebo in an overlapping set of 22 trials, they concluded ‘the frequency of antidepressant discontinuation symptoms to be in the range of approximately 15%, thus affecting about one in six to one in seven patients’ (p. 534) (Henssler et al., Reference Henssler, Schmidt, Schmidt, Schwarzer, Bschor and Baethge2024). They reported that only 3% of participants experienced severe withdrawal symptoms.

Henssler et al. claimed to have provided ‘a more comprehensive view’ than previous research (p. 534)(Henssler et al., Reference Henssler, Schmidt, Schmidt, Schwarzer, Bschor and Baethge2024).

Since this review was published, another systematic review produced a pooled incidence rate of 43% in mostly short-term trials (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Tan, Zheng, Zeng, Li, Horowitz and Li2024). To shed light on the seeming discrepancies between reviews and to inform clinicians and patients about the current evidence, we set out to appraise the Henssler et al.’s review, including the nature and quality of the data it included. As a secondary aim, we planned a meta-analysis of the occurrence of any withdrawal symptom in studies included by Henssler et al. that had applied a systematic and relevant assessment of withdrawal symptoms.

Methods

We inspected the original publications on the 62 study cohorts (some studies involved more than one cohort) included in Henssler et al.’s incidence analysis and the 19 studies included in the analysis of severe withdrawal. We extracted data on details not reported in the original review, including the method of assessment of withdrawal symptoms used in the analysis, conflicts of interest and sponsorship, and the potential for the misclassification of withdrawal symptoms as relapse or deterioration of the underlying condition. We also re-extracted data on the occurrence of withdrawal as defined by Henssler et al. (at least one withdrawal symptom). All data extraction was double-checked.

The possibility of misclassification was evaluated after inspecting the included studies according to the following criteria: ‘high potential’ for misclassification where data used to evaluate withdrawal were collected non-systematically and concurrently with measures of psychiatric symptoms or relapse, and there was evidence suggestive of misclassification (such as the non-inclusion of typical emotional symptoms among reported effects of withdrawal); ‘medium potential’ where misclassification may have influenced ratings of withdrawal; and ‘low potential’ where misclassification was judged not likely to have been a significant problem. Further details about the basis of these judgments are provided in the Supplementary Table.

We reanalyzed studies included in the Henssler et al.’s review according to a predefined analysis plan (see Supplementary material). We included studies that had usable data derived from a systematic and relevant method of assessment of withdrawal symptoms. We defined this as the use of a structured questionnaire or method that captured common withdrawal symptoms. We excluded studies that used measures designed for other purposes that did not cover withdrawal symptoms. Henssler et al. kindly supplied clarifications of the origin of their figures in certain instances. As in the original review, we analyzed the proportion of people who entered the study who reported at least one withdrawal symptom and conducted the meta-analysis of proportions, using the Logit method based on the inverse variance. We used R’s ‘meta’ package. Details of the code are available at the Open Science Foundation (OSF) https://osf.io/de3gj.

Results

In examining Henssler’s review, we identified several strengths, including the use of systematic searches, risk of bias assessments, the evaluation of withdrawal symptoms in people withdrawn from placebo, and the exploration of potential predictors of withdrawal. However, we also identified some significant limitations, many of which were not readily apparent in the published paper. These include the use of spontaneously reported adverse events to estimate withdrawal symptoms (including in many studies that also used a structured instrument), potential misclassification of withdrawal symptoms as relapse, inclusion of data from retrospective case-note studies, short duration of prior antidepressant use in many studies, short observation periods, lack of consideration of differences between placebo and drug withdrawal effects, and the use of questionable proxies for severe withdrawal. There were also some significant discrepancies and uncertainties in the figures used.

Design of studies

The majority of the 62 studies or cohorts included in the incidence analysis were acute efficacy studies, extension studies, or relapse prevention studies, in which withdrawal effects were an incidental concern and not reliably measured (see below). Only 16 (26%) were designed primarily to study withdrawal, and these were mostly small (See Table 1).

Table 1. Characteristics of studies used in Henssler et al.’s analysis of the incidence of any withdrawal symptom (eFigure 1 in Henssler’s supplementary appendix)

Abbreviations: GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; OCD, obsessive compulsive disorder; SSRI, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor; SNRI, serotonin and noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor; OL, Open Label; DESS, Discontinuation-Emergent Signs and Symptoms.

a We have classified studies according to the aim of the original study from which the data were gathered. Some papers focus on data on withdrawal from studies that were set up with a different aim, hence we have classified them as ‘secondary’ whereas Henssler et al. classified them as ‘primary’.

b ‘High’ = high potential for misclassification because withdrawal and relapse/psychiatric symptoms measured concurrently with evidence suggestive of misclassification (such as the noninclusion of typical emotional symptoms among reported effects of withdrawal); ‘Medium’ = medium potential for misclassification where misclassification may have influenced ratings of withdrawal; ‘Low’ = misclassification not likely to have been a significant problem. See Supplementary Table S1 for more detailed rationale for individual studies.

c Some or all participants did not stop their antidepressant.

Forty-six (74%) of the 62 studies had definite or probable funding from a pharmaceutical company (Table 1). Since funded studies were larger than non-funded studies, they accounted for 96.2% (12,119/12,603) of the participants included in the analysis.

