Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-jr42d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-16T14:51:45.246Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Refining our understanding of depressive states and state transitions in response to cognitive behavioural therapy using latent Markov modelling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2020

Ana Catarino*
Affiliation:
Digital Futures Lab, Ieso Digital Health, The Jeffrey's Building, Cowley Road, Cambridge, CB4 0DS, UK
Jonathan M. Fawcett
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science, Memorial University of Newfoundland, St John's, Canada
Michael P. Ewbank
Affiliation:
Digital Futures Lab, Ieso Digital Health, The Jeffrey's Building, Cowley Road, Cambridge, CB4 0DS, UK
Sarah Bateup
Affiliation:
Digital Futures Lab, Ieso Digital Health, The Jeffrey's Building, Cowley Road, Cambridge, CB4 0DS, UK
Ronan Cummins
Affiliation:
Digital Futures Lab, Ieso Digital Health, The Jeffrey's Building, Cowley Road, Cambridge, CB4 0DS, UK
Valentin Tablan
Affiliation:
Digital Futures Lab, Ieso Digital Health, The Jeffrey's Building, Cowley Road, Cambridge, CB4 0DS, UK
Andrew D. Blackwell
Affiliation:
Digital Futures Lab, Ieso Digital Health, The Jeffrey's Building, Cowley Road, Cambridge, CB4 0DS, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Ana Catarino, E-mail: a.catarino@iesohealth.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

It is increasingly recognized that existing diagnostic approaches do not capture the underlying heterogeneity and complexity of psychiatric disorders such as depression. This study uses a data-driven approach to define fluid depressive states and explore how patients transition between these states in response to cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT).

Methods

Item-level Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9) data were collected from 9891 patients with a diagnosis of depression, at each CBT treatment session. Latent Markov modelling was used on these data to define depressive states and explore transition probabilities between states. Clinical outcomes and patient demographics were compared between patients starting at different depressive states.

Results

A model with seven depressive states emerged as the best compromise between optimal fit and interpretability. States loading preferentially on cognitive/affective v. somatic symptoms of depression were identified. Analysis of transition probabilities revealed that patients in cognitive/affective states do not typically transition towards somatic states and vice-versa. Post-hoc analyses also showed that patients who start in a somatic depressive state are less likely to engage with or improve with therapy. These patients are also more likely to be female, suffer from a comorbid long-term physical condition and be taking psychotropic medication.

Conclusions

This study presents a novel approach for depression sub-typing, defining fluid depressive states and exploring transitions between states in response to CBT. Understanding how different symptom profiles respond to therapy will inform the development and delivery of stratified treatment protocols, improving clinical outcomes and cost-effectiveness of psychological therapies for patients with depression.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Decades of research have provided valuable insights into the nature of depression, with promising treatments emerging (Daly et al., Reference Daly, Trivedi, Janik, Li, Zhang, Li and Singh2019; The National Institute for Health & Care Excellence, 2009; Wijesinghe, Reference Wijesinghe2014). However, whilst there is evidence that pharmacological approaches combined with psychotherapy comprise one of the most effective treatments for depressive disorders to date (Khan, Faucett, Lichtenberg, Kirsch, & Brown, Reference Khan, Faucett, Lichtenberg, Kirsch and Brown2012), only half of patients undergoing treatment recover (Holtzheimer & Nemeroff, Reference Holtzheimer III and Nemeroff2006).

Existing diagnostic approaches are undoubtedly valuable in providing a unifying framework for patients with mental disorders and their clinicians. However, while useful for defining the primary presenting problem, current diagnostic systems may not be sufficient to explore the full range of human behaviour and to describe the rich underlying complexity of mental health disorders (Cuthbert & Insel, Reference Cuthbert and Insel2013). For example, by assuming a range of symptoms to be reflective of a singular underlying disease, diagnostic labels – considered in isolation – may mask considerable underlying heterogeneity within a given disorder. This is due to the fact that the classification of symptoms and the origin of diagnoses was built on expert consensus and the agglomeration of different phenotypes under the same diagnosis. Differences in symptom patterns within a condition and some degree of diagnostic overlap are therefore unavoidable. Although this is an issue for all mental health conditions, it is particularly problematic for depression. According to the DSM-5, amongst other diagnostic criteria, a diagnosis of major depressive disorder is suggested when a patient presents with five out of nine symptoms, one of which must be depressed mood or loss of interest or pleasure. This allows for a substantial degree of heterogeneity, as more than 100 combinations of symptom criteria can lead to the same unitary diagnosis of depression (Zimmerman, Ellison, Young, Chelminski, & Dalrymple, Reference Zimmerman, Ellison, Young, Chelminski and Dalrymple2015). This assumption that depression is a homogenous entity may be an important reason behind treatment failures; i.e. the application of a ‘one size fits all’ approach to treatment, without regard for the latent phenotype expressed by a particular person. On this basis, research exploring different depression classes or subtypes within the broader definition of depression is widespread.

