Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-xtgtn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-15T07:11:46.487Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Virtual reality cue exposure therapy for tobacco relapse prevention: a comparative study with standard intervention

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2022

Eric Malbos*
Affiliation:
Department of Adult Psychiatry, Conception University Hospital, Marseille, France Equipe Imothep, Institut Fresnel, UMR 7249, Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, Ecole Centrale Marseille, Marseille, France
Baptiste Borwell
Affiliation:
Department of Adult Psychiatry, Conception University Hospital, Marseille, France
Mélodie Einig-Iscain
Affiliation:
Department of Adult Psychiatry, Conception University Hospital, Marseille, France Equipe Imothep, Institut Fresnel, UMR 7249, Aix-Marseille Université, CNRS, Ecole Centrale Marseille, Marseille, France
Théo Korchia
Affiliation:
Department of Adult Psychiatry, Conception University Hospital, Marseille, France
Robin Cantalupi
Affiliation:
Cognitive Psychology Lab, UMR 7290, Aix-Marseille University, Marseille, France
Laurent Boyer
Affiliation:
CEReSS, EA 3279, Center, La Timone Faculty of Medicine, Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France
Christophe Lancon
Affiliation:
Department of Adult Psychiatry, Conception University Hospital, Marseille, France
*
Author for correspondence: Eric Malbos, E-mail: eric.malbos@gmail.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Successful interventions have been developed for smoking cessation although the success of smoking relapse prevention protocols has been limited. Cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) in particular has been hampered by a high relapse rate. Because relapse can be due to conditions associated with tobacco consumption (such as drinking in bars with friends), virtual reality cue exposure therapy (VRCE) can be a potential tool to generate 3D interactive environments that simulate risk situations for relapse prevention procedures.

Methods

To assess the effectiveness of VRCE with CBT, a comparative trial involving 100 smoking abstinent participants was designed with all required virtual environments (VE) created with an inexpensive graphic engine/game level editor.

Results

Outcome measures confirmed the immersive and craving eliciting effect of these VEs. Results demonstrated that more participants in the VRCE group did not experience smoking relapse and that VRCE is at least as efficacious as traditional CBT in terms of craving reduction and decrease in nicotine dependence. Dropout and relapse rate in the VRCE group was noticeably lower than the CBT group. Aside from mood scores, no significant differences were found regarding the other scales.

Conclusion

The present clinical trial provides evidence that VRCE was effective in preventing smoking relapse. Improvement in technology and methodology for future research and applications is delineated.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Drug craving is defined as a strong impetus or desire to use substances and is generally viewed as a central feature of addiction (Sayette et al., Reference Sayette, Shiffman, Tiffany, Niaura, Martin and Schadel2000; Tiffany, Reference Tiffany1990). It has been associated with drug consumption maintenance (Bagot, Heishman, & Moolchan, Reference Bagot, Heishman and Moolchan2007; Brandon, Piasecki, Quinn, & Baker, Reference Brandon, Piasecki, Quinn, Baker, Drummond, Tiffany, Glautier and Remington1995; Shiffman, Engberg, & Paty, Reference Shiffman, Engberg and Paty1997) and has been described as a barrier for individuals trying to quit (Baker, Mermelstein, & Collins, Reference Baker, Mermelstein and Collins2011). In particular, nicotine dependence is the leading cause of preventable morbidity, mortality and health expenses in developed countries (World Health Organisation, 2009). The World Health Organization, in its WHO Report on the Global Tobacco Epidemic, reported that if current tobacco use persists, it will cause the death of more than 8 million people worldwide every year by the year 2030 (World Health Organisation, 2009). This dependence involves compulsive use despite the awareness of adverse consequences and repeated cycles of abstinence and relapse. The high rates of relapse after smoking cessation programmes have ranged between 40% and 70%, suggesting the need to incorporate more effective strategies for relapse prevention into such programmes (Hatsukami, Stead, & Gupta, Reference Hatsukami, Stead and Gupta2008). To better understand the underlying mechanisms of relapse, a cognitive behavioural model was developed by Marlatt and Gordon, in which relapse to drug use was usually associated with high-risk situations characterized by the presence of drug-related stimuli (Marlatt & Gordon, Reference Marlatt and Gordon1985). In addition, several studies have reported that individuals with substance use disorders have physiological and subjective reactions to the presentation of drug-related stimuli, a phenomenon known as cue reactivity (Carter & Tiffany, Reference Carter and Tiffany1999). Recent studies have also reported that cue-induced craving does not decrease over an extended period of abstinence and might actually increase with a longer duration of abstinence (Bedi et al., Reference Bedi, Preston, Epstein, Heishman, Marrone and Shaham2011). Nowadays, several tobacco quitting methods exist such as psychotherapy, nicotine replacement therapy (NRT) and electronic cigarettes. Still, a significant challenge lies in the prevention of relapse (Hatsukami et al., Reference Hatsukami, Stead and Gupta2008). Indeed, unlike what can be observed with other substance abuse, where risk situations are highly specific and can be easily avoided, cigarette smoking is usually related to daily situations that either cannot or should not be avoided (i.e. workplace and coffee break with colleagues, friends smoking during social events or in public places). In this particular context, two techniques have emerged as potential smoking relapse prevention interventions: cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) and cue exposure therapy (CET). CBT aims to explain and treat the individual's cognitive dysfunctions and habits (i.e. dysfunctional beliefs and automatic thoughts related to smoking). This method can increase the individual's motivation and can play an important role in relapse prevention through analysis and understanding of this phenomenon (Guichenez et al., Reference Guichenez, Clauzel, Cungi, Quantin, Godard and Clauzel2007). However, a meta-analysis reported insufficient evidence to support the use of CBT to prevent relapse, and advised the evaluation of alternatives in attempts to teach coping skills in risk situations (Hajek et al., Reference Hajek, Stead, West, Jarvis, Hartmann-Boyce and Lancaster2013). On the other hand, CET is a behavioural approach to drug dependence based on the classical conditioning model (Drummond, Tiffany, Glautier, & Remington, Reference Drummond, Tiffany, Glautier and Remington1995). This treatment involves extinction procedures wherein addict patients are repeatedly exposed to drug-related cues with the aim of decreased reactivity (Carter & Tiffany, Reference Carter and Tiffany1999). CET can be added to CBT and applied through several modalities of exposure including videos, mental imagery procedures and in vivo presentations of cues. However, these situations are difficult to reconstruct effectively in passive video or images as well as in the artificial context of a hospital or an office, thus limiting the efficacy of CET. Because of the complexity of nicotine cue reactivity involving proximal (lit cigarette, ashtray, lighter), contextual (physical situations such as a party or a bar) and complex cues (a combination of contextual and proximal cues, such as situations involving social interactions where people are smoking or offered cigarettes), more ecological environments should be proposed than those used in traditional CET (Traylor, Parrish, Copp, & Bordnick, Reference Traylor, Parrish, Copp and Bordnick2011).