Assessment of withdrawal effects

A fundamental problem with the review is the manner of assessment of withdrawal symptoms (Table 1). In 52 of the 62 study cohorts, figures were derived from data on adverse events, responses to open questions, clinician judgment, or no method was specified. Where details were provided, adverse events and symptoms were ‘spontaneously reported’ in all but one study. In this study, specific withdrawal symptoms were enquired about during the measurement of adverse events (Tourian et al., Reference Tourian, Padmanabhan, Groark, Brisard and Farrington2009). Whether adverse events counted as withdrawal effects was further determined by the subjective judgment of the researchers who decided ‘if they occurred for the first time or worsened following discontinuation of treatment ’(p. 208) (Perahia, Kajdasz, Desaiah, & Haddad, Reference Perahia, Kajdasz, Desaiah and Haddad2005).

It is known that the detection of adverse events in studies designed to evaluate efficacy is unreliable, inconsistent, and likely to underestimate effects (Chrysant, Reference Chrysant2008; Hammad, Pinheiro, & Neyarapally, Reference Hammad, Pinheiro and Neyarapally2011; Phillips, Hazell, Sauzet, & Cornelius, Reference Phillips, Hazell, Sauzet and Cornelius2019). Ratings show poor reliability even for physical symptoms (Forster, Taljaard, Bennett, & van Walraven, Reference Forster, Taljaard, Bennett and van Walraven2012) and when raters are guided by a list of specific symptoms (Atkinson et al., Reference Atkinson, Li, Coffey, Sit, Shaw, Lavene and Basch2012). In a trial of a chiropractic intervention, 88 times more adverse events were identified using proactive monitoring than when relying on spontaneous reports (Pohlman et al., Reference Pohlman, Carroll, Tsuyuki, Hartling and Vohra2020). Subjective adverse effects, including symptoms such as fatigue and emotional changes, are more likely to be under-detected than objective physical signs such as oedema (Chrysant, Reference Chrysant2008). The reporting, as well as the detection of adverse effects in such studies, is also unreliable (Mayo-Wilson et al., Reference Mayo-Wilson, Fusco, Li, Hong, Canner, Dickersin and investigators2019; Phillips et al., Reference Phillips, Hazell, Sauzet and Cornelius2019).

Underestimation of adverse effects following antidepressant withdrawal is particularly likely because the most common symptoms include anxiety, fatigue, impaired concentration, and worsened mood, as documented in a study of over 1000 participants (Moncrieff, Read, & Horowitz, Reference Moncrieff, Read and Horowitz2024), which overlap with symptoms of the disorders for which antidepressants are most commonly prescribed. Therefore, withdrawal symptoms can be overlooked or misclassified as symptoms of the underlying condition.

Eleven studies that rated withdrawal symptoms and mental disorder symptoms concurrently were rated as showing a ‘high potential’ for misclassification (See Table 1 and Supplementary Table S1).

In several of these studies, the authors acknowledged the problem by conducting sensitivity analyses of their efficacy measure, excluding data from the first few weeks after randomization (Allgulander, Florea, & Huusom, Reference Allgulander, Florea and Huusom2006; Kornstein et al., Reference Kornstein, Bose, Li, Saikali and Gandhi2006; Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Boyer, Vialet, Hwang and Tourian2013). However, they did not consider how the potential misclassification might have impacted the detection of withdrawal symptoms.

Only 13 of the 62 studies were rated as showing a low potential for misclassification.

In addition to these problems, five studies in the incidence analysis were retrospective case note reviews identifying reports of withdrawal symptoms entered by clinicians during routine clinical care (Table 1). Such studies are likely to miss all but the most distinctive and severe symptoms of withdrawal due to the lack of awareness of the range of effects (Guy et al., Reference Guy, Brown, Lewis and Horowitz2020).

Use of structured instruments

Although 18 of the 62 studies included in Henssler’s incidence analysis used a structured instrument to assess withdrawal symptoms, in 10 of these, Henssler et al.’s analysis was based on adverse events because data from the instrument were not available in the required form (Table 2). In three studies, the instrument was developed for other purposes and did not include common antidepressant withdrawal symptoms. In one, data did not reflect the proportion of people experiencing ‘any’ symptom as per Henssler et al.’s criteria (Table 2) (Yasui-Furukori et al., Reference Yasui-Furukori, Hashimoto, Tsuchimine, Tomita, Sugawara, Ishioka and Nakamura2016). In only four studies were withdrawal symptoms measured using a relevant instrument and reported in such a way as to be eligible for Henssler et al.’s analysis. Two of these used the Discontinuation-Emergent Signs and Symptoms (DESS) (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Soares, Lyster, Cassano, Brandes and Leblanc2004; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Chartier, Hazen, Kroft, Chale, Coté and Walker1996) and two used similar instruments or sets of questions (Mourad, Lejoyeux, & Adès, Reference Mourad, Lejoyeux and Adès1998; Murata et al., Reference Murata, Kobayashi, Imuta, Haraguchi, Ieiri, Nishimura and Mine2010). One further study presented data on ‘specific’ adverse events that were elicited alongside the DESS questionnaire (Tourian et al., Reference Tourian, Padmanabhan, Groark, Brisard and Farrington2009).

Table 2. Studies included in Henssler et al.’s incidence analysis that used a structured instrument

References for articles not cited in-text can be found in the supplementary material.

