Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T23:07:20.826Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Grief is a family affair: examining longitudinal associations between prolonged grief in parents and their adult children using four-wave cross-lagged panel models

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 May 2023

L. I. M. Lenferink*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Health, & Technology, Faculty of Behavioural, Management, and Social Sciences, University of Twente, Drienerlolaan 5, 7522 NB Enschede, the Netherlands Department of Clinical Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology, Faculty of Behavioral and Social Sciences, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands Department of Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, Utrecht University, Heidelberglaan 1, 3584 CS Utrecht, the Netherlands
M. O'Connor
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Unit for Bereavement Research, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark The Danish National Center for Grief, Copenhagen, Denmark
*
Corresponding author: L. I. M. Lenferink; Email: l.i.m.lenferink@utwente.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Losing a parent or spouse in adulthood may result in prolonged grief disorder (PGD) symptoms. PGD levels in parents may affect PGD levels in their adult offspring and the other way around. However, research on transmission of PGD in parent–child dyads is lacking. Consequently, we aimed to examine temporal associations between PGD levels in parent and adult children.

Methods

In doing so, we analyzed longitudinal self-report data on PGD levels (using the PG-13) assessed at 2, 11, 18, and 26 months after loss in 257 adult parent–child dyads from Denmark. Cross-lagged panel modeling was used for data-analyses.

Results

Changes in PGD levels in parents significantly predicted PGD levels in adult children, but not vice versa. Small through moderate cross-lagged effects (β = 0.05 through 0.07) were found for PGD levels in parents predicting PGD levels in adult children at a subsequent time-point. These cross-lagged effects were found while taking into account the association between PGD levels in parents and adult children at the same time-point as well as the associations between the same construct over time and relevant covariates.

Conclusions

Pending replication of these findings in clinical samples and younger families, our findings offer tentative support for expanding our focus in research and treatment of PGD from the individual to the family level.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

A prolonged grief disorder (PGD) is characterized by intense and long-lasting grief reactions, such as separation distress, that interfere with daily life of people who lost a significant other at least 6 months earlier (World Health Organization, 2022). A pooled prevalence rate of 9.8% for probable PGD has been found in adults who experienced a non-violent loss (Lundorff, Holmgren, Zachariae, Farver-Vestergaard, & O'Connor, Reference Lundorff, Holmgren, Zachariae, Farver-Vestergaard and O'Connor2017). Higher PGD severity has been found in women, people who are lower educated, more recently bereaved people, and people whose loved one died suddenly and/or violently (Heeke, Kampisiou, Niemeyer, & Knaevelsrud, Reference Heeke, Kampisiou, Niemeyer and Knaevelsrud2019; Lobb et al., Reference Lobb, Kristjanson, Aoun, Monterosso, Halkett and Davies2010). There is also a body of research indicating that losing a member of the nuclear family, such as a spouse or parent, is associated with higher PGD severity than losing a more distantly related family member or friend (Heeke et al., Reference Heeke, Kampisiou, Niemeyer and Knaevelsrud2019; Lobb et al., Reference Lobb, Kristjanson, Aoun, Monterosso, Halkett and Davies2010).

It has been repeatedly shown that children whose mothers or fathers are depressed are likely to experience elevated psychopathological problems (Brennan, Hammen, Katz, & Le Brocque, Reference Brennan, Hammen, Katz and Le Brocque2002; Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Rouse, Connell, Broth, Hall and Heyward2011). Similar findings have been found for the transmission of posttraumatic stress symptomatology in parent–child dyads after trauma exposure (Dyb, Jensen, & Nygaard, Reference Dyb, Jensen and Nygaard2011; Lambert, Holzer, & Hasbun, Reference Lambert, Holzer and Hasbun2014; Schwerdtfeger & Goff, Reference Schwerdtfeger and Goff2007). After a death of a parent, children often do not grieve alone. Based on findings in the field of depression and posttraumatic stress, it is likely that PGD levels in parentally bereaved people are influenced by the PGD levels in their remaining parent.

Theoretical and empirical work offers several hypotheses on how PGD levels may rise in children of people with elevated PGD symptoms. Firstly, transmission of psychopathology in parent–child dyads may occur because of the shared genetics (Rutter, Reference Rutter2007). To illustrate this, heritability of susceptibility for developing posttraumatic stress symptoms, and even exposure to traumatic events, have been found in prior research among trauma-exposed samples (Kremen, Koenen, Afari, & Lyons, Reference Kremen, Koenen, Afari and Lyons2012); this may also apply to PGD transmission within biological parent–child dyads. From a neurobiological perspective, PGD has been conceptualized as a disorder of reward (e.g. craving for the deceased while simultaneously avoiding emotional pain related to the loss), which shows some similarities with other disorders of reward, such as addiction (Kakarala et al., Reference Kakarala, Roberts, Rogers, Coats, Falzarano, Gang and Prigerson2020; O'Connor et al., Reference O'Connor, Wellisch, Stanton, Eisenberger, Irwin and Lieberman2008). Dysregulation in brain reward systems seems to some extent heritable (Dichter, Damiano, & Allen, Reference Dichter, Damiano and Allen2012), which may partly explain why PGD is transmissible from parent to child.