Clinicians have used various terms to distinguish between different manifestations of depression, including melancholic, atypical, anxious, psychotic, agitated and retarded depression (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg2011; Insel, Reference Insel2014; Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Cui, Hickie, Roca, Machado-Vieira, Zarate and Merikangas2016; O'Connor & Agius, Reference O'Connor and Agius2015). The terminology varies widely based on various criteria, including symptom features (e.g. melancholic features) but also the time of onset, clinical history and comorbid symptoms of other mental health disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Not surprisingly this leads to a high degree of comorbidity, i.e. a patient can meet clinical criteria for more than one subtype or specifier, and the intensity of single symptoms is not usually considered, making it difficult for clinicians to navigate the wide range of different treatment options available and choose the most appropriate one for each case (Fried & Nesse, Reference Fried and Nesse2015; Goldberg, Reference Goldberg2011; Linden & Rath, Reference Linden and Rath2014; Musil et al., Reference Musil, Seemüller, Meyer, Spellmann, Adli, Bauer and Riedel2018). Despite this, studies have been conducted evaluating the effect of different treatments on each specifier or subtype. Unfortunately, results have been mixed and difficult to interpret (Arnow et al., Reference Arnow, Blasey, Williams, Palmer, Rekshan, Schatzberg and Rush2015; Uher et al., Reference Uher, Dernovsek, Mors, Hauser, Souery, Zobel and Farmer2011). Together with the fact that these definitions have not led to the development of subtype-specific treatment protocols, this throws into question the value of this classification system for determining the most appropriate treatment for a particular patient.

Latent class and transition analyses of depression

In response to this, a number of data-driven approaches (i.e. approaches where theoretical constructs are not enforced upon the statistical model a priori) have arisen recently using techniques such as latent class analysis (LCA) or latent profile analysis (LPA) to identify depression subtypes on the basis of observable responses (e.g. taken from a diagnostic questionnaire, without recourse to subjective judgments) (Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Robertson-Blackmore, Sharkey, Payne, Bergink, Munk-Olsen and Meltzer-Brody2015; Ulbricht, Chrysanthopoulou, Levin, & Lapane, Reference Ulbricht, Chrysanthopoulou, Levin and Lapane2018a; Ulbricht, Rothschild, & Lapane, Reference Ulbricht, Rothschild and Lapane2015). Within health research, these approaches have proven useful in clustering patients across a range of multidimensional symptoms and disorders (for a summary of LCA in health research, see Kongsted and Nielsen, Reference Kongsted and Nielsen2017).

However, LCA has been relatively less consistent in drawing strong qualitative distinctions amongst depression subtypes. Early studies adopting this technique have tended to identify subtypes that differ on the basis of severity rather than qualitative response profiles (for a recent review, see Ulbricht et al., Reference Ulbricht, Chrysanthopoulou, Levin and Lapane2018a). Whereas more recent work has continued to support severity as a major indicator, the distinction between cognitive-affective and psychosomatic symptoms has increasingly been identified as playing an important role (Barton, Reference Barton2017; Carragher, Adamson, Bunting, & McCann, Reference Carragher, Adamson, Bunting and McCann2009; Chen, Eaton, Gallo, & Nestadt, Reference Chen, Eaton, Gallo and Nestadt2000; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Leoutsakos, Lyketsos, Steffens, Breitner and Norton2012; Lee, Stroo, Fuemmeler, Malhotra, & Østbye, Reference Lee, Stroo, Fuemmeler, Malhotra and Østbye2014). Cross-sectional approaches such as factor analysis, which reveal how symptoms cluster together for a given metric such as the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-9), provide further support to the idea that cognitive-affective and somatic symptoms may reflect unique latent variables within depression (Chilcot et al., Reference Chilcot, Rayner, Lee, Price, Goodwin, Monroe and Hotopf2013; Doi, Ito, Takebayashi, Muramatsu, & Horikoshi, Reference Doi, Ito, Takebayashi, Muramatsu and Horikoshi2018; Krause, Reed, & McArdle, Reference Krause, Reed and McArdle2010). This distinction may be particularly relevant when evaluating the effectiveness of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) protocols targeting cognitive v. somatic features of depression.

Despite advancing our knowledge of depression, the use of LCA has been limited to rigid clustering of patients into static classes, providing no indication of how different depression subtypes evolve over time or in response to treatment. A better understanding of how patients in different classes respond to treatment is essential in promoting the delivery of personalized treatment protocols, with the aim of improving clinical outcomes for patients. Latent transition analysis (LTA) is an extension of LCA which uses longitudinal data to explore transitions between classes over time. However, this technique has not been applied broadly within the area of depression research: as with LCA, most LTA studies report a classification of depressive subtypes based on severity, with transition analyses focusing on whether patients transitioned to symptom resolution states or show symptom stability over time (Ni, Tein, Zhang, Yang, & Wu, Reference Ni, Tein, Zhang, Yang and Wu2017; Tay, Jayasuriya, Jayasuriya, & Silove, Reference Tay, Jayasuriya, Jayasuriya and Silove2017; Tisminetzky, Bray, Miozzo, Aupont, & McLaughlin, Reference Tisminetzky, Bray, Miozzo, Aupont and McLaughlin2011). Although some studies also report classification of depression subtypes according to clinical features (e.g. psychomotor disturbances, changes in appetite, insomnia), small sample sizes and the overall confounding effect of severity mean that consensus across studies and patient cohorts remains poor (Ulbricht et al., Reference Ulbricht, Rothschild and Lapane2015; Ulbricht, Dumenci, Rothschild, & Lapane, Reference Ulbricht, Dumenci, Rothschild and Lapane2016, Reference Ulbricht, Dumenci, Rothschild and Lapane2018b). This results in a wide range of findings with limited interpretability and applicability to improving clinical care in the future (see Li et al., Reference Li, Aggen, Shi, Gao, Li, Tao and Kendler2014 for a summary table of 16 studies using latent class analysis to subtype depression).