Abbreviations: AE, adverse event; DESS, Discontinuation-Emergent Signs and Symptoms.

a The figures used by Henssler et al. for these studies were based on any withdrawal-related symptom (criteria for the incidence analysis) measured by a structured instrument or specific questions relevant to antidepressant withdrawal.

Therefore, only 8.1% (5/62) of the studies included in Henssler et al.’s meta-analysis, involving 4.8% (601/12,603) of total participants, presented data derived from a systematic and relevant assessment of withdrawal symptoms (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Flow diagram of studies included in Henssler et al.’s incidence calculation that used a structured instrument or method of assessment of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms.

Discrepancies and uncertainties

Minor discrepancies in data extraction are common in systematic reviews, but some of those in the Henssler et al.’s review are likely to have impacted the results of the analysis, given the size of the studies involved (see Table 1).

For example, participants in a large, pooled analysis of studies of desvenlafaxine by Montgomery et al. (Reference Montgomery, Fava, Padmanabhan, Guico-Pabia and Tourian2009) were double counted, so that 947 participants from these studies were included in the meta-analysis twice.

Figures for several further studies are unreliable due to minimal reporting of adverse events, leading to uncertainty about the total number of people who were followed up after discontinuing their antidepressant (details in Table 1 and Supplementary Table S1). Henssler et al.’s use of the number randomized as the denominator in these cases would tend to reduce the rate of reported withdrawal effects, unless there were no dropouts (which is unlikely).

In two studies, all or some participants only reduced the dose of their antidepressant and did not stop (Bakish et al., Reference Bakish, Bose, Gommoll, Chen, Nunez, Greenberg and Khan2014; Murata et al., Reference Murata, Kobayashi, Imuta, Haraguchi, Ieiri, Nishimura and Mine2010). The reductions made may not have had a large enough impact on receptor occupancy to trigger a withdrawal reaction (Horowitz & Taylor, Reference Horowitz and Taylor2019).

The study by Rapoport et al. should not have been included because the number of participants who experienced a withdrawal symptom is not reported. Henssler et al. used the number of people who withdrew from the trial due to a discontinuation-emergent adverse effect (Rapaport et al., Reference Rapaport, Wolkow, Rubin, Hackett, Pollack and Ota2001).

Observation periods

Observation periods in the studies included in Henssler et al.’s review were generally short – the mode was two weeks. Short follow-up periods are likely to miss some withdrawal effects, which may not necessarily start immediately (Stockmann, Odegbaro, Timimi, & Moncrieff, Reference Stockmann, Odegbaro, Timimi and Moncrieff2018) due to receptor occupancy taking weeks to fall for many drugs (not just fluoxetine)(Sørensen, Ruhé, & Munkholm, Reference Sørensen, Ruhé and Munkholm2022b) or the accumulation of downstream effects that are not well understood (Horowitz & Taylor, Reference Horowitz and Taylor2024).

Duration of treatment

Previous research has shown that the incidence and severity of antidepressant withdrawal effects are greater following long-term use (Horowitz et al., Reference Horowitz, Framer, Hengartner, Sørensen and Taylor2023; NICE, 2022a).

The weighted average duration of exposure to antidepressants in the 58 studies included in the incidence analysis, which reported this data, was less than six months (23.4 weeks). In 30 of these, participants had used antidepressants for less than three months, and only nine involved a majority of participants who had taken antidepressants for a year or more (Table 1). Moreover, in two of these, figures that underestimate withdrawal events were inadvertently used in Henssler et al.’s analysis (Rapaport et al., Reference Rapaport, Wolkow, Rubin, Hackett, Pollack and Ota2001; Tourian, Pitrosky, Padmanabhan, & Rosas, Reference Tourian, Pitrosky, Padmanabhan and Rosas2011).

Placebo withdrawal

Henssler’s final estimates were computed by subtracting the incidence of withdrawal effects reported following the discontinuation of a placebo (nocebo effects), in trials that reported this data, from the incidence rate among people who had withdrawn from an antidepressant, derived from a larger group of studies. Although the occurrence of nocebo effects, or the misclassification of non-specific symptoms as withdrawal-related effects, is an important consideration, the use of different groups of studies to estimate antidepressant and placebo withdrawal contravenes recommendations because of likely differences between the groups (Glenny et al., Reference Glenny, Altman, Song, Sakarovitch, Deeks and D’Amico2005).

Henssler et al.’s strategy also assumes that the adverse effects reported by people withdrawing from a placebo and an antidepressant are the same. However, it is unlikely that these are ‘like for like’.

Antidepressant withdrawal is associated with common, nonspecific symptoms such as dizziness, headache, and anxiety (as well as more specific symptoms, such as electric ‘zaps’). These will occur to some extent as ‘background noise’ in the placebo group, as highlighted by Baldwin, Montgomery, Nil, and Lader (Reference Baldwin, Montgomery, Nil and Lader2007). However, antidepressant withdrawal symptoms are likely to be more severe and occur more frequently. It has been reported that people can become so dizzy they have physical accidents (Moncrieff et al., Reference Moncrieff, Read and Horowitz2024) or be referred for neurological workups (Haddad, Devarajan, & Dursun, Reference Haddad, Devarajan and Dursun2001), for example. Therefore, incidental or background symptoms can only be distinguished from genuine withdrawal symptoms by measuring their severity and frequency, in the same way that symptoms of anxiety and depression are usually rated. Only one study included in Henssler et al. reported the severity of individual symptoms, but there was no placebo group in this study (Khan et al., Reference Khan, Musgnung, Ramey, Messig, Buckley and Ninan2014).