Secondly, environmental factors are likely to impact the interactions between PGD levels in parents and children. Drawn from theories about social learning (Bandura, Reference Bandura1977) and interpersonal theories of depression and posttraumatic stress (Coyne, Reference Coyne1976; Maercker & Horn, Reference Maercker and Horn2013), being exposed to negative cognitions and behaviors from a significant other may elucidate negative cognitions and behaviors through modeling behavior. When applying this to PGD in parent–child dyads, witnessing intense grief reactions in your child after losing your spouse, or witnessing this in your remaining parent after losing one of your parents, may contaminate your own grief reactions. Other environmental factors that may relate to severity of grief reactions in parent–child dyads are family functioning (Gelcer, Reference Gelcer1983) and family dynamics (Bowen, Reference Bowen1978). Family functioning includes among others the level of adaptability to change, level of conflict, and quality of communication within families (Alderfer et al., Reference Alderfer, Fiese, Gold, Cutuli, Holmbeck, Goldbeck and Patterson2008). The death of a nuclear family member likely brings about dynamic changes within the family, which may revive former dysfunctional patterns of family functioning, such as insecure attachment styles (Krause & Haverkamp, Reference Krause and Haverkamp1996). In a similar vein, stemming from Bowen's family systems theory (Bowen, Reference Bowen1978; Ceja & Gasbarrini, Reference Ceja, Gasbarrini, Lebow, Chambers and Breunlin2018), parents may consciously as well as unconsciously project their own emotions, such as intense sadness after loss (e.g. originating from anxious attachment), onto their children. Those belonging to families with healthy family functionality and dynamics may cope more adaptively with stressful life changes (such as a death), conflicts, and negative emotions than families with low family functionality (Ceja & Gasbarrini, Reference Ceja, Gasbarrini, Lebow, Chambers and Breunlin2018; Çankaya & Alan Dikmen, Reference Çankaya and Alan Dikmen2022; Kissane et al., Reference Kissane, Bloch, Onghena, McKenzie, Snyder and Dowe1996; Thomas, Liu, & Umberson, Reference Thomas, Liu and Umberson2017).

While prior work suggests that depression and posttraumatic stress are partly transmissible between parents and children, empirical evidence for intergenerational grief within parent–child dyads after loss is still limited, with one notable exception. A study among bereaved families with a refugee background indicated that elevated PGD levels in parents were positively associated with children's (aged 11–17) behavioral, emotional, and social problems (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Edwards, Creamer, O'Donnell, Forbes, Felmingham and Hadzi-Pavlovic2020). This was, however, a cross-sectional study, whereby PGD levels were assessed in parents, but not in their children. This study therefore precludes to draw conclusions about to what extent grief reactions in parents precede grief reactions in children or vice versa.

Examining the interplay between grief reactions within families is urgently needed to inform theorizing about intergenerational effects of bereavements. Furthermore, these findings can ultimately improve treatment effects for PGD, for example, such that when PGD interacts between parents and their offspring, using a more systematic approach by targeting PGD in the family context in treatment is of utmost important for alleviating PGD. Accordingly, our main aim of the current study was to provide first insights into reciprocal associations between PGD levels in dyads of spousal bereaved people and adult children. Based on prior research on temporal associations between posttraumatic stress reactions in parent–child dyads (Egberts, van de Schoot, Geenen, & Van Loey, Reference Egberts, van de Schoot, Geenen and Van Loey2018; Honda et al., Reference Honda, Fujiwara, Yagi, Homma, Mashiko, Nagao and Kihara2019; Silverstein et al., Reference Silverstein, Herress, Ostrowski-Delahanty, Stavropoulos, Kassam-Adams and Daly2022), we expected that changes in PGD levels in parents preceded changes in PGD levels in adult children (and not vice versa).