The aim of this study was to identify depressive states in a large-scale patient population and to explore how different symptom profiles respond to psychotherapy while controlling for overall severity. This study represents the first application of LTA to isolate latent depressive states and characterize transitions amongst them, within a large-scale patient population receiving a course of internet-enabled Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (IECBT). With a strong evidence base, CBT is the most common psychological therapy used to treat depression in the USA and the UK. In IECBT, a patient communicates with a qualified CBT therapist using a real-time text-based message system. IECBT has been shown to be clinically effective for the treatment of depression (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Lewis, Kaur, Wiles, King, Weich and Peters2009) and is currently deployed within the English National Health Service. By understanding how different symptom profiles respond to therapy, it may be possible to develop and deliver personalized treatment protocols with the aim of improving treatment outcomes for patients with depression.

Methods

Data were obtained from patients receiving IECBT, delivered using a commercial package provided by Ieso Digital Health (https://www.iesohealth.com/), following internationally recognized standards for information security (ISO 27001; https://www.iesohealth.com/en-gb/legal/iso-certificates). Patients self-referred or were referred by a primary healthcare worker directly to the service. Upon registration, patients were assigned to a qualified CBT therapist accredited by the British Association for Behavioural & Cognitive Psychotherapies (BABCP). Initial assessments and NICE approved disorder-specific CBT treatment protocols (The National Institute for Health & Care Excellence, 2009), based on Roth and Pilling's CBT competences framework (Roth & Pilling, Reference Roth and Pilling2008), were delivered during scheduled sessions in an online therapy room, via one-to-one real-time written conversation.

The Improving Access to Psychological Therapies (IAPT) program, under which Ieso Digital Health operates, is a large-scale national initiative aimed at increasing access to evidence-based psychological therapy for common mental health disorders within the English NHS (Clark, Reference Clark2011). The information captured through IAPT's minimum dataset, including IECBT, is intended to support monitoring of implementation and effectiveness of national policy/legislation, policy development, performance analysis and benchmarking, national analysis and statistics and national audit of IAPT services. At registration, patients agree to the services' terms and conditions, including the use of anonymized data for audit purposes and to support research, including academic publications or conference presentations.

Clinical outcomes

Clinical outcomes were measured in terms of IAPT-engagement, reliable improvement, per cent improvement and deterioration, and were included as binary measures (i.e. 0 or 1). Following IAPT guidelines a patient was classed as engaged if they attended two or more treatment sessions. This is the minimum dose of therapy a patient must receive such that pre- and post-treatment scores are collected and clinical change can be estimated (Gyani, Shafran, Layard, & Clark, Reference Gyani, Shafran, Layard and Clark2013). Clinically reliable improvement, per cent improvement and deterioration are calculated based on two severity measures completed by the patient at initial assessment and before every therapy session: PHQ-9 (Kroenke, Spitzer, & Williams, Reference Kroenke, Spitzer and Williams2001) and GAD-7 (Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams, & Löwe, Reference Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams and Löwe2006), corresponding to depressive and anxiety symptoms, respectively.

Patients with two or more therapy sessions who show a significant reduction in at least one of the outcome measures from assessment to the last treatment session (i.e. decrease of six points or more in the PHQ-9 and/or four points or more in the GAD-7), while not showing a significant increase in the other outcome measure (i.e. an increase of six points or more in the PHQ-9 or four points or more in the GAD-7), were classed as showing reliable improvement. Patients showing a significant increase in at least one of the outcome measures from assessment to last treatment session (i.e. increase of six points or more in the PHQ-9 or four points or more in the GAD-7), were classed as showing deterioration.

Similar to IAPT convention, we classed a patient as achieving per cent improvement if they showed a 25% decrease in scores in one or both scales, without showing symptom worsening in either scale (i.e. 25% increase in scores in either scale). IAPT's improvement metric is, by definition, biased by initial symptom severity, i.e. more severe patients are more likely to improve due to their initial higher scores. Per cent improvement has the advantage of reducing this bias while retaining similar properties to the IAPT-improvement metric, in the sense that it is a binary measure that reflects the change in scores from start to end of treatment (Hiller, Schindler, & Lambert, Reference Hiller, Schindler and Lambert2012). While per cent improvement may reduce bias for more symptomatic patients, it naturally introduces a small bias for less symptomatic patients. Nevertheless, considered alongside each other, these two metrics provide a more accurate representation of patients' response to treatment, relative to either metric considered in isolation.

Sample size

More than 48 000 patients were discharged from the IECBT service between June 2012 and January 2019. Of these, 10 795 received a diagnosis of depression, recurrent depression disorder or dysthymia from a qualified clinician, and met inclusion criteria for the service (over 18 years old, registered with a general practitioner in the geographical region where the service is commissioned, not at significant risk of self-harm and no presence of an axis II disorder). A total of 9891 patients with at least one PHQ-9 score, collected at initial assessment, but no more than 10 scores were included in the analysis. The latter criterion was instated to keep the computational demands of our modelling approach manageable; importantly, few patients had more than 10 scores (8%). Of the patients included in the analysis, 6958 (70%) attended two or more therapy sessions (IAPT engaged) and were therefore included in analyses on clinical improvement outcomes. Patients with only one PHQ-9 score were included to inform the model at timepoint 1 (i.e. when patients present to the service), which in turn informs our understanding of the model and transitions for subsequent timepoints.