This point is supported by the fact that although withdrawal symptoms in general were only about twice as frequent among people taking an antidepressant compared to those taking a placebo in Henssler et al.’s analysis, the limited indicators of severe withdrawal used (see below) were almost five times more common in antidepressant users.

Severity

Table 3 shows the data used by Henssler et al. to calculate the proportion of people experiencing severe withdrawal effects. This was not presented in the paper, and readers might assume the figures referred to withdrawal symptoms whose severity had been measured using an instrument, or at least to adverse effects that had been judged to be severe. However, in 11 of the 19 studies, the analysis was based on figures for adverse events that led to study discontinuation or on Serious Adverse Events (SAEs). The basis for the selection of these particular studies is unclear since others presented such data.

Table 3. Studies used in Henssler et al.’s analysis of the incidence of severe withdrawal (eFigure 2 in Henssler’s supplementary appendix)

References for articles not cited in-text can be found in the supplementary material.

Abbreviation: SAE, serious adverse event.

a The SAEs in this study are described as occurring either during treatment with the antidepressants or in the 14-day period after discontinuation, so they are not necessarily related to withdrawal.

b Participants did not necessarily stop the drug completely in this study, and there were high rates of use of concomitant medications, including benzodiazepines (see Supplementary Table S1).

c According to Henssler et al. (personal communication), this was based on the authors’ conclusions. In the paper, the authors justify this on the basis that there was no excess risk of early relapse in the placebo group during the relapse prevention trial, and that the mean number of DESS symptoms following discontinuation of agomelatine and switch to placebo at the end of the trial was similar to the mean among those who continued agomelatine.

In any case, neither is a valid indicator of the severity of withdrawal symptoms. Decisions to discontinue from a trial involve many considerations. Researchers usually make concerted efforts to retain participants so as not to lose data and power, thereby making it likely that only unusually severe events culminate in someone leaving a trial. SAEs are a formal category of events with a precise definition, which includes events that lead to death, are life-threatening, lead to hospital admission, cause persistent or significant disability or incapacity, or a congenital abnormality (Health Research Authority, 2024). Therefore, there is a high threshold for categorizing an event as an SAE, and severe symptoms, even if painful, uncomfortable or debilitating, would rarely qualify, especially after short-term exposure.

The few studies that reported authors’ qualitative assessment of the severity of withdrawal symptoms yielded varied results. Some suggested that symptoms were generally mild (Kragh-Sorensen et al., Reference Kragh-Sorensen, Hansen, Larsen, Nasestoft and Hvidberg1974; Rickels, Schweizer, Weiss, & Zavodnick, Reference Rickels, Schweizer, Weiss and Zavodnick1993), and some suggested that they were commonly or not infrequently severe (Dallal & Chouinard, Reference Dallal and Chouinard1998; Kramer, Klein, & Fink, Reference Kramer, Klein and Fink1961; Murata et al., Reference Murata, Kobayashi, Imuta, Haraguchi, Ieiri, Nishimura and Mine2010). The authors of one noted that the symptoms of withdrawal in general were ‘fairly distressing and uncomfortable’ and that people who had severe withdrawal had ‘very distressing symptoms’ (p. 16) (Murata et al., Reference Murata, Kobayashi, Imuta, Haraguchi, Ieiri, Nishimura and Mine2010).

In another small study identified by Henssler et al. but not included in their analysis of severity, 12 of 14 participants who abruptly stopped fluvoxamine after 7–8 months experienced withdrawal symptoms, and of these five had to take time off work, six contacted researchers for help, three sought medical attention, one was re-medicated because of panic, and one became suicidal (Black, Wesner, & Gabel, Reference Black, Wesner and Gabel1993). Incidentally, it is also interesting to note that several studies documented rare cases of hospitalization and other serious events that were considered likely or possible complications of withdrawal (see Supplementary Table S1).

Meta-analysis of studies using a systematic and relevant assessment of withdrawal symptoms

We identified five studies that conducted a systematic assessment of withdrawal symptoms using an appropriate structured instrument or method (Figure 1; Table 2). In all but one of these trials, participants had used antidepressants for 12 weeks or less. In one trial of paroxetine withdrawal, the mean duration of prior use was 106 weeks, but 59% of participants in this trial underwent a very slow withdrawal over a period of up to four years and not all participants discontinued their antidepressant. The majority were also using concomitant benzodiazepines and other drugs prescribed for depression and anxiety (Murata et al., Reference Murata, Kobayashi, Imuta, Haraguchi, Ieiri, Nishimura and Mine2010). All five studies were rated as having a low probability of the misclassification of withdrawal and relapse (Table 1).