Methods

Participants and procedures

Data for the present study were drawn from The Aarhus Bereavement study (TABstudy) (Harris, Brookman, & O'Connor, Reference Harris, Brookman and O'Connor2021; Lundorff, Bonanno, Johannsen, & O'Connor, Reference Lundorff, Bonanno, Johannsen and O'Connor2020; Maccallum, Lundorff, Johannsen, Farver-Vestergaard, & O'Connor, Reference Maccallum, Lundorff, Johannsen, Farver-Vestergaard and O'Connor2021). This on-going study aims to collect data for at least a decade to examine the severity, etiology, course, and treatment of reactions to loss by using self-report survey data on physical and mental health. Participants were identified through the Danish Civil Registration System (DCRS) which provided information on bereaved individuals who lost their spouses. All people who lived in the metropolitan area of Aarhus in Denmark and lost a spouse between January 2017 and March 2018 were sent a condolence letter approximately one month post loss. In this letter, they were informed that they would be invited by telephone for participation in this study 2 months post-loss. Participants were also invited to share information about the research with their adult children and to offer contact information of their children to the researchers, if the children were interested in being a part of the study. Participation consisted of completing surveys online or with paper-and-pencil.

In total, 2006 spousal bereaved people were sent a condolence letter, of which 542 (27%) were undetectable by phone for the following reasons: (1) phone number was unavailable (n = 253), (2) no contact after multiple phone calls (n = 190), and (3) declined participation after receiving the condolence letter (n = 99). Of the 1464 contacted spousal bereaved people, 986 (67%) were willing to participate [for more information see Harris et al. (Reference Harris, Brookman and O'Connor2021); Maccallum et al. (Reference Maccallum, Lundorff, Johannsen, Farver-Vestergaard and O'Connor2021)]. Adult children of these bereaved spouses were also invited to participate. In total, 257 families, including a spousal bereaved person and at least one adult child, participated. In some cases, multiple children from one parent participated (18% of cases in this sample). We only analyzed data from parent–child dyads in this study. Following prior research (Silverstein et al., Reference Silverstein, Herress, Ostrowski-Delahanty, Stavropoulos, Kassam-Adams and Daly2022), we randomly selected which child data we included in our analyses in case multiple adult children participated from one spousal bereaved person.

The first assessment [referred to as wave 1 (W1)] took place 2 months post-loss (data were collected between March 2017 and May 2018). Wave 2 (W2) was conducted 11 months post-loss (data were collected between December 2017 and February 2019). Wave 3 (W3) was completed 18 months post-loss (data were collected between July 2018 and September 2019). Wave 4 (W3) was completed 26 months post-loss (data were collected between March 2019 and May 2020). While in between W1 and W2 another assessment toke place 6 months post-loss, we excluded this wave in this study, in order to make sure that the time-interval between each wave was equal [which is crucial for accuracy in estimating parameters in time-dependent models (Baribeau et al., Reference Baribeau, Vigod, Brittain, Vaillancourt, Szatmari and Pullenayegum2022)]. The overall time-interval between all waves was 8 months.

Measures

PGD levels in parents and adult children at all waves were measured using the Prolonged Grief Disorder-13 (PG-13) (Prigerson et al., Reference Prigerson, Horowitz, Jacobs, Parkes, Aslan, Goodkin and Maciejewski2009). This13-item scale includes two dichotomized items regarding the duration and impairment of grief reactions. The remaining 11 items assesses cognitive, behavioral, and emotional symptoms of precursor criteria for PGDFootnote Footnote 1. An example item is ‘In the past month, how often have you tried to avoid reminders that the person you lost is gone?’ People answered these items on five-point scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (several times a day or overwhelmingly). The 11 items were summed to represent PGD severity, with higher scores reflecting more pervasive PGD severity. A preliminary cut-off score of ⩾35 has been recommended for probable PGD caseness (Pohlkamp, Kreicbergs, Prigerson, & Sveen, Reference Pohlkamp, Kreicbergs, Prigerson and Sveen2018). The PG-13 have shown to yield sound psychometric properties in prior research (Pohlkamp et al., Reference Pohlkamp, Kreicbergs, Prigerson and Sveen2018; Prigerson et al., Reference Prigerson, Horowitz, Jacobs, Parkes, Aslan, Goodkin and Maciejewski2009). Cronbach's α levels were 0.90 for W1, 0.92 for W2, 0.91 for W3, and 0.89 for W4.

Statistical analysis

Cross-lagged panel analyses were performed using Mplus Version 8.4 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén1998), to examine to what extent PGD levels in parents predict PGD levels in children over time (or vice versa) across bereaved families. In doing so, we relied on dyad data on PGD levels collected across four waves. The cross-lagged panel model included cross-lagged paths, in which we regressed the PGD levels in parents assessed at a certain wave on the PGD levels in adult children assessed at a preceding wave, and the other way around; so PGD levels in adult children assessed at a certain wave were regressed on PGD levels in parent assessed at a preceding wave. Autoregressive paths were included, which takes into account the correlation between a construct over time. In our case, this was included for the associations between PGD levels in adult children at W1, W2, W3, and W4 and the associations between PGD levels assessed in parents across these four waves. Lastly, we also took the associations between adult child and parent PGD levels assessed at the same wave into account (e.g. adult child PGD at W1 with parent PGD at W1).