Modelling depressive states

Even though PHQ-9 and GAD-7 are used in combination to assess clinical improvement within the IAPT program, variations in clinical presentation for depressed patients are more likely to be captured by the PHQ-9 questionnaire (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Patient health questionnaire (PHQ-9).

Item-level PHQ-9 scores, collected at registration and before each therapy session for all patients, were used as input to a Hidden Markov Model (HMM) implemented using the LMest (Bartolucci, Pandolfi, & Pennoni, Reference Bartolucci, Pandolfi and Pennoni2017) package in R v.3.5.0 (R Core Team, 2018) to estimate latent states and transition probabilities between states. In the reported models, we assumed heterogeneity of transition probabilities across time – allowing for the possibility that specific state-to-state transitions become more likely (or unlikely) throughout the treatment process. Models were fitted assuming a number of states ranging from 1 through 16, with the final number of states selected based on the corresponding Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC). This approach revealed empirical support for 14 states. However, an inspection of the resulting profiles revealed several states as representing minor empirical differences (e.g. gradations of severity) rather than meaningful, qualitative distinctions. We believe these gradations in severity are of less interest, and for that reason, we chose to interpret a model with seven states as a compromise between optimal model fit and interpretability (Fig. 2). We have run the 7-state model on three independent folds of our data, each replicating similar states and transition probabilities (see online Supplementary Figs S4 and S5). This supports the hypothesis that the 7-state latent structure presented is not spurious. For transparency, the full 14-states model and transition probabilities supported by the data are also detailed in Supplementary Materials (see online Supplementary Figs S1, S2 and S3 and Supplementary Table S2). Following the model fit, the depressive state at each time point was estimated for all patients using global decoding. Decoded data were used to explore state transitions over time and in post-hoc analyses to evaluate differences in patient demographics and clinical outcomes, based on starting state.

Fig. 2. Graphical summary of state symptom profiles for the 7-state model. States 1 and 2 represent states of minimal to mild overall severity; State 3 shows peak symptom intensity around feelings of depression, tiredness and low self-esteem (cognitive/affective state); State 5 shows peak symptom intensity around difficulties sleeping, feelings of tiredness, and changes in appetite (somatic state); State 4 shows a relatively even spread in symptom intensity across items (hybrid state); States 6 and 7 represent moderately severe and severe states, respectively.

Statistical analyses

The PHQ-9 has been demonstrated to be comprised of two factors, one loading on somatic symptoms (e.g. difficulties sleeping, tiredness, changes in appetite), and one loading on cognitive/affective symptoms (e.g. feeling down and depressed, low self-esteem) (Chilcot et al., Reference Chilcot, Rayner, Lee, Price, Goodwin, Monroe and Hotopf2013; Doi et al., Reference Doi, Ito, Takebayashi, Muramatsu and Horikoshi2018; Krause et al., Reference Krause, Reed and McArdle2010). Post-hoc analyses investigating differences in clinical outcomes and demographics, therefore, focused on depressive states loading more markedly on the cognitive/affective factor (State 3) and somatic factor (State 5). We first performed statistical analyses to investigate differences in outcomes and demographics between patients presenting to treatment in State 3 and State 5.

Pearson's χ2 tests were performed to compare rates of IAPT-engagement for patients entering treatment at State 3 or State 5 (n = 2959 patients: State 3: 1685; State 5: 1274), and to compare rates of reliable improvement, deterioration and per cent improvement for engaged patients (n = 2092: State 3: 1234; State 5: 858).

Logistic regression was then performed to investigate which patient demographics were predictive of state. Starting state was included as a binary outcome measure (State 3 = 1, State 5 = 0). Predictor variables were patient age, gender, sexual orientation, ethnicity, religion, whether the patient was in the perinatal period, whether the patient suffered from a long-term physical condition, whether the patient was taking psychotropic medication at the start of treatment, whether the patient was in active military service and whether the patient had a disability. PHQ-9 and GAD-7 scores at assessment were also included as continuous variables to account for any baseline differences in symptom severity. Continuous predictor variables were scaled and centred to the mean. Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05 two-tailed, uncorrected. Multicollinearity analyses revealed that variance inflation factors were smaller than two for all predictor variables, confirming that the regression model was not affected by the presence of multicollinearity. All analyses were performed in R.

Results

Our chosen HMM revealed seven depressive states with varied symptom profiles and differing overall severity levels (Fig. 2) (see Methods). States 1 and 2 represent states of minimal to mild overall severity, with low intensity across all items. States 3, 4 and 5 represent states of moderate severity, but differing symptom profiles. While State 4 shows a relatively even spread in symptom intensity across items, State 3 shows peak intensity for items centred around feelings of depression, tiredness and low self-esteem (cognitive/affective state). State 5 shows peak intensity for items centred around difficulties sleeping, feelings of tiredness and changes in appetite (somatic state). States 6 and 7 represent moderately severe and severe states respectively, both showing evenly high intensity across items.

