Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T12:58:20.771Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Is adolescent internet use a risk factor for the development of depression symptoms or vice-versa?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 February 2023

Caroline Fitzpatrick*
Affiliation:
Department of Preschool and Elementary School Education, University of Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, Canada Department of Childhood Education, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa
Annie Lemieux
Affiliation:
Department of Preschool and Elementary School Education, University of Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, Canada
Jonathan Smith
Affiliation:
Department of Preschool and Elementary School Education, University of Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, Canada
Greg L. West
Affiliation:
Department of Childhood Education, University of Johannesburg, Johannesburg, South Africa
Véronique Bohbot
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, Montreal, Canada
Mark Asbridge
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, McGill University, Montreal, Canada Department of Community Health and Epidemiology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Canada
*
Author for correspondence: Caroline Fitzpatrick, E-mail: Caroline.Fitzpatrick@USherbrooke.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The extent to which digital media use by adolescents contributes to poor mental health, or vice-versa, remains unclear. The purpose of the present study is to clarify the strength and direction of associations between adolescent internet use and the development of depression symptoms using a longitudinal modeling approach. We also examine whether associations differ for boys and girls.

Methods

Data are drawn from (N = 1547) participants followed for the Quebec longitudinal Study of Child Development (QLSCD 1998–2020). Youth self-reported internet use in terms of the average hours of use per week at the ages of 13, 15, and 17. Youth also self-reported depression symptoms at the same ages.

Results

After testing sex-invariance, random intercepts cross-lagged panel models stratified by sex, revealed that internet use by girls was associated with significant within-person (time-varying) change in depression symptoms. Girl's internet use at age 13 was associated with increased depression symptoms at age 15 (ß = 0.12) and internet use at age 15 increased depression at age 17 (ß = 0.10). For boys, internet use was not associated with significant time varying change in depression symptoms.

Conclusions

The present findings support the hypothesis that internet use by adolescents can represent a significant risk factor for the development of depressive symptoms, particularly in girls.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

The World Health Organization projects that major depression will be a leading cause of disease burden by 2030 (Bucksch et al., Reference Bucksch, Sigmundova, Hamrik, Troped, Melkevik, Ahluwalia and Inchley2016). Adolescent's experience of depressive symptoms predicts later risk of developing major depressive disorder and impairment into adulthood (Jones, Reference Jones2013; Naicker, Galambos, Zeng, Senthilselvan, & Colman, Reference Naicker, Galambos, Zeng, Senthilselvan and Colman2013; Pine, Cohen, Cohen, & Brook, Reference Pine, Cohen, Cohen and Brook1999). Furthermore, the experience of poor mental health and depression during adolescence is linked to academic difficulties, interpersonal challenges, and decreased quality of life and earning potential (Naicker et al., Reference Naicker, Galambos, Zeng, Senthilselvan and Colman2013). The experience of depressive symptoms can lead youth to self-medicate, resulting in the adoption of unhealthy habits such as smoking, increased alcohol consumption, and illicit drugs use (Harris & Edlund, Reference Harris and Edlund2005). Over the span of the last decade, depression and suicide rates have climbed significantly among youth, particularly for girls (Hedegaard, Curtin, & Warner, Reference Hedegaard, Curtin and Warner2021; Keyes, Gary, O'Malley, Hamilton, & Schulenberg, Reference Keyes, Gary, O'Malley, Hamilton and Schulenberg2019; Lin et al, Reference Lin, Sidani, Shensa, Radovic, Miller, Heimer and Primack2016). This trend towards worse mental health has raised concerns that the rising popularity of digital media use among youth may be to blame for their worsening mental health (Twenge, Joiner, Rogers, & Martin, Reference Twenge, Joiner, Rogers and Martin2018).