In subsequent steps of the analyses, we constrained parameters, as described below, to be equal in order to select the most parsimonious model (Little, Reference Little2013). In doing so, we compared the fit of the unconstrained model (model 1), with the fit of a model in which we constrained the autoregressive paths to be equal for child as well as parent PGD levels (model 2). In the next step, we further constrained the model by assuming the cross lagged effects to be equal across the waves (model 3). Lastly, we also included constraints on the associations between the constructs at the same wave by assuming these to be equal over time (model 4).

Selection of most parsimonious model was based on the following fit statistics and guidelines: comparative fit index (CFI) and Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) values >0.90 are indicative of acceptable fit and >0.95 excellent fit, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) values <0.10 reflect acceptable fit and <0.06 excellent fit, standardize root mean square residual (SRMR) values <0.08 reflect acceptable fit and <0.06 excellent fit, and for nested models smaller values for Akaike information criterion (AIC) and sample size-adjusted Bayes information criterion (SA-BIC) are preferred (Baribeau et al., Reference Baribeau, Vigod, Brittain, Vaillancourt, Szatmari and Pullenayegum2022). After selecting the most parsimonious model, we also included the following known risk factors for PGD as covariates in our model by regressing PGD at W1 on these covariates: gender of parent, child, and deceased (0 = man, 1 = woman) and educational level of parent and child (0 = other than college/university, 1 = college/university). Lastly, we also tried to include random intercepts into the cross-lagged models (cf. Keijsers, Reference Keijsers2016), but none of the models converged. Our limited sample size likely resulted in convergence issues; we therefore estimated cross-lagged models without random intercepts. Full information maximum likelihood estimation was used to handle missing data on PGD outcomes. Two-tailed tests were run with α = 0.05.

Results

Characteristics of the participants

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the sample. Three out of five adult children and three-quarter of the parents identified themselves as woman. Mean age was 44 in adult children and 71 in parents. About half of the adult children completed college or university and one-third of the parents did so. In three out of five cases, the death was caused by cancer. Regarding probable PGD caseness, 11% of adult children scored above cut-off for PGD at W1, 7% at W2, 5% at W3, and 1% at W4. For parents these rates were 19, 11, 8, and 3%, respectively.

Table 1. Characteristics of the participants

Note. - = The responses were equal for parents and children; n/a, not applicable. The following responses were missing: for education level 12 responses were missing in parents and five in adult children; for income seven were missing in parents and two in adult children; for cause of death nine responses were missing.

Associations between PGD levels in parents and adult children

Table 2 shows means, standard deviations, and score ranges of, and correlations among, PGD levels in adult children and parents at all waves. PGD levels in adult children were significantly, positively, and strongly associated across each wave. Similar associations were found for PGD levels in parents across the waves. Associations between PGD levels in adult children and parents over time were significant, positive, and small through moderate.

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and bivariate associations between PGD in adult children and PGD in parents across the four waves (N = 514)

M, mean; PGD, prolonged grief disorder; s.d., standard deviation; W, wave.

Note. All correlations are significant at p < 0.001.

Online Supplementary Table S1 shows the fit indices of the cross-lagged models. The model including constrained autoregressive and cross-lagged paths showed the best fit as evidenced by an excellent CFI and acceptable TLI value, the lowest RMSEA value and acceptable SRMR value, and the smallest SA-BIC and AIC values. Standardized estimates of the most optimal model are shown in Fig. 1 (for unstandardized estimates see online Supplementary Table S2). PGD levels in parents significantly predicted PGD levels in adult children at a subsequent wave. However, PGD levels in adult children did not predict PGD levels in parents at a subsequent wave. When adding covariates to this model results did not change meaningfully (i.e. same paths were significantFootnote 2).

Figure. 1. Standardized auto-regression and cross-lagged paths between PGD levels over time in parent–child dyads (N = 257).

Note. Associations between PGD in adult children and parents at the same wave are not shown. Dashed lines present non-significant paths. PGD, prolonged grief disorder; W, wave. ***p < 0.001, *p < 0.05.

Discussion

This is the first study examining reciprocal associations between PGD levels in parent–child dyads. In doing so, we analyzed PGD data from 257 adult parent–child dyads who lost their spouse or parent due to (in vast majority of the cases) natural causes (e.g. illness). Parents and their adult children completed PGD measures 2, 11, 18, and 26 months post-loss. The time-intervals cover acute PGD levels (i.e. people bereaved less than 6 months earlier) and long-term PGD severity, which enabled us to examine reciprocal associations between PGD levels in parents and children over the first 2 years of bereavement.