The model used in the current study assumes heterogeneity of transition probabilities across time. To illustrate state progression over time, we estimated the probable state of each patient at each time point, plotted as a function of their initial state (Fig. 3a). A transition probability graph, showing a range of transition probabilities across time, is also shown in Fig. 3b. The full 3-dimensional transition probability matrix (‘starting state’ by ‘end state’ by ‘time’) can be found in Supplementary Materials (online Supplementary Table S3). Overall, most patients tended to either remain in their initial state, or transition to a state of lower overall severity, as would be expected in response to a therapeutic intervention. It is interesting to note however that these transitions are not homogeneous across starting states. For example, despite its lower overall symptom severity, State 4 is bypassed by patients starting in cognitive/affective State 3 and somatic State 5, as they progress to recovery. On the other hand, patients starting in more severe States 6 and 7 do seem to transition to State 4 as they progress through therapy but are less likely to transition to somatic State 5, and almost never transition to cognitive/affective State 3. A similar pattern is observed for patients experiencing worsening of their symptoms, where a small proportion of patients starting in State 4 deteriorate towards State 6, but not to somatic State 5 or cognitive/affective State 3. It is also interesting to observe differences across starting states in terms of patients' likelihood to remain in their starting state. For example, while only around a quarter of patients starting at States 3, 4 and 6 remain at their initial state after a course of therapy, approximately half of patients starting in States 5 and 7 do so.

Fig. 3. (a) Stacked area plots showing transitions between states over time for each starting state; patients leaving treatment were considered to remain at whatever state they last exhibited. (b) Transition probability graph showing the range of transition probabilities across time for each depressive state; transition probabilities below 0.05 for more than half of the time points are omitted; thicker arrows represent the most likely transitions between two given states. A full transition probability matrix is available in online Supplementary Materials.

Cognitive/affective and somatic depression: association with outcomes and demographics

A chi-square test comparing the rate of IAPT-engagement in cognitive/affective (State 3) and somatic (State 5) states revealed significant differences, with higher engagement rates found for patients entering treatment in a cognitive/affective state [χ2(12 959) = 11.86, p < 0.001]. Additional chi-square tests revealed that both the rate of reliable improvement and per cent improvement were also significantly higher for patients starting treatment in cognitive/affective relative to somatic state [Improvement: χ2(12 092) = 11.64, p < 0.001; Per cent improvement: χ2(12 092) = 4.17, p = 0.041; Table 1]. No significant difference in deterioration rates was observed between patients starting treatment in cognitive/affective and somatic states (χ2(12 092) = 1.65, n.s.).

Table 1. Engagement and clinical outcomes for each starting state

A logistic regression investigating the relationship between starting state and patient demographics revealed a significant relationship between the state and patient gender, the presence of a long-term medical condition and medication status (Table 2). Patients entering treatment in a somatic state were significantly more likely to present with lower overall symptom severity (PHQ-9 and GAD-7) and more likely to be female, be prescribed and taking medication, and have a long-term medical condition.

Table 2. Results of logistic regression analysis investigating the relationship between patient demographics and starting state [cognitive/affective (State 3) or somatic (State 5)]

A positive relationship indicates that a variable is significantly more likely to occur in patients starting in the cognitive/affective state (State 3). Gender ‘Female’, sexual orientation ‘Heterosexual’, long term condition ‘No’, psychotropic medication ‘Not Prescribed’, ethnicity ‘Non-White’, religion ‘Christian’, military ‘No’, perinatal ‘No’ and disabled ‘No’ were reference classes for the categorical variables ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first paper to isolate depression states and characterize transitions between those states using LTA in a large sample of patients receiving CBT. This study represents a novel advancement, by providing an investigation of different depression states in a real-world clinical setting, and their response to a course of CBT.

Main findings

HMM analysis of item-level PHQ-9 data revealed seven depressive states varying in symptom profile and overall severity. While States 1, 2 and 7 represent minimal, mild and severe states respectively, with symptom severity either at the floor or at the ceiling, moderate to moderate-severe states (State 3 to 6) demonstrate interesting variations in item-level severity (Fig. 2). Cross-sectional approaches such as factor analyses explore how certain symptoms cluster together for a given metric. Although these types of analyses provide no information on how different symptom clusters are associated with or interact with each other, they provide an interesting context on which to interpret the current findings. The depressive states described here can be considered in the context of research demonstrating a two-factor structure for the PHQ-9 scale, separating somatic and cognitive/affective symptoms (Chilcot et al., Reference Chilcot, Rayner, Lee, Price, Goodwin, Monroe and Hotopf2013; Doi et al., Reference Doi, Ito, Takebayashi, Muramatsu and Horikoshi2018; Krause et al., Reference Krause, Reed and McArdle2010). For example, the most severe symptoms for State 3 appear to load on the cognitive/affective factor of the PHQ-9, while the most severe symptoms for State 5 load on the somatic factor of the scale. In States 4 and 6, patients' symptoms seem to load equally on both cognitive/affective and somatic factors (hybrid states). This distinction between cognitive/affective and somatic depressive symptoms is further supported by previous research exploring depression subtyping using latent classification analysis (Carragher et al., Reference Carragher, Adamson, Bunting and McCann2009). Interestingly, to our knowledge, our study is the first to demonstrate a similar latent structure using LTA, and therefore the first to characterize transitions between these subtypes.