The pooled rate of withdrawal symptoms in all five trials was 0.55 (95% confidence interval CI, 0.36–0.72, N = 601) using a random effects model, without subtracting nocebo effects (Figure 2). Heterogeneity was high (I 2 = 77%; τ 2 = 0.59; Q = 17.1, df = 4, p = 0.002). Excluding the trial by Murata, in which not all participants stopped their antidepressant, yielded a pooled estimate of 0.61 (CI 0.38–0.80; N = 545; I 2 = 74.4%; τ 2 = 0.67; Q = 11.7, df = 3, p = 0.009). Removing the trial of agomelatine (Stein, Ahokas, & de Bodinat, Reference Stein, Ahokas and de Bodinat2008), which has a different mechanism of action from other antidepressants and has consistently been found to have a low potential for dependence (Goodwin, Emsley, Rembry, & Rouillon, Reference Goodwin, Emsley, Rembry and Rouillon2009; Montgomery et al., Reference Montgomery, Kennedy, Burrows, Lejoyeux and Hindmarch2004), produced an estimate of 0.69 (0.43–0.87; N = 482; I 2 = 72.7%; τ 2 = 0.63; Q = 7.3, df = 2, p = 0.02) (see Supplementary material Figures S1–S2).

Figure 2. Meta-analysis of studies using a structured instrument or method of assessment of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms: Forest Plot.

These figures are likely to include nocebo withdrawal or incidental symptoms. Although these were not reliably measured in the original review, for illustration, we deducted Henssler et al.’s estimate of placebo withdrawal in trials using a structured instrument (30%) from our estimates. This resulted in a range of 25%–39% of people experiencing withdrawal symptoms.

Discussion

The data that form the basis of Henssler’s review were derived from trials, which were mostly funded by drug companies to assess efficacy, in which withdrawal was assessed cursorily, most often based on spontaneously reported adverse events. The problematic nature of such data is not discussed in Henssler’s paper, even though it is known to be inconsistent and unreliable and is particularly likely to miss emotional symptoms of withdrawal. This, and discrepancies in, and uncertainty of some of the figures, short duration of prior treatment, short observation periods, and other limitations, make most of the data unreliable and inadequate for the task of estimating the incidence of withdrawal. Likewise, the data selected for the analysis of severity were not justified or transparent and do not adequately represent the severity of withdrawal symptoms.

The limitations of the data may explain why there were no associations between the prevalence of withdrawal symptoms and pharmaceutical industry funding or length of prior antidepressant treatment across studies in Henssler et al.’s analyses. Differences between antidepressant agents and the relative lack of data from non-funded studies and studies with participants with longer durations of use may also have contributed to the failure to find differences.

Although nocebo or incidental withdrawal symptoms are relevant, Henssler et al.’s subtraction of the rate of placebo symptoms from antidepressant withdrawal symptoms is not justified. It does not account for the likely differences in the severity of symptoms following antidepressant and placebo withdrawal, and the estimates derive from different groups of studies.

Only five studies included in Henssler et al.’s meta-analysis of incidence had assessed withdrawal symptoms in a systematic and relevant manner. Depending on which studies were included, rates of withdrawal symptoms in these studies ranged between 55% and 69%, which reduced to between 25% and 39% after deducting Henssler et al.’s rate of nocebo withdrawal symptoms. However, since only one of these studies lasted longer than 12 weeks, these figures do not represent the effects of withdrawing from long-term treatment. They suggest withdrawal symptoms are common even after short-term use.

Conducting and obtaining funding for high-quality research on antidepressant withdrawal symptoms is challenging. Ideally, a randomized trial comparing people who are withdrawn from placebo or antidepressants after a clinically relevant duration of treatment is needed. Such a trial would need to employ a systematic and comprehensive measure of withdrawal symptoms, rated for frequency and severity, to have the best chance of distinguishing them from background events and symptoms of the underlying problem.

The results of Henssler et al.’s review have been interpreted as suggesting that antidepressant withdrawal is rare and unproblematic (Pariante, Reference Pariante2024), although we note this was not necessarily the conclusion of its authors. However, as we have shown, the review does not provide good grounds to make reliable judgments about withdrawal. Clinicians and patients need to be aware of its limitations to inform decisions about the use of antidepressants.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725100652.

Data availability statement

All data associated with this manuscript have been published in the paper. Further enquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We kindly thank the authors of Henssler et al. (Reference Henssler, Schmidt, Schmidt, Schwarzer, Bschor and Baethge2024) for providing answers to the queries we made to them.

Author contribution

JM conceived and designed the study and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AS, MAH, HH, JR, and JM evaluated individual studies. MP performed the meta-analysis. All authors helped to design the study and substantially revised the manuscript.

Funding statement

This study received no specific funding.

Competing interests

JM receives royalties for books about psychiatric drugs and was a co-applicant on the REDUCE trial, funded by the National Institute of Health Research, evaluating digital support for patients stopping long-term antidepressant treatment. MAH and JM are both co-applicants on the RELEASE and RELEASE+ trials in Australia, funded by the Medical Research Future Fund and the National Health and Medical Research Council, evaluating hyperbolic tapering of antidepressants against care as usual. MAH reports being a co-founder of and consultant to Outro Health, a digital clinic which provides support for patients in the US to help stop no longer needed antidepressant treatment using gradual, hyperbolic tapering. MAH receives royalties for the Maudsley Deprescribing Guidelines. MPH receives royalties from a book about antidepressants. JD reports being a practicing psychotherapist and secretariat member of the previous All-Party Parliamentary Group for Prescribed Drug Dependence. He has royalties on authored and edited books. AS receives royalties from a book about psychiatric drug withdrawal and honoraria for lectures about psychiatric drug withdrawal. JR is Chair of the International Institute for Psychiatric Drug Withdrawal (unpaid). All other authors report no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standard

No ethical approval was required for this manuscript because no participants were involved.