These observations address de facto the need to implement new exposure strategies to help abstinent smokers cope with smoking-related situations in an active way. Consequently, virtual reality, an immersive media allowing subjects to be exposed and interact in computer-generated environments in real time has been considered an option. When virtual reality exposure is associated with drug-related stimuli, this method is entitled virtual reality cue exposure (VRCE) and has been recently under examination as a possible alternative instrument to traditional CBT (García-Rodríguez, Valverde, Maldonado, & García, Reference García-Rodríguez, Valverde, Maldonado and García2009). In the specific field of nicotine dependence treatment, past studies have listed VRCE's possible advantages such as the realistic simulation of situations related to drug use, simultaneous presentation of proximal and distal tobacco cues and ad libitum re-experiencing (Baumann & Sayette, Reference Baumann and Sayette2006). Thus, the use of VR within smoking cessation programmes could be a relevant approach (Lee, Lim, Graham, Kim, & Wiederhold, Reference Lee, Lim, Graham, Kim and Wiederhold2004). Even though previous researches have studied VRCE and demonstrated that artificial 3D situations can induce tobacco craving with success or lead to decrease in nicotine addiction (Girard, Turcotte, Bouchard, & Girard, Reference Girard, Turcotte, Bouchard and Girard2009; Pericot-Valverde, Secades-Villa, Gutiérez_Maldonado, & Garcia-Rodriguez, Reference Pericot-Valverde, Secades-Villa, Gutiérez_Maldonado and Garcia-Rodriguez2014), the efficacy of VRCE on relapse prevention has yet to be analysed.

Therefore, this study sought to investigate the effect of VRCE on smoking relapse prevention in the context of a comparative study involving VRCE and traditional CBT/CET. The objectives of this trial are threefold. The main goal was to evaluate the effectiveness of VRCE compared to traditional CBT/CET in the prevention of smoking relapse and the effect on dependence and craving. Secondly, to measure the impact of VRCE on other psychological features including depression, anxiety, self-esteem and quality of life. Thirdly, this assay aims to ensure that the six virtual environments (VEs) constructed for the experiment produced craving and presence with limited cybersickness. Indeed, a key to successful immersion and therapy is this sense of presence, which has been frequently defined as a feeling of transportation: a sensation of being present in a virtual world (Lombard & Ditton, Reference Lombard and Ditton1997).

The overall efficacy and immersive properties of the prevention protocol and its VEs were assessed with self-report questionnaires as well as and physiological measures.