This work also shows how patients starting in each of these states transition between states over a course of therapy. It is interesting to note that state transitions seem to stabilize at around treatment session 6. This is likely a reflection of the mean treatment duration across all starting states (i.e. approximately five treatment sessions), but also that in CBT the greatest clinical benefit is likely to be achieved in the first half of treatment (Ilardi & Craighead, Reference Ilardi and Craighead1994; Tang & DeRubeis, Reference Tang and DeRubeis2006). It is also interesting to observe that patients in the cognitive/affective and somatic states (State 3 and 5 respectively) do not transition to hybrid State 4, despite this being lower in overall severity (Fig. 3a and 3b). Similarly, about a quarter of patients starting in hybrid State 6 transition to hybrid State 4, with a small probability of transition to less severe States 3 or 5. A similar pattern is observed for symptom deterioration, where patients starting in hybrid State 4 deteriorate with low probability to hybrid State 6, but not to cognitive/affective and somatic States 3 and 5. Patients in cognitive/affective State 3 and somatic State 5 also do not seem to deteriorate to hybrid States 6 or 7. Differential loading on the cognitive/affective v. somatic factors of the PHQ-9 metric, together with differences in transition probabilities across states, provide initial evidence for the existence of different depression subtypes.

We further explored this hypothesis by evaluating differences in clinical outcomes and patient demographics across cognitive/affective and somatic states. Despite similar overall severity, patients starting treatment in cognitive/affective and somatic states (States 3 and 5) show significant differences in outcomes, with somatic patients less likely to engage with treatment, improve or show per cent improvement. Related to this, we note that patients starting in hybrid State 6 show a small probability of transitioning to somatic State 5, but not to cognitive/affective State 3. Together, this may suggest that IECBT (or CBT in general) may be more effective at targeting cognitive/affective symptoms, with somatic symptoms appearing to be more resistant to treatment – the literature suggests that treatments targeting maladaptive cognitions are sufficient to improve symptoms for some patients, whereas for others this approach is significantly less effective (Hayes, Reference Hayes2016; Kazdin, Reference Kazdin2007; Lorenzo-Luaces, German, & DeRubeis, Reference Lorenzo-Luaces, German and DeRubeis2015).

A regression analysis on patient demographics also revealed that patients who start in a somatic state are more likely to be female, suffer from long-term physical comorbidity, and be taking psychotropic medication (Table 2). These findings suggest demographic and clinical differences between the two states that go beyond mental health presentation, although the nature of the causal relationship between demographic variables and depressive state remains unclear. For example, it can be hypothesized that female patients with a long-term physical condition share a physiological substrate that makes them more likely to develop depression with somatic features. On the other hand, the prevalence of somatic symptoms in patients with long-term physical comorbidity is also expected to some degree, as some of these comorbidities can be associated with physical symptoms, such as persistent lack of energy and tiredness. Equally, patients who are prescribed antidepressants and anxiolytics are more likely to suffer from somatic symptoms (which can include fatigue, insomnia and changes in appetite) as a consequence of medication side-effects. It can be noted that the regression analysis also revealed that patients who start in a cognitive/affective state show higher overall symptom severity for both the PHQ-9 and GAD-7 scales. However, this association is unlikely to be clinically meaningful (as supported by a difference of less than 1 in the group average for both scales), with its significance being inflated by the large size of the sample.

Overall, this study provides important preliminary evidence for the existence of different depression subtypes, characterized by depressive states with different symptom profiles and different transition probabilities between states. Differences in clinical outcomes and demographics between patients in cognitive/affective v. somatic depressive states further support this hypothesis.

Data-driven approaches such as the one used in this paper do not fully address the weaknesses of existing classification systems, such as symptom and diagnostic overlap. Indeed, the interpretation of the results of these data-driven approaches is still informed by existing theories on diagnostic classification. Nevertheless, we believe the current model presents remarkable clinical potential – possibly implemented as part of a digital triage tool – which would allow clinicians to identify patients in depressive states which are typically less responsive to therapy. This would then enable the development and deployment of pharmacological and psychotherapeutic interventions in a stratified manner, aimed at increasing engagement and addressing core symptoms of a patient's condition, potentially improving their likelihood of responding to treatment.

The development of stratified treatment interventions also merits further research investigating the effect of therapeutic features (e.g. therapist effects, therapeutic content) on transition probabilities between depressive states. Better targeted interventions would have the dual advantage of improving clinical outcomes, as well as improving the cost-effectiveness of psychological therapies. Finally, future research should also investigate the generalizability of these models to clinical populations receiving other types of therapy (e.g. face-to-face CBT, psychodynamic therapy), or patients presenting with depressive symptoms as a secondary problem (e.g. primary presenting mental health condition with comorbid depressive features).

In this light, the present study not only deepens our knowledge of depression as a mental health disorder but by exploring the dynamic response to therapy in different depression subtypes also raises interesting possibilities for future research and the development of stratified treatment interventions aimed at improving clinical outcomes in patients with depression.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002032.