Our main finding was that PGD levels in parents predicted PGD levels in adult children, while taking into account the association between PGD levels in parents and adult children at the same time-point as well as the associations between the same construct over time and relevant covariates. PGD levels in adult children did not predict PGD levels in parents. These findings accord with research in non-bereaved samples indicating that parental depression or PTSD are related to increased psychopathology levels in children (Egberts et al., Reference Egberts, van de Schoot, Geenen and Van Loey2018; Lieb, Isensee, Höfler, Pfister, & Wittchen, Reference Lieb, Isensee, Höfler, Pfister and Wittchen2002; Silverstein et al., Reference Silverstein, Herress, Ostrowski-Delahanty, Stavropoulos, Kassam-Adams and Daly2022). These prior dyad studies predominantly included minor children. While our sample included adults only, we were still able to detect small through moderate cross-lagged effects (Orth et al., Reference Orth, Meier, Bühler, Dapp, Krauss, Messerli and Robins2022). Assuming that these intergenerational effects are partly explained by environmental factors (Bandura, Reference Bandura1977; Bowen, Reference Bowen1978; Coyne, Reference Coyne1976; Maercker & Horn, Reference Maercker and Horn2013), it is plausible that these effects are even more profound in families with minor children [who share (greater parts of) their household with their parents] than in families with adult children.

That we were still able to detect that parental PGD is predictive of PGD levels in adult children might be (partly) explained by the composition of this sample. The majority of this sample were bereaved by illness. Most parents and adult children provided care to the deceased prior to death and, thus, likely faced pre-existing family patterns that were already formed when they were a young family. It is common that more time is spent with family members in the palliative phase of a nuclear family member as well as in the acute grief phase, which may partly explain why in adulthood it is possible to find effects for children mirroring parental grief.

If similar or even greater cross-lagged effects are found in future studies including bereaved parents and minor children who are distressed post-loss, this may suggest that targeting PGD in parents is a valuable addition to PGD treatment for bereaved children. Indeed, treatment for childhood PGD that includes parental involvement has shown to be effective (Boelen, Lenferink, & Spuij, Reference Boelen, Lenferink and Spuij2021; Sandler et al., Reference Sandler, Ma, Tein, Ayers, Wolchik, Kennedy and Millsap2010). More specifically, Sandler et al. (Reference Sandler, Ma, Tein, Ayers, Wolchik, Kennedy and Millsap2010) showed that the group-based Family Bereavement Program, targeted at enhancing effective parenting skills (e.g. anger management techniques and listening skills) and improving coping skills in children (e.g. good communication and problem solving skills) (Ayers et al., Reference Ayers, Wolchik, Sandler, Twohey, Weyer, Padgett-Jones and Kriege2013), alleviated grief in parentally bereaved youth in the short and long run. More recently, individual grief-specific cognitive-behavioral therapy in bereaved minors with PGD, including paralleled counseling sessions for parents/caretakers focused on supporting their child and strengthen the parent–child relationship, yielded large within-group effects. Small to medium between-group effects for PGD were found compared to supportive counselling immediately after treatment and at 3, 6, and 12 months follow-up (Boelen et al., Reference Boelen, Lenferink and Spuij2021).

This seminal work on treatment for childhood PGD with parental involvement suggests that parental involvement seems effective, however, research comparing effects of childhood PGD treatment with and without parental involvement is, to the best of our knowledge, lacking. Furthermore, the potential impact of the extent and type of parental involvement on treatment outcomes for PGD in children still remains to be studied. Future research may also benefit from exploring the efficacy of other family treatment approaches, such as Bowen's family systems approach or a conceptual framework of attachment to explain parent–child interactions (Bowen, Reference Bowen1978; Ceja & Gasbarrini, Reference Ceja, Gasbarrini, Lebow, Chambers and Breunlin2018; Krause & Haverkamp, Reference Krause and Haverkamp1996). For the latter, it should be noted that empirical evidence supporting its effectiveness and working mechanisms is still very much needed (Calatrava, Martins, Schweer-Collins, Duch-Ceballos, & Rodríguez-González, Reference Calatrava, Martins, Schweer-Collins, Duch-Ceballos and Rodríguez-González2022; Suomi, Evans, Rodgers, Taplin, & Cowlishaw, Reference Suomi, Evans, Rodgers, Taplin and Cowlishaw2019).

While our findings offer first tentative support that PGD is transmissible from parent through adult child, we were not able to examine why this might be the case. Insights in malleable factors [such as problematic parent–child communication (Zhen, Yao, & Zhou, Reference Zhen, Yao and Zhou2022), poor conflict management styles (Chiariello & Orvaschel, Reference Chiariello and Orvaschel1995), deficient emotion-regulation strategies (Lougheed, Brinberg, Ram, & Hollenstein, Reference Lougheed, Brinberg, Ram and Hollenstein2020), or maladaptive coping styles (Krause & Haverkamp, Reference Krause and Haverkamp1996)] potentially mediating associations between parent–child transmission of PGD may provide input for further refinement of PGD treatment.