In a recent survey, 45% of youth reported being online ‘almost constantly’ (Pew Research Center, 2018). Research has linked digital media use by adolescents to increased depression symptoms cross-sectionally (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Sidani, Shensa, Radovic, Miller, Heimer and Primack2016; Twenge et al., Reference Twenge, Joiner, Rogers and Martin2018). However, cross-sectional studies are unable to establish a clear direction of effects. In addition, most studies have been unable to rule out the possibility that adolescents with more depression symptoms may choose to spend more time online. For this reason, some have disputed the existence of a causal and clinically meaningful relationship between media use and adolescent depression (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Sidani, Shensa, Radovic, Miller, Heimer and Primack2016). One exception to these methodological limits is Boers, Afzali, Newton, and Conrod (Reference Boers, Afzali, Newton and Conrod2019), who provided longitudinal evidence that digital media use by adolescents is a causal risk factor for the development of depression symptoms in Canadian youth. To inform public health interventions, replications with independent samples are required. Furthermore, Boers et al., did not address the possibility that associations between time spent online and depression symptoms may differ for boys and girls. Previous studies have reported sex-based differences in the effect of digital media use on youth depression symptoms and have found sex differences in the etiology and prevalence rates of mood disorders (Ophir, Lipshits-Braziler, & Rosenberg, Reference Ophir, Lipshits-Braziler and Rosenberg2020). Furthermore, boys and girls are likely to have different experiences online, as girls spend more time engaged in social and interactive forms of internet use, while boys are more likely to engage in gaming and view adult online websites (Ciarrochi et al., Reference Ciarrochi, Parker, Sahdra, Marshall, Jackson, Gloster and Heaven2016). As such, it remains important to address whether the association between internet use and depression is the same in boys and girls.

Our aim is to better understand the direction of associations between adolescent internet use and depressive symptoms in a Canadian population-based sample. Given the prior literature, we hypothesize that higher levels of internet use will be associated with inter-individual increases in symptoms of depression over time. We also examine the extent to which patterns of associations differ for boys and girls and predict that associations will be stronger for girls.

Methods

Sample

The present study draws on data collected between 2011 and 2015 when youth were 13, 15, and 17 years old, in the context of the Quebec longitudinal Study of Child Development (QLSCD 1998–2018). The QLSCD was planned and implemented by the Institut de la Statistique du Québec. The sample originates from a randomly selected, stratified sample of 2837 infants born between 1997 and 1998 in the province of Quebec, Canada. At the onset of the study, 49.1% of participants were girls, 72% were described by their parent or primary caregiver as being Canadian, and 21.7% of parents reported being under the poverty line cut-off for Canadian families. The third phase of this study, upon which our research draws, was conducted to better understand the psychosocial and academic adjustment of youth during high school.

In total, data from 1547 participants (n = 897 girls and n = 740 boys) were analyzed. Participants were retained in the analytic sample if they provided data at one of the data collection waves at ages 13, 15, or 17. In terms of the distribution of missing data for boy and girls, respectively, 11.6 and 9.0% had data available at only one measurement wave, 29.1 and 21.2% had data at two measurement waves, and finally 59.3 and 69.8% had available data at all three waves. Little's test was computed to evaluate if data were missing completely at Random (MCAR). This test was non-significant for both girls and boys (χ2 = 9.987, df = 9, p = 0.352; χ2 = 8.839, df = 9, p = 0.452), indicating that missing data was not systemic. We used full-information maximum likelihood estimation to handle missing data in our analyses.

Procedure

Questionnaires were administered predominantly in French, reflecting the linguistic distribution of the province of Quebec, Canada. This study received approval from the ethics review boards of the Quebec Institute of Statistics. From school entry onward, informed consent was obtained from the child and parents. The main predictors for this study were collected when participants were in their first, third, and fifth year of high school, respectively.

Predictors

Screen time. At ages 13, 15, and 17 youth self-reported how much time they spent per week accessing the internet on a computer to play browser games, do searches, chat or go on Facebook (excluding time spent on the internet at school). Questionnaires are available here (www.jesuisjeserai.stat.gouv.qc.ca/informations_chercheurs/outils_collecte/outils_collecte_an.html). Response options included: (1) None; (2) Less than an hour; (3) 1 to 2 h; (4) 3 to 5 h; (5) 6 to 10 h; (6) 11 to 14 h; (7) 15 to 20 h; or (8) more than 20 h. Scores were converted to measures of hours per week by using the midpoint value for each range except for 20 or more hours which was scored as 20 h.