Conflict of interest

This manuscript has not been published in full and is not under consideration for publication elsewhere. All authors have approved the final version to be submitted and contributed significantly to the analysis and interpretation of data as well as drafting and revising the article. The current study was privately sponsored by Ieso Digital Health. Dr Ana Catarino, Dr Michael P Ewbank, Sarah Bateup, Dr Ronan Cummins, Dr Valentin Tablan and Dr Andrew D Blackwell are employees of the sponsor. Dr Jonathan M Fawcett was paid as a consultant by the sponsor for his contribution to this work.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Journal of Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053Google Scholar
Arnow, B. A., Blasey, C., Williams, L. M., Palmer, D. M., Rekshan, W., Schatzberg, A. F., … Rush, A. J. (2015). Depression subtypes in predicting antidepressant response: A report from the iSPOT-D trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 172(8), 743750. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14020181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartolucci, F., Pandolfi, S., & Pennoni, F. (2017). LMest: An R package for latent Markov models for longitudinal categorical data. Journal of Statistical Software, 81(4). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v081.i04CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton, Y. A. (2017). Deconstructing depression: A latent class analysis of potential depressive subtypes in emerging adults. Spirituality in Clinical Practice, 4(1), 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carragher, N., Adamson, G., Bunting, B., & McCann, S. (2009). Subtypes of depression in a nationally representative sample. Journal of Affective Disorders, 113(1-2), 8899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2008.05.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, L. S., Eaton, W. W., Gallo, J. J., & Nestadt, G. (2000). Understanding the heterogeneity of depression through the triad of symptoms, course and risk factors: A longitudinal, population-based study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 59(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(99)00132-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chilcot, J., Rayner, L., Lee, W., Price, A., Goodwin, L., Monroe, B., … Hotopf, M. (2013). The factor structure of the PHQ-9 in palliative care. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 75(1), 6064. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.12.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, D. M. (2011). Implementing NICE guidelines for the psychological treatment of depression and anxiety disorders: The IAPT experience. International Review of Psychiatry, 23(4), 318327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuthbert, B. N., & Insel, T. R. (2013). Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: The seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine, 11(126). https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-11-126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daly, E. J., Trivedi, M. H., Janik, A., Li, H., Zhang, Y., Li, X., … Singh, J. B. (2019). Efficacy of Esketamine nasal spray plus oral antidepressant treatment for relapse prevention in patients with treatment-resistant depression: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(9), 893903. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.1189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doi, S., Ito, M., Takebayashi, Y., Muramatsu, K., & Horikoshi, M. (2018). Factorial validity and invariance of the Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ)-9 among clinical and non-clinical populations. PLoS ONE, 13(7), e0199235. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199235CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, E. I., & Nesse, R. M. (2015). Depression sum-scores don't add up: Why analyzing specific depression symptoms is essential. BMC Medicine, 13(72). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-015-0325-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, D. (2011). The heterogeneity of “major depression.”. World Psychiatry, 10(3), 226228. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2051-5545.2011.tb00061.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gyani, A., Shafran, R., Layard, R., & Clark, D. M. (2013). Enhancing recovery rates: Lessons from year one of IAPT. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(9), 597606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2013.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, S. C.. (2016). Acceptance and commitment therapy, relational frame theory, and the third wave of behavioral and cognitive therapies - republished article. Behavior therapy, 47(6), 869885. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2016.11.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hiller, W., Schindler, A. C., & Lambert, M. J. (2012). Defining response and remission in psychotherapy research: A comparison of the RCI and the method of percent improvement. Psychotherapy Research, 22(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2011.616237CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holtzheimer III, P. E., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2006). Emerging treatments for depression. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 7(17), 23232339. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1517/14656566.7.17.2323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilardi, S. S., & Craighead, W. E. (1994). The role of nonspecific factors in cognitive-behavior therapy for depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 1(2), 138156. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2850.1994.tb00016.xGoogle Scholar
Insel, T. R. (2014). The NIMH research domain criteria (RDoC) project: Precision medicine for psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(4), 395397. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14020138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kazdin, A. E. (2007). Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 127. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, D., Lewis, G., Kaur, S., Wiles, N., King, M., Weich, S., … Peters, T. J. (2009). Therapist-delivered internet psychotherapy for depression in primary care: A randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 374(9690), 628634. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61257-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khan, A., Faucett, J., Lichtenberg, P., Kirsch, I., & Brown, W. A. (2012). A systematic review of comparative efficacy of treatments and controls for depression. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e41778. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041778CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kongsted, A., & Nielsen, A. M. (2017). Latent class analysis in health research. Journal of Physiotherapy, 63(1), 5558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jphys.2016.05.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krause, J. S., Reed, K. S., & McArdle, J. J. (2010). Factor structure and predictive validity of somatic and nonsomatic symptoms from the patient health questionnaire-9: A longitudinal study after spinal cord injury. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 91(8), 12181224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2010.04.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., & Williams, J. B. (2001). The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 16(9), 606613. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamers, F., Cui, L., Hickie, I. B., Roca, C., Machado-Vieira, R., Zarate, C. A., & Merikangas, K. R. (2016). Familial aggregation and heritability of the melancholic and atypical subtypes of depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 204, 241246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, C. T., Leoutsakos, J. M., Lyketsos, C. G., Steffens, D. C., Breitner, J. C. S., & Norton, M. C. (2012). Latent class-derived subgroups of depressive symptoms in a community sample of older adults: The cache county study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 27(10), 10611069. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2824CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, C. T., Stroo, M., Fuemmeler, B., Malhotra, R., & Østbye, T. (2014). Trajectories of depressive symptoms over 2 years postpartum among overweight or obese women. Women's Health Issues, 24(5), 559566. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.whi.2014.05.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Y., Aggen, S., Shi, S., Gao, J., Li, Y., Tao, M., … Kendler, K. S. (2014). Subtypes of major depression: Latent class analysis in depressed Han Chinese women. Psychological Medicine, 44(15), 32753288. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714000749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linden, M., & Rath, K. (2014). The impact of the intensity of single symptoms on the diagnosis and prevalence of major depression. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(7), 15671571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.06.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenzo-Luaces, L., German, R. E., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2015). It's complicated: The relation between cognitive change procedures, cognitive change, and symptom change in cognitive therapy for depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 41, 315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.12.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musil, R., Seemüller, F., Meyer, S., Spellmann, I., Adli, M., Bauer, M., … Riedel, M. (2018). Subtypes of depression and their overlap in a naturalistic inpatient sample of major depressive disorder. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 27(1), e1569. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence. (2009). Depression: The treatment and management of depression in adults. NICE guidelines [CG90]. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpcs.2014.01.022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ni, Y., Tein, J. Y., Zhang, M., Yang, Y., & Wu, G. (2017). Changes in depression among older adults in China: A latent transition analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 209, 39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, S., & Agius, M. (2015). A systematic review of structural and functional MRI differences between psychotic and nonpsychotic depression. Psychiatria Danubina, 27(Suppl 1), S235S239.Google ScholarPubMed
Putnam, K., Robertson-Blackmore, E., Sharkey, K., Payne, J., Bergink, V., Munk-Olsen, T., … Meltzer-Brody, S. (2015). Heterogeneity of postpartum depression: A latent class analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 2(1), 5967. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00055-8Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2018). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://doi.org/10.1108/eb003648Google Scholar
Roth, A. D., & Pilling, S. (2008). Using an evidence-based methodology to identify the competences required to deliver effective cognitive and behavioural therapy for depression and anxiety disorders. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36(2), 129147. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465808004141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B. W., & Löwe, B. (2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine, 166(10), 10921097. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, T. Z., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2006). Reconsidering rapid early response in cognitive behavioral therapy for depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 6(3), 283288. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.6.3.283Google Scholar
Tay, A. K., Jayasuriya, R., Jayasuriya, D., & Silove, D. (2017). Twelve-month trajectories of depressive and anxiety symptoms and associations with traumatic exposure and ongoing adversities: A latent trajectory analysis of a community cohort exposed to severe conflict in Sri Lanka. Translational Psychiatry, 7(8), e1200. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2017.166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tisminetzky, M., Bray, B. C., Miozzo, R., Aupont, O., & McLaughlin, T. J. (2011). Identifying symptom profiles of depression and anxiety in patients with an acute coronary syndrome using latent class and latent transition analysis. The International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 42(2), 195210. https://doi.org/10.2190/PM.42.2.gCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uher, R., Dernovsek, M. Z., Mors, O., Hauser, J., Souery, D., Zobel, A., … Farmer, A. (2011). Melancholic, atypical and anxious depression subtypes and outcome of treatment with escitalopram and nortriptyline. Journal of Affective Disorders, 132(1-2), 112120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulbricht, C. M., Chrysanthopoulou, S. A., Levin, L., & Lapane, K. L. (2018a). The use of latent class analysis for identifying subtypes of depression: A systematic review. Psychiatry Research, 266, 228246. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulbricht, C. M., Dumenci, L., Rothschild, A. J., & Lapane, K. L. (2016). Changes in depression subtypes for women during treatment with citalopram: A latent transition analysis. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 19(5), 769778. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-016-0606-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulbricht, C. M., Dumenci, L., Rothschild, A. J., & Lapane, K. L. (2018b). Changes in depression subtypes Among Men in STAR*D: A latent transition analysis. American Journal of Men's Health, 12(1), 513. https://doi.org/10.1177/1557988315607297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulbricht, C. M., Rothschild, A. J., & Lapane, K. L. (2015). The association between latent depression subtypes and remission after treatment with citalopram: A latent class analysis with distal outcome. Journal of Affective Disorders, 188, 270277. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.039CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wijesinghe, R. (2014). Emerging therapies for treatment resistant depression. Mental Health Clinician, 4(5), 226230. https://doi.org/10.9740/mhc.n207179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, M., Ellison, W., Young, D., Chelminski, I., & Dalrymple, K. (2015). How many different ways do patients meet the diagnostic criteria for major depressive disorder? Comprehensive Psychiatry, 56, 2934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.09.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Patient health questionnaire (PHQ-9).

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Graphical summary of state symptom profiles for the 7-state model. States 1 and 2 represent states of minimal to mild overall severity; State 3 shows peak symptom intensity around feelings of depression, tiredness and low self-esteem (cognitive/affective state); State 5 shows peak symptom intensity around difficulties sleeping, feelings of tiredness, and changes in appetite (somatic state); State 4 shows a relatively even spread in symptom intensity across items (hybrid state); States 6 and 7 represent moderately severe and severe states, respectively.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. (a) Stacked area plots showing transitions between states over time for each starting state; patients leaving treatment were considered to remain at whatever state they last exhibited. (b) Transition probability graph showing the range of transition probabilities across time for each depressive state; transition probabilities below 0.05 for more than half of the time points are omitted; thicker arrows represent the most likely transitions between two given states. A full transition probability matrix is available in online Supplementary Materials.

Figure 3

Table 1. Engagement and clinical outcomes for each starting state

Figure 4

Table 2. Results of logistic regression analysis investigating the relationship between patient demographics and starting state [cognitive/affective (State 3) or somatic (State 5)]

Supplementary material: File

Catarino et al. Supplementary Materials

Catarino et al. Supplementary Materials

Download Catarino et al. Supplementary Materials(File)
File 791.9 KB