Several study limitations should be noted. First, the majority of our sample did not meet criteria for probable PGD. About one out of 10 adult children and one out of five parents reported elevated PGD levels at W1 and these PGD rates seem to decrease over time. Caution is warranted when generalizing our findings to clinical samples. Second, data collection for this study started before PGD criteria entered the DSM-5-TR and ICD-11 (American Psychiatric Association, 2022; World Health Organization, 2022). The PGD instrument that we used does not capture all of these most recent PGD criteria. Our results therefore may not necessarily generalize to studies using these updated criteria (Lenferink, Boelen, Smid, & Paap, Reference Lenferink, Boelen, Smid and Paap2019). Third, it was not possible to include random intercepts in our statistical models, likely due to our limited sample size. Consequently, we were unable to make a distinction between within-dyad and between-dyad differences (Orth, Clark, Donnellan, & Robins, Reference Orth, Clark, Donnellan and Robins2021). Including random intercepts in future studies likely provide stronger evidence for potential causal linkage between PGD in parent and their offspring (Keijsers, Reference Keijsers2016).

Taken together, this is the first study examining reciprocal associations between PGD levels in adult–child dyads after loss. Within the first 2 years after loss, we found that changes in PGD levels in parents seem to proceed changes in PGD levels in adult children. When replicating our findings in clinical samples as well as younger families, this offers preliminary support for transmission of PGD from parents to children, which points to the need of expanding the focus of PGD research and treatment from the individual level to family level.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001101

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all people who participated in this study.

Author contributions

L. I. M. L. and M. O'C. formulated the research questions. Study design and data-collection were done by M. O'C. L. I. M. L analyzed the data. L. I. M. L and M. O'C. interpreted the findings. L. I. M. L. wrote draft of manuscript. L. I. M. L. and M. O'C. finalized the manuscript.

Financial support

This work was supported by a grant from the Aarhus University Research Foundation (AUFF) awarded to the last author [grant number AUFF-E-2015-FLS-8-63] and a visiting researcher stipend for L. I. M. L., also from AUFF. The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

This study is part of a larger project that was pre-registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (2017; NCT03049007), but this specific study was not pre-registered. All measures used can be found in this pre-registration. Data are not publicly available due to privacy legislation. Syntaxes are available on request. We report how we determined our sample size and all data exclusions. All participants provided written informed consent. The TABstudy adheres to the General Data Protection Regulation of the European Union [2016/679] and complies with the ethical standards of the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. Data were collected and managed using the research electronic data capture (REDCap) tool hosted at Aarhus University, Denmark.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 Data collection for this study started before the introduction of PGD criteria in ICD-11 (World Health Organization, 2022), we were therefore unable to measure PGD criteria as defined in ICD-11.

2 The standardized significant (p < 0.05) cross-lagged effects for PGD levels in parents predicting subsequent PGD levels in adult children varied between 0.05 and 0.06.