Outcome measures

Depression age 13. At age 13 youth self-reported depression symptoms over the past two weeks based on their agreement with eight statements such as I hate myself, or I do most things well (inverse item), Cronbach's alpha = 0.79. These items were developed by the Quebec Institute of Statistics for the purpose of the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development. All scores were converted to a mean ranging from 0 to 10, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of depression.

Depression age 15 and 17. In 2013 and 2015, the QLSCD included a special mental health and adjustment component. Youth reported depression symptoms over the past 12 months using an online questionnaire which has been previously validated (Sidani, Shensa, Hoffman, Hanmer, & Primack, Reference Sidani, Shensa, Hoffman, Hanmer and Primack2016). Youth responded to eight items (i.e. I felt I wasn't as good-looking or as smart as other people, Nothing was fun for me, I wasn't interested in anything) scored on a Likert scale from 1 (never true) to 3 (always true), Cronbach's alpha = 0.84. Scores were converted to means ranging from 0 to 10, with 10 indicating the highest levels of symptomology.

Data analysis strategy

Our aim is to examine whether a pattern of temporal associations exists between adolescent internet use and depression symptoms and whether associations are the same for boys and girls. We begin by testing sex-invariance for the variances and covariances of all study variables. More specifically, we assess if the variance/covariance matrix structure is the same for boys and girls. The χ2 improvement test is considered between the model freely estimated across groups and then compared to the fixed model. A significant test would indicate that the variance/covariance matrix differs enough to reject the hypothesis of equal structures among boys and girls.

To verify the longitudinal relationship between average weekly hours spent surfing the internet and depression symptoms, we employ a random-intercept cross-lagged panel model (RI-CLPM). Our model was estimated across the ages of 13, 15 and 17 (13Y, 15Y and 17Y). The RI-CLPM model proposed by Hamaker, Kuiper, and Grasman (Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015) extends the auto-regressive cross-lagged model (CLPM) by splitting variance into between- (stable) and within- (time varying) person components. The stable component is represented by two latent random intercepts loading on internet use and depression symptoms, respectively, across all time-points. The time varying component is captured by a latent factor at each wave which represents changes from one's own mean level of depression symptom as a function of changes in one's own level of internet use. As was the case with CLPM, autoregressive effects of depression and internet use are assessed from the previous assessment point of the same measure and cross-lagged effects across measures are assessed from two consecutive time point. The model was tested via structural equation modeling (SEM) with Mplus 8.6 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2020) using the robust full-information maximum likelihood estimator. Model fit was assessed following the benchmarks proposed by Hu and Bentler (Reference Hu and Bentler1999) for global fit measures (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation, RMSEA; Comparative Fit Index, CFI; Chi-Square test of model fit, χ2). Hu and Bentler suggest a RMSEA < 0.05 and a CFI > 0.95 as a conservative criterion for good model fit. As for the value of χ2, it should ideally be small enough not to reach the significance level (p > 0.05).

An advantage of this modeling approach is that it helps address residual confounding by partitioning the variance in the repeated outcome measure (depression symptoms) into variance of interest (time-invariant, dynamic within person) from the stable, between-person component. Simulation studies have indicated that this approach can reduce the occurrence of spurious associations and bias in directional estimates of association, and more closely approximates causal inference than multivariable regression or standard cross-lagged panel models (Berry & Willoughby, Reference Berry and Willoughby2017; Hamaker et al., Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015). For these reasons we employ this analytical strategy in favor of standard cross-lagged panel models or multiple regression models adjusted for confounders. A similar analytical approach has been used to investigate the association between child digital media use and the emergence of developmental delays (Madigan, Browne, Racine, Mori, & Tough, Reference Madigan, Browne, Racine, Mori and Tough2019).