Methodology

All participants gave written informed consent to their participation in this study in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the study was approved by the Ethics Institutional Review Board CPP Sud Méditerranée. The present trial was registered on ClinicalTrials.gov (Identifier: NCT02205060) and a comprehensive report on the methodology was previously published before the completion of the recruitment process (Giovancarli et al., Reference Giovancarli, Malbos, Baumstarck, Parola, Pélissier, Lançon and Boyer2016).

Sample

One hundred participants (71 women, 29 men) meeting DSM-5 criteria for recent chronic smoking were recruited for the clinical trial through local media and onsite consultations. The inclusion criteria were as follows: male or female, aged of 18 years or more with a past diagnosis of chronic smoking as defined by the DSM-5 and with the presence of at least three of the 11 DSM-5 criteria for nicotine dependence (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Additionally, the participants had to report smoking abstinence for at least 1 week (defined by the total absence of tobacco consumption reported by the individual) and their current abstinence was verified by measuring the carbon monoxide (CO) exhaled at pretest. The accepted threshold for abstinence followed past recommendations for optimal sensitivity and sensibility: a CO exhaled levels of less than 4 parts per million (ppm) (Javors, Hatch, & Lamb, Reference Javors, Hatch and Lamb2005). In order to avoid possible contamination by other therapeutic means, the main exclusion criteria were the simultaneous use of any concurrent method of tobacco abstinence (i.e. NRT, smoking cessation drugs such as bupropion or varenicline, electronic cigarettes, hypnosis). Other exclusion criteria were as follows: pregnancy or breastfeeding; unstable physical or psychiatric disease and contraindications to virtual reality therapy such as photosensitive epilepsy. Sociodemographic characteristics are listed in Table 1.

Table 1. Social and demographics characteristics of the sample

s.d., standard deviation.

Assessments

The primary measure was the proportion of individuals with maintenance of their tobacco abstinence at the completion of the protocol (Lancaster, Stead, & Cahill, Reference Lancaster, Stead and Cahill2008; Mottillo et al., Reference Mottillo, Filion, Bélisle, Joseph, Gervais and O'Loughlin2009). Abstinence is defined by the total absence of tobacco consumption as assessed during a post-treatment inquiry and the systematic use of a CO breath monitor that measures the CO levels exhaled for the sake of a more objective evaluation. Should the results be greater than or equal to the cut-off point of 4 ppm, the participant was considered as having a relapse (Javors et al., Reference Javors, Hatch and Lamb2005; Underner & Peiffer, Reference Underner and Peiffer2010). Other addiction and global psychological well-being psychometric measures include:

Cigarette Dependence Scale (CDS-12) (Etter, Houezec, Huguelet, & Etter, Reference Etter, Houezec, Huguelet and Etter2009), a 12-item self-report instrument which measures the severity of cigarette dependence with scores ranging from 12 (no dependence) to 60 (high dependence). This scale has high test-retest reliability (⩾0.83) and high internal consistency (Cronbach's α ⩾0.84).

French Tobacco Craving Questionnaire (FTCQ-12) (Berlin, Singleton, & Heishman, Reference Berlin, Singleton and Heishman2010), a 12-item self-report instrument with scores ranging from 12 (no craving) to 84 (high craving). The internal consistency α coefficients were 0.83, 0.79, 0.69 and 0.66 for the different factors (emotionality, expectancy, compulsivity, and purposefulness). In session craving itself is also evaluated using an analogical craving scale ranging from 0 (no urge) to 100 (extreme urge) that measures the perceived level of craving at a given time during the exposure to virtual or imaginary smoking-related cues (Sayette & Hufford, Reference Sayette and Hufford1994) during each session.

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) Y-A (state) (Gauthier & Bouchard, Reference Gauthier and Bouchard1993). The STAI is a 20-item self-report instrument with scores ranging from 20 (absence of anxiety) to 80 (high anxiety). The STAI is among the most widely researched and widely used measurements of general anxiety, with satisfactory internal consistency α coefficients (⩾0.7).

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Collet & Cottraux, Reference Collet and Cottraux1986; Steer, Beck, Riskind, & Brown, Reference Steer, Beck, Riskind and Brown1986). The BDI is a 13-item self-report instrument. A total score between 4 and 7 shows a mild state of depression, between 8 and 15 an average to moderate state of depression and 16 or higher a severe state of depression. This scale has satisfactory psychometric properties. A meta-analysis of the BDI's internal consistency estimates yielded a mean α coefficient of 0.81. The concurrent validity of the BDI with respect to clinical ratings and the Hamilton Psychiatric Rating Scale for Depression was also high. The BDI also distinguishes subtypes of depression and differentiates depression from anxiety (Beck, Steer, & Carbin, Reference Beck, Steer and Carbin1988).