References

Alderfer, M. A., Fiese, B. H., Gold, J. I., Cutuli, J. J., Holmbeck, G. N., Goldbeck, L., … Patterson, J. (2008). Evidence-based assessment in pediatric psychology: Family measures. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 33(9), 10461061. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsm083CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, text revision DSM-5–TR. Washington, DC: Amer Psychiatric Pub Inc.Google Scholar
Ayers, T. S., Wolchik, S. A., Sandler, I. N., Twohey, J. L., Weyer, J. L., Padgett-Jones, S., … Kriege, G. (2013). The family bereavement program: Description of a theory-based prevention program for parentally-bereaved children and adolescents. Omega, 68(4), 293314. doi: 10.2190/om.68.4.aCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Hoboken, New Jersey, US: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Baribeau, D. A., Vigod, S., Brittain, H., Vaillancourt, T., Szatmari, P., & Pullenayegum, E. (2022). Application of transactional (cross-lagged panel) models in mental health research: An introduction and review of methodological considerations. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(3), 124134. Retrieved from https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9275371/Google ScholarPubMed
Boelen, P. A., Lenferink, L. I. M., & Spuij, M. (2021). Cognitive behavioral therapy for prolonged grief in children and adolescents: A randomized clinical trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 178(4), 294304. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20050548CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowen, M. (1978). Family therapy in clinical practice. New York: Jason Aronson.Google Scholar
Brennan, P. A., Hammen, C., Katz, A. R., & Le Brocque, R. M. (2002). Maternal depression, paternal psychopathology, and adolescent diagnostic outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70(5), 10751085. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.70.5.1075CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, R. A., Edwards, B., Creamer, M., O'Donnell, M., Forbes, D., Felmingham, K. L., … Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (2020). Prolonged grief in refugees, parenting behaviour and children's mental health. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 863873. 10.1177/0004867420967420.Google ScholarPubMed
Calatrava, M., Martins, M. V., Schweer-Collins, M., Duch-Ceballos, C., & Rodríguez-González, M. (2022). Differentiation of self: A scoping review of Bowen family systems theory's core construct. Clinical Psychology Review, 91, 102101. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Çankaya, S., & Alan Dikmen, H. (2022). The effects of family function, relationship satisfaction, and dyadic adjustment on postpartum depression. Perspectives in Psychiatric Care, 24602470. 10.1111/ppc.13081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ceja, A., & Gasbarrini, M. F. (2018). Multigenerational transmission process in Bowen therapy. In Lebow, J., Chambers, A. & Breunlin, D. C. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of couple and family therapy (pp. 14). Cham: Springer International Publishing. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-15877-8_356-1Google Scholar
Chiariello, M. A., & Orvaschel, H. (1995). Patterns of parent-child communication: Relationship to depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 15(5), 395407. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(95)00022-HCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coyne, J. C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of depression. Psychiatry, 39(1), 2840. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1976.11023874CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dichter, G. S., Damiano, C. A., & Allen, J. A. (2012). Reward circuitry dysfunction in psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders and genetic syndromes: Animal models and clinical findings. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 4(1), 19. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-4-19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dyb, G., Jensen, T. K., & Nygaard, E. (2011). Children's and parents’ posttraumatic stress reactions after the 2004 tsunami. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16(4), 621634. doi: 10.1177/1359104510391048CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egberts, M. R., van de Schoot, R., Geenen, R., & Van Loey, N. E. E. (2018). Mother, father and child traumatic stress reactions after paediatric burn: Within-family co-occurrence and parent-child discrepancies in appraisals of child stress. Burns: Journal of the International Society for Burn Injuries, 44(4), 861869. doi: 10.1016/j.burns.2018.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelcer, E. (1983). Mourning is a family affair. Family Process, 22(4), 501516. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.1983.00501.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, M. R., Hall, C. M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14(1), 127. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, C. B., Brookman, R., & O'Connor, M. (2021). It's not who you lose, it's who you are: Identity and symptom trajectory in prolonged grief. Current Psychology, 111. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02343-wGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heeke, C., Kampisiou, C., Niemeyer, H., & Knaevelsrud, C. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of correlates of prolonged grief disorder in adults exposed to violent loss. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10(1). doi: 10.1080/20008198.2019.1583524Google ScholarPubMed
Honda, Y., Fujiwara, T., Yagi, J., Homma, H., Mashiko, H., Nagao, K., … Kihara, M. (2019). Long-term impact of parental post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms on mental health of their offspring after the great East Japan earthquake. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00496CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kakarala, S. E., Roberts, K. E., Rogers, M., Coats, T., Falzarano, F., Gang, J., … Prigerson, H. G. (2020). The neurobiological reward system in Prolonged Grief Disorder (PGD): A systematic review. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 303, 111135. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2020.111135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keijsers, L. (2016). Parental monitoring and adolescent problem behaviors: How much do we really know? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(3), 271281. doi: 10.1177/0165025415592515CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kissane, D. W., Bloch, S., Onghena, P., McKenzie, D. P., Snyder, R. D., & Dowe, D. L. (1996). The Melbourne family grief study, II: Psychosocial morbidity and grief in bereaved families. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 153(5), 659666. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.5.659Google ScholarPubMed