Results

Descriptive statistics and correlations

Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1 for boys and girls separately. Girls reported significantly higher depression scores at all ages. Girls also reported more internet use than boys at 15Y. However, boys and girls did not differ in the amount of time spent using the internet at the ages of 13Y and 17Y. Finally, as shown in Table 2, mean hours of internet use increased significantly between 13Y and 15Y for both boys and girl and only for boys between 15Y and 17Y. Mean depression scores also increased between 15Y and 17Y for both boys and girls (13Y not the same scale). Bivariate correlations at the between-subject level, stratified by sex, are presented in Table 3. Depression scores and internet use showed developmental continuity for both boys and girls. Furthermore, internet use and depression symptoms were consistently correlated for both boys and girls between the ages of 13 and 17, though this pattern was stronger and more consistent for girls.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variables stratified by sex

Note. Internet use is measured in hours/week. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (2011–2015), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Table 2. Paired samples t tests stratified by sex indicating change in depression symptoms and internet use between the ages of 13 and 17

Note. Depression (13Y–15Y) not applicable. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (2011-2015), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Table 3. Correlations between adolescent internet use and depression symptoms stratified by sex

Note. Internet use is measured in hours/week. *** denotes alpha < 0.0001, ** denotes alpha < 0.01, and * denotes alpha < 0.05. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (2011–2015), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Sex invariance

The χ2 sex-invariance test between the two models was significant: Δχ2 = 415, Δdf = 21, p = 0.0001. This indicates that relaxing the equality constraints of the variance/covariance matrix between boys and girls improved the model significantly, thus providing support for the hypothesis that these matrices are different. In addition, the means of four of the six variables were also different between boys and girls. As such, statistical, empirical, and theoretical rationales provide support for conducting sex-based analyses.

Longitudinal Models Using RI-CLPM

Table 3 presents the results of estimated models for boys and girls separately. Both models generated very good fit indices (RMSEA < 0.05, CFI > 0.95 and nonsignificant χ2) [boys: (RMSEA = 0.000, CFI = 1.000 and χ2(1) = 0.284, p = 0.594; girls: RMSEA = 0.000, CFI = 1.000 and χ2(1) = 0.841; p = 0.359).

As shown in Fig. 1, the model estimating association between internet use and depression in boys revealed a significant covariance of intercepts at the stable (between-person) level b = 0.12, s.e. = 0.05, p = 0.012). This indicates that on average boys who spent more time online also reported more symptoms of depression across all three time points. Within person, the stable, auto-regressive path from 13Y to 15Y were non-significant, indicating that depression symptoms and internet use at 13Y did not predict later depression symptoms and internet use respectively at age 15Y. However, the auto-regressive path coefficients between 15Y and 17Y were significant indicating some stability in depression symptoms and internet use between the ages of 15 and 17, respectively (depression: b = 0.38, s.e. = 0.06, p < 0.0001; internet use: b = 0.19, s.e. = 0.08, p = 0.021).

Fig. 1. Random intercept cross-lagged panel model examining associations between internet use (N) and depressive symptoms (D) adolescent in boys and girls.

For girls a different pattern emerged. The intercepts' covariance at the stable level (between-person) was not significant (b = 0.04, s.e. = 0.09, p = 0.144). This indicates that on average girls who spent more time using the internet across all study waves, did not have higher depression scores. For girls at the within person-level, auto-regressive associations were significant across all time points indicating relative rank order stability over time in internet use and depression at the ages of 13, 15 and 17 (depression 13Y–15Y: b = 0.26, s.e. = 0.12, p = 0.033; depression 15Y–17Y: b = 0.44, s.e. = 0.05, p < 0.0001; internet use 13Y–15Y: b = 0.30, s.e. = 0.08, p < 0.0001; internet use 15Y–17Y: b = 0.25, s.e. = 0.07, p < 0.0001). Finally, the cross-lagged path coefficients indicate that the more hours girls spent using the internet at 13 and 15, the more symptoms of depression they reported at age 15 and 17 (13Y–15Y: b = 0.47, s.e. = 0.19, p = 0.013; 15Y–17Y: b = 0.35, s.e. = 0.15, p = 0.017) at the time varying level. These time-varying associations at the within person-level statistically controlled for stable, between-person differences (Table 4).