References

Alabaku, O., Yang, A., Tharmarajah, S., Suda, K., Vigod, S., & Tadrous, M. (2023). Global trends in antidepressant, atypical antipsychotic, and benzodiazepine use: A cross-sectional analysis of 64 countries. PLoS One, 18(4), e0284389. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0284389.Google ScholarPubMed
Allgulander, C., Florea, I., & Huusom, A. K. T. (2006). Prevention of relapse in generalized anxiety disorder by escitalopram treatment. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 9(5), 495505. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145705005973.Google ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, T. M., Li, Y., Coffey, C. W., Sit, L., Shaw, M., Lavene, D., & Basch, E. (2012). Reliability of adverse symptom event reporting by clinicians. Quality of Life Research: an International Journal of Quality of Life Aspects of Treatment, Care and Rehabilitation, 21(7), 11591164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-011-0031-4.Google ScholarPubMed
Bakish, D., Bose, A., Gommoll, C., Chen, C., Nunez, R., Greenberg, W. M., & Khan, A. (2014). Levomilnacipran ER 40 mg and 80 mg in patients with major depressive disorder: A phase III, randomized, double-blind, fixed-dose, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience: JPN, 39(1), 4049. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.130040.Google ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. S., Montgomery, S. A., Nil, R., & Lader, M. (2007). Discontinuation symptoms in depression and anxiety disorders. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology / Official Scientific Journal of the Collegium Internationale Neuropsychopharmacologicum, 10(1), 7384. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145705006358.Google ScholarPubMed
Black, D. W., Wesner, R., & Gabel, J. (1993). The abrupt discontinuation of fluvoxamine in patients with panic disorder. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 54(4), 146149. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8486592Google ScholarPubMed
Brody, D., & Gu, Q. (2020, September 8). Antidepressant use among adults: United States, 2015–2018. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Website. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/databriefs/db377.htmGoogle Scholar
Burn, W., Horowitz, M., Roycroft, G., & Taylor, D. (2020). Stopping antidepressants. Stopping Antidepressants Website. https://www.rcpsych.ac.uk/mental-health/treatments-and-wellbeing/stopping-antidepressantsGoogle Scholar
Chrysant, S. G. (2008). Proactive compared with passive adverse event recognition: Calcium channel blocker-associated edema. Journal of Clinical Hypertension, 10(9), 716722. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-7176.2008.00006.x.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohen, L. S., Soares, C. N., Lyster, A., Cassano, P., Brandes, M., & Leblanc, G. A. (2004). Efficacy and tolerability of premenstrual use of venlafaxine (flexible dose) in the treatment of premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 24(5), 540543. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.jcp.0000138767.53976.10.Google ScholarPubMed
Dallal, A., & Chouinard, G. (1998). Withdrawal and rebound symptoms associated with abrupt discontinuation of venlafaxine. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 18(4), 343344. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004714-199808000-00017.Google ScholarPubMed
Davies, J., & Read, J. (2019a). A systematic review into the incidence, severity and duration of antidepressant withdrawal effects: Are guidelines evidence-based? Addictive Behaviors, 97, 111121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.08.027.Google Scholar
Davies, J., & Read, J. (2019b). Review of authors’ response to a critique by Jauhar and Hayes of “a systematic review into the incidence, severity and duration of antidepressant withdrawal effects: Are guideline evidence-based?”. Addictive Behaviors, 97, 127130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.01.026.Google Scholar
Forster, A. J., Taljaard, M., Bennett, C., & van Walraven, C. (2012). Reliability of the peer-review process for adverse event rating. PLoS One, 7(7), e41239. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041239.Google ScholarPubMed
Glenny, A. M., Altman, D. G., Song, F., Sakarovitch, C., Deeks, J. J., D’Amico, R., … International Stroke Trial Collaborative Group. (2005). Indirect comparisons of competing interventions. Health Technology Assessment, 9(26), 1134, iii–iv. https://doi.org/10.3310/hta9260Google ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, G. M., Emsley, R., Rembry, S., & Rouillon, F. (2009). Agomelatine prevents relapse in patients with major depressive disorder without evidence of a discontinuation syndrome: A 24-week randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 70(8), 11281137. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.08m04548.Google ScholarPubMed
Guy, A., Brown, M., Lewis, S., & Horowitz, M. (2020). The “patient voice”: patients who experience antidepressant withdrawal symptoms are often dismissed, or misdiagnosed with relapse, or a new medical condition. Ther Adv Psychopharmacol, 10, 2045125320967183.Google ScholarPubMed
Haddad, P., Devarajan, S., & Dursun, S. (2001). Antidepressant discontinuation (withdrawal) symptoms presenting as “stroke”. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 15(2), 139141. https://doi.org/10.1177/026988110101500210.Google ScholarPubMed
Hammad, T. A., Pinheiro, S. P., & Neyarapally, G. A. (2011). Secondary use of randomized controlled trials to evaluate drug safety: A review of methodological considerations. Clinical Trials, 8(5), 559570. https://doi.org/10.1177/1740774511419165.Google ScholarPubMed
Health Research Authority. (2024). Safety and progress reports (other research) procedural table. Health Research Authority Website. https://www.hra.nhs.uk/approvals-amendments/managing-your-approval/safety-reporting/safety-and-progress-reports-other-research-procedural-table/Google Scholar