Krause, A. M., & Haverkamp, B. E. (1996). Attachment in adult child-older parent relationships: Research, theory, and practice. Journal of Counseling & Development, 75(2), 8392. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.1996.tb02318.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kremen, W. S., Koenen, K. C., Afari, N., & Lyons, M. J. (2012). Twin studies of posttraumatic stress disorder: Differentiating vulnerability factors from sequelae. Neuropharmacology, 62(2), 647653. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.03.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambert, J. E., Holzer, J., & Hasbun, A. (2014). Association between parents’ PTSD severity and children's psychological distress: A meta-analysis. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 27(1), 917. doi: 10.1002/jts.21891CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenferink, L. I. M., Boelen, P. A., Smid, G. E., & Paap, M. C. S. (2019). The importance of harmonising diagnostic criteria sets for pathological grief. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 219(3), 14. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2019.240.Google Scholar
Lieb, R., Isensee, B., Höfler, M., Pfister, H., & Wittchen, H.-U. (2002). Parental major depression and the risk of depression and other mental disorders in offspring: A prospective-longitudinal community study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(4), 365374. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.4.365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. (2013). Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York: Guilford Press. Retrieved from https://www.guilford.com/books/Longitudinal-Structural-Equation-Modeling/Todd-Little/9781462510160Google Scholar
Lobb, E. A., Kristjanson, L. J., Aoun, S. M., Monterosso, L., Halkett, G. K. B., & Davies, A. (2010). Predictors of complicated grief: A systematic review of empirical studies. Death Studies, 34(8), 673698. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2010.496686CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lougheed, J. P., Brinberg, M., Ram, N., & Hollenstein, T. (2020). Emotion socialization as a dynamic process across emotion contexts. Developmental Psychology, 56, 553565. doi: 10.1037/dev0000801CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundorff, M., Bonanno, G. A., Johannsen, M., & O'Connor, M. (2020). Are there gender differences in prolonged grief trajectories? A registry-sampled cohort study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 129, 168175. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.06.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lundorff, M., Holmgren, H., Zachariae, R., Farver-Vestergaard, I., & O'Connor, M. (2017). Prevalence of prolonged grief disorder in adult bereavement: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 212, 138149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.01.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maccallum, F., Lundorff, M., Johannsen, M., Farver-Vestergaard, I., & O'Connor, M. (2021). An exploration of gender and prolonged grief symptoms using network analysis. Psychological Medicine, 53(5), 18. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721003391.Google ScholarPubMed
Maercker, A., & Horn, A. B. (2013). A socio-interpersonal perspective on PTSD: The case for environments and interpersonal processes. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 20(6), 465481. doi: 10.1002/cpp.1805CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus user's guide. Los Angeles, CA. https://www.statmodel.com/html_ug.shtml.Google Scholar
O'Connor, M.-F., Wellisch, D. K., Stanton, A. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Irwin, M. R., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain's reward center. NeuroImage, 42(2), 969972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.256CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orth, U., Clark, A., Donnellan, B., & Robins, R. W. (2021). Testing prospective effects in longitudinal research: Comparing seven competing cross-lagged models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(4), 10131034. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000358CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orth, U., Meier, L. L., Bühler, J. L., Dapp, L. C., Krauss, S., Messerli, D., … Robins, R. W. (2022). Effect size guidelines for cross-lagged effects. Psychological Methods, Advance online publication. 10.1037/met0000499.Google ScholarPubMed
Pohlkamp, L., Kreicbergs, U., Prigerson, H. G., & Sveen, J. (2018). Psychometric properties of the prolonged grief disorder-13 (PG-13) in bereaved Swedish parents. Psychiatry Research, 267, 560565. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prigerson, H. G., Horowitz, M. J., Jacobs, S. C., Parkes, C. M., Aslan, M., Goodkin, K., … Maciejewski, P. K. (2009). Prolonged grief disorder: Psychometric validation of criteria proposed for DSM-V and ICD-11. PLoS Medicine, 6(8), 112. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2007). Gene–environment interdependence. Developmental Science, 10(1), 1218. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00557.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandler, I. N., Ma, Y., Tein, J. Y., Ayers, J. Y., Wolchik, S., Kennedy, C., & Millsap, R. (2010). Long-term effects of the family bereavement program on multiple indicators of grief in parentally bereaved children and adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78(2), 131143. doi: 10.1037/a0018393CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwerdtfeger, K. L., & Goff, B. S. N. (2007). Intergenerational transmission of trauma: Exploring mother–infant prenatal attachment. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20(1), 3951. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts.20179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverstein, M. J., Herress, J., Ostrowski-Delahanty, S., Stavropoulos, V., Kassam-Adams, N., & Daly, B. P. (2022). Associations between parent posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) and later child PTSS: Results from an international data archive. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 35(6), 16201630. 10.1002/jts.22864.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suomi, A., Evans, L., Rodgers, B., Taplin, S., & Cowlishaw, S. (2019). Couple and family therapies for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 12(12), CD011257. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011257.pub2Google ScholarPubMed
Thomas, P. A., Liu, H., & Umberson, D. (2017). Family relationships and well-being. Innovation in Aging, 1(3), igx025. doi: 10.1093/geroni/igx025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2022). ICD-11 for mortality and morbidity statistics: 6B42 Prolonged grief disorder. Retrieved from https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en#/http://id.who.int/icd/entity/1183832314Google Scholar
Zhen, B., Yao, B., & Zhou, X. (2022). How does parent–child communication affects posttraumatic stress disorder and growth in adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic? The mediating roles of self-compassion and disclosure. Journal of Affective Disorders, 306, 18. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.03.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Characteristics of the participants

Figure 1

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, and bivariate associations between PGD in adult children and PGD in parents across the four waves (N = 514)

Figure 2

Figure. 1. Standardized auto-regression and cross-lagged paths between PGD levels over time in parent–child dyads (N = 257).Note. Associations between PGD in adult children and parents at the same wave are not shown. Dashed lines present non-significant paths. PGD, prolonged grief disorder; W, wave. ***p < 0.001, *p < 0.05.

Supplementary material: File

Lenferink and O'Connor supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 15.6 KB