Table 4. RI-CLPM longitudinal relationship between major depression symptoms and average weekly hours spent on the internet for boys and girls separately

Note. b, unstandardized regression coefficient; ß, standardized regression coefficient; D, Depression symptoms; N, Internet use (hours/week); RI, Random intercept. Note. Data were compiled from the final master file of the Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (2011–2015), ©Gouvernement du Québec, Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Clinical significance

Effect sizes were small to moderate. More specifically, our analyses suggest that an increase of one standard deviation in girls internet use (approximately 50 min per day), corresponds to a 0.12 and 0.10% standard deviation increases in depression scores two and four years later.

Discussion

In the present study, we examined associations between adolescent internet use and emerging depression symptoms, separately for boys and girls. Our analytic strategy allowed us to specifically estimate the contribution of within-individual changes in internet use on subsequent increases in depressive symptoms while also considering whether depression symptoms contribute to internet use. Our results indicated that internet use was consistently associated with inter-individual increases in depression symptoms for girls in a manner that suggests that internet use may be a significant risk factor for the development of depression symptoms. For boys, associations were observed at the between-group level only. An inverse association, whereby more symptoms of depression contribute to increases in media consumption, was not born out in our data. Effect sizes for the present study were in the low to moderate range and were comparable to those observed in other studies (Boers et al., Reference Boers, Afzali, Newton and Conrod2019; Ciarrochi et al., Reference Ciarrochi, Parker, Sahdra, Marshall, Jackson, Gloster and Heaven2016).

The results of previous longitudinal studies examining associations between adolescent screen media use and internalizing mental health problems have provided a mixed picture. Some have found evidence for bidirectionality between screen use and later depression (Houghton et al., Reference Houghton, Lawrence, Hunter, Rosenberg, Zadow, Wood and Shilton2018). Others have found small, negative associations between screen media use and adolescent wellbeing (Orben & Przybylski, Reference Orben and Przybylski2019). These diverging results could be explained by the heterogeneity in methodological approaches used across studies. For instance, the present study examined depression symptoms as an outcome, whereas others have examined mental wellbeing or life satisfaction (Orben & Przybylski, Reference Orben and Przybylski2019; Orben, Dienlin, & Przybylski, Reference Orben, Dienlin and Przybylski2019). Furthermore, many studies have combined boys and girls in their analyses, which can obscure sex and gender differences in sensitivity to screen use. A recent review finds that associations are strongest between adolescent screen use and depression, as opposed to other internalizing problems or more general measures of wellbeing (Tang, Werner-Seidler, Torok, Mackinnon, & Christensen, Reference Tang, Werner-Seidler, Torok, Mackinnon and Christensen2021). Furthermore, according to the same review, the directionality of associations matters, with studies providing stronger effects sizes between screen use and depression than vise versa. Finally, the use of newer forms of screen media (ex., surfing the internet, smart phone use) by youth appears to be more strongly associated with risk of depression than older forms of screen use (ex., television viewing).

Some studies suggest that internet use may benefit youth mental health under the right circumstances. For instance, youth often report that time spent online leads them to feel less lonely and more confident (Rideout & Robb, Reference Rideout and Robb2018). Other research indicates that time spent online may allow youth to feel more connected to others and to help them maintain their existing friendships (Bessière, Kiesler, Kraut, & Boneva, Reference Bessière, Kiesler, Kraut and Boneva2008; Valkenburg & Peter, Reference Valkenburg and Peter2009). Being able to identify online activities and content that are most damaging, along with those that are beneficial, can be useful for establishing harm-reduction approaches to inform adolescent media use interventions. Furthermore, a better understanding of the circumstances under which individual vulnerabilities and strengths may give rise to positive and negative outcomes remains an important area of investigation.