Henssler, J., Schmidt, Y., Schmidt, U., Schwarzer, G., Bschor, T., & Baethge, C. (2024). Incidence of antidepressant discontinuation symptoms: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 11(7), 526535. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(24)00133-0.Google ScholarPubMed
Horowitz, M., & Taylor, D. (2024). The Maudsley deprescribing guidelines in psychiatry: Antidepressants, Benzodiazepines, Gabapentinoids and Z-drugs. https://scholar.google.com/citations?view_op=view_citation&hl=en&citation_for_view=sEcbBuUAAAAJ:abG-DnoFyZgCGoogle Scholar
Horowitz, M. A., Framer, A., Hengartner, M. P., Sørensen, A., & Taylor, D. (2023). Estimating risk of antidepressant withdrawal from a review of published data. CNS Drugs, 37(2), 143157. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40263-022-00960-y.Google ScholarPubMed
Horowitz, M. A., & Taylor, D. (2019). Tapering of SSRI treatment to mitigate withdrawal symptoms. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(6), 538546. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30032-X.Google ScholarPubMed
Iacobucci, G. (2019). NICE updates antidepressant guidelines to reflect severity and length of withdrawal symptoms. BMJ, 367, l6103. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l6103.Google ScholarPubMed
Jauhar, S., & Hayes, J. (2019). The war on antidepressants: What we can, and can’t conclude, from the systematic review of antidepressant withdrawal effects by Davies and Read. Addictive Behaviors, 97, 122125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.01.025.Google ScholarPubMed
Khan, A., Musgnung, J., Ramey, T., Messig, M., Buckley, G., & Ninan, P. T. (2014). Abrupt discontinuation compared with a 1-week taper regimen in depressed outpatients treated for 24 weeks with desvenlafaxine 50 mg/d. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 34(3), 365368. https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0000000000000100.Google ScholarPubMed
Kornstein, S. G., Bose, A., Li, D., Saikali, K. G., & Gandhi, C. (2006). Escitalopram maintenance treatment for prevention of recurrent depression: A randomized, placebo-controlled trial. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67(11), 17671775. https://doi.org/10.4088/jcp.v67n1115.Google ScholarPubMed
Kragh-Sorensen, P., Hansen, C. E., Larsen, N. E., Nasestoft, J., & Hvidberg, E. F. (1974). Long-term treatment of endogenous depression with nortriptyline with control of plasma levels. Psychological Medicine, 4(2), 174180. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291700042008.Google ScholarPubMed
Kramer, J. C., Klein, D. F., & Fink, M. (1961). Withdrawal symptoms following dicontinuation of imipramine therapy. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 118(6), 549550. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.118.6.549.Google ScholarPubMed
Mayo-Wilson, E., Fusco, N., Li, T., Hong, H., Canner, J. K., Dickersin, K., & investigators, M. U. D. S. (2019). Harms are assessed inconsistently and reported inadequately part 2: Nonsystematic adverse events. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 113, 1119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2019.04.020.Google ScholarPubMed
Moncrieff, J., Read, J., & Horowitz, M. A. (2024). The nature and impact of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms and proposal of the discriminatory antidepressant withdrawal symptoms scale (DAWSS). Journal of Affective Disorders Reports, 16, 100765. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadr.2024.100765.Google Scholar
Montgomery, S. A., Kennedy, S. H., Burrows, G. D., Lejoyeux, M., & Hindmarch, I. (2004). Absence of discontinuation symptoms with agomelatine and occurrence of discontinuation symptoms with paroxetine: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled discontinuation study. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 19(5), 271280. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.yic.0000137184.64610.c8.Google ScholarPubMed
Montgomery, S. A., Fava, M., Padmanabhan, S. K., Guico-Pabia, C. J., & Tourian, K. A. (2009). Discontinuation symptoms and taper/poststudy-emergent adverse events with desvenlafaxine treatment for major depressive disorder. Int Clin Psychopharmacol, 24, 296–305.Google ScholarPubMed
Mourad, I., Lejoyeux, M., & Adès, J. (1998). Prospective evaluation of antidepressant discontinuation. L’Encephale, 24(3), 215222. https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9696914/Google ScholarPubMed
Murata, Y., Kobayashi, D., Imuta, N., Haraguchi, K., Ieiri, I., Nishimura, R., & Mine, K. (2010). Effects of the serotonin 1A, 2A, 2C, 3A, and 3B and serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms on the occurrence of paroxetine discontinuation syndrome. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 30(1), 1117. https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0b013e3181c8ae80.Google ScholarPubMed
NHS Digital. (2023). Medicines used in Mental Health England 2022/23. NHS Business Services Authority Website https://nhsbsa-opendata.s3.eu-west-2.amazonaws.com/mumh/mumh_annual_2223_v001.htmlGoogle Scholar
NICE. (2022a). Medicines associated with dependence or withdrawal symptoms: safe prescribing and withdrawal management for adults: Guidance. NICE. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng215/chapter/RecommendationsGoogle Scholar
NICE. (2022b, June). Depression in adults: treatment and management: Guidance. NICE. https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng222Google Scholar
Pariante, C. M. (2024, June 8). The myth that antidepressants are addictive has been debunked – they are a vital tool in psychiatry. The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/article/2024/jun/08/antidepressants-addictive-has-been-debunked-psychiatry-depression-withhdrawal-symptomsGoogle Scholar