There are several reasons internet use could lead youth to develop depression symptoms. First, youth who spend much of their time interacting with screen media are likely to experience reduced opportunities for face-to-face interactions with peers. According to a recent study, based on a large representative sample of American youth, there has been a generational decrease in the amount of time adolescents spend engaging in face-face interactions (Twenge, Spitzberg, & Campbell, Reference Twenge, Spitzberg and Campbell2019). In particular, the amount of time youth spent getting together with friends has decreased by an average of 1 h a day in current generations.

We observed negative individual time-varying effects for girls but not boys. This may reflect sex- and gender-based differences in adolescents' on-line experiences. Although boys and girls spend similar amounts of time online, boys and girls differ with respect to their media diets. First, girls are more likely to use the internet to access social media sites (Jurewicz, Reference Jurewicz2015). Social media use has been linked to increased symptoms of depression and anxiety, and decreased self-esteem, emotional wellbeing, and life satisfaction (Boers et al., Reference Boers, Afzali, Newton and Conrod2019; Ophir et al., Reference Ophir, Lipshits-Braziler and Rosenberg2020; Seedat et al., Reference Seedat, Scott, Angermeyer, Berglund, Bromet, Brugha and Kessler2009). Each of these in turn represent important psychological risk factors for the development of mood disorders. This may be the case because social media platforms can promote negative social comparisons with other users that may seem to be happier and more successful (Lup, Trub, & Rosenthal, Reference Lup, Trub and Rosenthal2015).

Girls are also more likely to be exposed to objectifying images and to engage in negative appearance-related social comparisons than boys (Ciarrochi et al., Reference Ciarrochi, Parker, Sahdra, Marshall, Jackson, Gloster and Heaven2016; Myers & Crowther, Reference Myers and Crowther2009). Research has indicated that women report more negative feelings after upward social comparisons than men (Fox & Vendemia, Reference Fox and Vendemia2016). More time spent online may therefore increase girls' exposure to contents that lead to repeated upward social comparisons which can be detrimental to self-esteem and mental health (Seedat et al., Reference Seedat, Scott, Angermeyer, Berglund, Bromet, Brugha and Kessler2009). Previous research has also shown that adolescent girls report lower self-esteem than boys, particularly for appearance-related dimensions of self-esteem (Gentile, Grabe, Dolan-Pascoe, Twenge, & Wells, Reference Gentile, Grabe, Dolan-Pascoe, Twenge and Wells2009). As such, it may be the case that girls are more vulnerable to certain types of internet use, such as social media engagement.

Social media use is also thought to influence reward prediction error. Tonic dopamine release in areas of the basal ganglia increases when unpredicted rewards occur, but not when predicted rewards occur. Importantly, when predicted rewards do not occur, individuals experience a decrease in dopamine release, resulting in symptoms of withdrawal (Schultz, Reference Schultz1998). Social media interactions (e.g. receiving a like on a post or a positive comment; engaging in social comparisons) are likely to contribute to the experience of unexpected rewards or the absence of expected rewards. The latter could then cause psychological withdrawal contributing to depressive symptoms. Withdrawal symptoms, in turn, may lead to reward seeking behavior expressed with increased internet use, thereby completing a negative feedback loop that could lead to further dopamine depletion. Indeed, dopamine dysregulation is associated with symptoms of depression in adolescents (Forbes & Dahl, Reference Forbes and Dahl2012).

A strength of the present study was our ability to establish the direction of effects between internet use and depressive symptoms in adolescents. As such, we can shed light on what comes first, poor mental health or internet use. Furthermore, it was possible to examine developmental associations over four years spanning between the ages of 13 and 17. Finally, we were able to observe associations in a large population-based sample and to describe how patterns of associations may differ for boys and girls.

In terms of limitations, our correlational design prevents us from ruling out the possibility that associations are due to common risk factors such as genetic predisposition or environmental influences. Another limitation is our use of data collected between 2011 and 2015. As such, it is possible that the nature of youth online activities have shifted since, given the rising popularity of apps such as Tik Tok, Instagram, and SnapChat. Similarly, it was not possible to account for the specific types activities youth were engaged with while online. For instance, it remains important to better understand which online activities (i.e. messaging, social media use, gaming) are most closely linked to mental health outcomes in boys and girls.