Perahia, D. G., Kajdasz, D. K., Desaiah, D., & Haddad, P. M. (2005). Symptoms following abrupt discontinuation of duloxetine treatment in patients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 89(1–3), 207212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2005.09.003.Google ScholarPubMed
Phillips, R., Hazell, L., Sauzet, O., & Cornelius, V. (2019). Analysis and reporting of adverse events in randomised controlled trials: A review. BMJ Open, 9(2), e024537. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-024537.Google ScholarPubMed
Pohlman, K. A., Carroll, L., Tsuyuki, R. T., Hartling, L., & Vohra, S. (2020). Comparison of active versus passive surveillance adverse event reporting in a paediatric ambulatory chiropractic care setting: A cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open Quality, 9(4), e000972. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjoq-2020-000972.Google Scholar
Public Health England. (2019). Dependence and withdrawal associated with some prescribed medicines. An evidence review. https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/prescribed-medicines-review-reportGoogle Scholar
Rapaport, M. H., Wolkow, R., Rubin, A., Hackett, E., Pollack, M., & Ota, K. Y. (2001). Sertraline treatment of panic disorder: Results of a long‐term study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104(4), 289298. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2001.00263.x.Google ScholarPubMed
Rickels, K., Schweizer, E., Weiss, S., & Zavodnick, S. (1993). Maintenance drug treatment for panic disorder. II. Short- and long-term outcome after drug taper. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50(1), 6168. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820130067010.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, J. Z., Boyer, P., Vialet, C., Hwang, E., & Tourian, K. A. (2013). Efficacy and safety of desvenlafaxine 50 mg/d for prevention of relapse in major depressive disorder:A randomized controlled trial. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 74(2), 158166. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.12m07974.Google ScholarPubMed
Sørensen, A., Jørgensen, K. J., & Munkholm, K. (2022a). Description of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms in clinical practice guidelines on depression: A systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 27, 192201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.08.011.Google Scholar
Sørensen, A., Ruhé, H. G., & Munkholm, K. (2022b). The relationship between dose and serotonin transporter occupancy of antidepressants-a systematic review. Molecular Psychiatry, 27(1), 192201. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-021-01285-w.Google Scholar
Stein, D. J., Ahokas, A. A., & de Bodinat, C. (2008). Efficacy of agomelatine in generalized anxiety disorder: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 28(5), 561566. https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0b013e318184ff5b.Google ScholarPubMed
Stein, M. B., Chartier, M. J., Hazen, A. L., Kroft, C. D., Chale, R. A., Coté, D., & Walker, J. R. (1996). Paroxetine in the treatment of generalized social phobia: Open-label treatment and double-blind placebo-controlled discontinuation. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(3), 218222. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004714-199606000-00005.Google ScholarPubMed
Stockmann, T., Odegbaro, D., Timimi, S., & Moncrieff, J. (2018). SSRI and SNRI withdrawal symptoms reported on an internet forum. The International Journal of Risk & Safety in Medicine, 29(3–4), 175180. https://doi.org/10.3233/JRS-180018.Google Scholar
Tourian, K. A., Padmanabhan, S. K., Groark, J., Brisard, C., & Farrington, D. (2009). Desvenlafaxine 50 and 100 mg/d in the treatment of major depressive disorder: An 8-week, phase III, multicenter, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group trial and a post hoc pooled analysis of three studies. Clinical Therapeutics, 31(Pt 1), 14051423. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinthera.2009.07.006.Google Scholar
Tourian, K. A., Pitrosky, B., Padmanabhan, S. K., & Rosas, G. R. (2011). A 10-month, open-label evaluation of desvenlafaxine in outpatients with major depressive disorder. The Primary Care Companion to CNS Disorders, 13(2). https://doi.org/10.4088/PCC.10m00977blu.Google ScholarPubMed
Yasui-Furukori, N., Hashimoto, K., Tsuchimine, S., Tomita, T., Sugawara, N., Ishioka, M., & Nakamura, K. (2016). Characteristics of Escitalopram discontinuation syndrome: A preliminary study. Clinical Neuropharmacology, 39(3), 125127. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNF.0000000000000139.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhang, M.-M., Tan, X., Zheng, Y.-B., Zeng, N., Li, Z., Horowitz, M. A., … Li, S.-X. (2024). Incidence and risk factors of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms: a meta-analysis and systematic review, 1–12. https://scholar.google.com/citations?view_op=view_citation&hl=en&citation_for_view=sEcbBuUAAAAJ:uWQEDVKXjbECGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of studies used in Henssler et al.’s analysis of the incidence of any withdrawal symptom (eFigure 1 in Henssler’s supplementary appendix)

Figure 1

Table 2. Studies included in Henssler et al.’s incidence analysis that used a structured instrument

Figure 2

Figure 1. Flow diagram of studies included in Henssler et al.’s incidence calculation that used a structured instrument or method of assessment of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms.

Figure 3

Table 3. Studies used in Henssler et al.’s analysis of the incidence of severe withdrawal (eFigure 2 in Henssler’s supplementary appendix)

Figure 4

Figure 2. Meta-analysis of studies using a structured instrument or method of assessment of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms: Forest Plot.

Supplementary material: File

Moncrieff et al. supplementary material

Moncrieff et al. supplementary material
Download Moncrieff et al. supplementary material(File)
File 156.5 KB