As a modifiable risk factor, youth media habits may represent an important intervention target. There is evidence that parental monitoring of child media habits including setting limits, and actively discussing contents can lead to improvements in academic performance and interpersonal behavior (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Benkelfat, Dagher, Delaney, Durand, McKenzie and Leyton2009). As a result, health professionals can play an important role by encouraging parents and youth themselves to establish family plans and strategies for supporting healthy screen use habits.

The potential harms and benefits of media use by adolescents remains an important preoccupation. Furthermore, news outlets frequently report seemingly contradictory findings which has led to the public perception that there is scant evidence to support a link between media use and mental health. Our results suggest that internet use by adolescent girls may represent a significant environmental risk factor for the development of depression symptoms.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723000284

Acknowledgments

The Québec Longitudinal Study of Child Development was supported by funding from the ministère de la Santé et des Services sociaux, le ministère de la Famille, le ministère de l’Éducation et de l'Enseignement supérieur, the Lucie and André Chagnon Foundation, the Institut de recherche Robert-Sauvé en santé et en sécurité du travail, the Research Centre of the Sainte-Justine University Hospital, the ministère du Travail, de l'Emploi et de la Solidarité sociale and the Institut de la statistique du Québec.

Financial support

The authors acknowledge funding from the Canada Research Chair's program from the Social Sciences and humanities Research Council. Research was funded by a grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council (CRC-2021-00009)

Conflict of interest

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

Berry, D., & Willoughby, M. T. (2017). On the practical interpretability of cross-lagged panel models: Rethinking a developmental workhorse. Child Development, 88, 11861206. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bessière, K., Kiesler, S., Kraut, R., & Boneva, B. S. (2008). Effects of internet use and social resources on changes in depression. Information, Community & Society, 11, 4770. https://doi.org/10.1080/13691180701858851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boers, E., Afzali, M. H., Newton, N., & Conrod, P. (2019). Association of screen time and depression in adolescence. JAMA Pediatrics, 173, 853859. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.1759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucksch, J., Sigmundova, D., Hamrik, Z., Troped, P. J., Melkevik, O., Ahluwalia, N., … Inchley, J. (2016). International trends in adolescent screen-time behaviors from 2002 to 2010. Journal of Adolescent Health, 58, 417425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.11.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciarrochi, J., Parker, P., Sahdra, B., Marshall, S., Jackson, C., Gloster, A. T., & Heaven, P. (2016). The development of compulsive internet use and mental health: A four-year study of adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 52, 272283. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000070.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, S. M. L., Benkelfat, C., Dagher, A., Delaney, J. S., Durand, F., McKenzie, S. A., … Leyton, M. (2009). Striatal dopamine responses to intranasal cocaine self-administration in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 846850. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.01.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, E. E., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Research review: Altered reward function in adolescent depression: What, when and how? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J., & Vendemia, M. A. (2016). Selective self-presentation and social comparison through photographs on social networking sites. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 19, 593600. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2016.0248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentile, B., Grabe, S., Dolan-Pascoe, B., Twenge, J. M., & Wells, B. E. A. (2009). Gender differences in domain-specific – self esteem: A meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology, 13, 3445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20, 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, K. M., & Edlund, M. J. (2005). Self-medication of mental health problems: New evidence from a national survey. Health Services Research, 40, 117134. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6773.2005.00345.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedegaard, H., Curtin, S. C., & Warner, M. (2021). Suicide mortality in the United States, 1999–2019. NCHS Data Brief, no 398. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics. https://dx.doi.org/10.15620/cdc:101761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houghton, S., Lawrence, D., Hunter, S. C., Rosenberg, M., Zadow, C., Wood, L., & Shilton, T. (2018). Reciprocal relationships between trajectories of depressive symptoms and screen media use during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47, 24532467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, P. B. (2013). Adult mental health disorders and their age at onset. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 202, S5S10. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.119164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jurewicz, I. (2015). Mental health in young adults and adolescents–supporting general physicians to provide holistic care. Clinical Medicine, 15, 151154. https://doi.org/10.7861/clinmedicine.15-2-151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, K. M., Gary, D., O'Malley, P. M., Hamilton, A., & Schulenberg, J. (2019). Recent increases in depressive symptoms among US adolescents: Trends from 1991 to 2018. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 54, 987996. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-019-01697-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, L. Y., Sidani, J. E., Shensa, A., Radovic, A., Miller, E., Heimer, H., … Primack, B. A. (2016). Association between social media use and depression among U.S. young adults. Depression & Anxiety, 33, 323331. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lup, K., Trub, L., & Rosenthal, L. (2015). Instagram# instasad?: Exploring associations among Instagram use, depressive symptoms, negative social comparison, and strangers followed. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18, 247252. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2014.0560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madigan, S., Browne, D., Racine, N., Mori, C., & Tough, S. (2019). Association between screen time and children's performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatrics, 173, 244250. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2020). Mplus (version 8.6). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Myers, T. A., & Crowther, J. H. (2009). Social comparison as a predictor of body dissatisfaction: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 683698. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naicker, K., Galambos, N. L., Zeng, Y., Senthilselvan, A., & Colman, I. (2013). Social, demographic, and health outcomes in the 10 years following adolescent depression. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52, 533538. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.12.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ophir, Y., Lipshits-Braziler, Y., & Rosenberg, H. (2020). New-media screen time is not (necessarily) linked to depression: Comments on Twenge, Joiner, Rogers, and Martin (2018). Clinical Psychological Science, 8, 374378. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702619849412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orben, A., Dienlin, T., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). Social media's enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 1022610228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orben, A., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). The association between adolescent well-being and digital technology use. Nature Human Behaviour, 3, 173182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pine, D. S., Cohen, E., Cohen, P., & Brook, J. (1999). Adolescent depressive symptoms as predictors of adult depression: Moodiness or mood disorder? The American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 133135. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.156.1.133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rideout, V., & Robb, M. B. (2018). Social media, social life: Teens reveal their experiences. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media.Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 127. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1998.80.1.1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seedat, S., Scott, K. M., Angermeyer, M. C., Berglund, P., Bromet, E. J., Brugha, T. S., … Kessler, R. C. (2009). Cross-national associations between gender and mental disorders in the World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 785795. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidani, J. E., Shensa, A., Hoffman, B., Hanmer, J., & Primack, B. A. (2016). The association between social media use and eating concerns among US young adults. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 116, 14651472. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2016.03.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, S., Werner-Seidler, A., Torok, M., Mackinnon, A. J., & Christensen, H. (2021). The relationship between screen time and mental health in young people: A systematic review of longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 86, 102021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, J. M., Joiner, T. E., Rogers, M. L., & Martin, G. N. (2018). Increases in depressive symptoms, suicide-related outcomes, and suicide rates among US adolescents after 2010 and links to increased new media screen time. Clinical Psychological Science, 6, 317. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617723376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twenge, J. M., Spitzberg, B. H., & Campbell, W. K. (2019). Less in-person social interaction with peers among U.S. Adolescents in the 21st century and links to loneliness. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 36, 18921913. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407519836170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2009). Social consequences of the internet for adolescents: A decade of research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 15. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01595.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variables stratified by sex

Figure 1

Table 2. Paired samples t tests stratified by sex indicating change in depression symptoms and internet use between the ages of 13 and 17

Figure 2

Table 3. Correlations between adolescent internet use and depression symptoms stratified by sex

Figure 3

Fig. 1. Random intercept cross-lagged panel model examining associations between internet use (N) and depressive symptoms (D) adolescent in boys and girls.

Figure 4

Table 4. RI-CLPM longitudinal relationship between major depression symptoms and average weekly hours spent on the internet for boys and girls separately

Supplementary material: File

Fitzpatrick et al. supplementary material

Fitzpatrick et al. supplementary material

Download Fitzpatrick et al. supplementary material(File)
File 17 KB