SF-12 (Ware, Kosinski, & Turner-Bowker, Reference Ware, Kosinski and Turner-Bowker2002). Quality of life was assessed with this 12-item scale assessing physical function, physical pain, general health, vitality (energy and tiredness), social functioning and well-being as well as limitations due to physical and mental health. Two composite scores are obtained with this self-report instrument: a Physical Component Score (PCS) and a Mental Component Score (MCS). A high score indicates a high-level quality of life. Several studies have reported that the SF-12 is able to produce the two summary scales originally developed from the SF-36, one of the most widely used quality of life instruments, with considerable accuracy and yet with far less of a respondent burden (Jenkinson & Layte, Reference Jenkinson and Layte1997).

Rosenberg Self-esteem scale (EES) (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1965). This is a self-report questionnaire related to self-esteem and consisting of 10 items which produces a total score of 10–40; a high score indicates high self-esteem. This questionnaire is a reliable and valid measurement of global self-worth (Gray-Little, Williams, & Hancock, Reference Gray-Little, Williams and Hancock1997).

In addition to the instruments measuring the psychological and physiological impact, when using virtual reality, a specific questionnaire related to presence (the Presence Questionnaire) is usually required. The most common definition of presence which pertains to the immersion in VEs is the one related to the concept of transportation to elsewhere: the feeling of ‘being there’ (Lombard & Ditton, Reference Lombard and Ditton1997) that is the pregnant impression of being existent in a real or artificially created place (Malbos, Rapee, & Kavakli, Reference Malbos, Rapee and Kavakli2013).

In the framework of experimental conditions where ecologically valid situations are required for the study of behaviour or reactions, presence is of significant importance. Indeed, a key requirement is that a subject will behave in a VE as they would when confronted to similar cues in real life. Similar characteristics are required for the use of virtual reality in clinical interventions.

Side effects of using virtual reality and related apparatus are summarized as cybersickness, a form of motion sickness which induces various symptoms (such as nausea, sweating, dizziness) when the user is immersed in VEs. Following past recommendations(Robillard, Bouchard, & Fournier, Reference Robillard, Bouchard and Fournier2003), measuring cybersickness through questionnaires such as the Simulation Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ) is especially important since cybersickness may hinder perceived presence and consequently the treatment efficiency.

Presence and cybersickness were registered after each VR exposure session in the VRCE group using the Presence Questionnaire PQ v3.0 (Witmer & Singer, Reference Witmer and Singer1998) and the SSQ (Kennedy, Lane, Berbaum, & Lilienthal, Reference Kennedy, Lane, Berbaum and Lilienthal1993). The PQ consists of 32 items rated on a seven-point scale, and factor analysis consists of six factors: involvement, interface quality, adaptation and immersion, consistency with expectation, visual fidelity and auditory fidelity (Witmer & Singer, Reference Witmer and Singer1998). The PQ is a self-report instrument that has been validated in many empirical studies (Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Lane, Berbaum and Lilienthal1993; Price & Page, Reference Price and Page2007; Witmer, Jerome, & Singer, Reference Witmer, Jerome and Singer2005; Witmer & Singer, Reference Witmer and Singer1998) and has also demonstrated a relationship between presence and anxiety (Price & Page, Reference Price and Page2007). A high score indicates a satisfactory perception of presence.

The SSQ is a 16-item self-report instrument with scores ranging from 16 (absence of cybersickness) to 48 (high cybersickness). Factor analysis consists of three main factors: oculomotor (i.e. blurred vision), disorientation (i.e. dizziness) and nausea (i.e. vomiting).

Heart rate (HR) and heart rate variability (HRV) were monitored and carried out throughout each session in the VRCE group using a Polar RS800CX™ device, which includes a transmitter and a wrist receiver. This monitor has proved its efficacy in clinical research (Quintana, Heathers, & Kemp, Reference Quintana, Heathers and Kemp2012). The HRV indicates the fluctuations in HR around an average HR (Vanderlei, Silva, Pastre, Azevedo, & Godoy, Reference Vanderlei, Silva, Pastre, Azevedo and Godoy2008). HRV was assessed by calculating a time domain variable entitled root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) which is the square root of the mean squared difference of successive RR or NN waves, and by the pNN50 which is the proportion of adjacent R or N waves more than 50 ms (Vanderlei et al., Reference Vanderlei, Silva, Pastre, Azevedo and Godoy2008). HR and HRV reflect the autonomic responses involved in emotional arousal, most notably during anxiety or craving where the HR is expected to increase and the HRV to decrease (Bernston & Cacioppo, Reference Bernston and Cacioppo2007; Carter & Tiffany, Reference Carter and Tiffany1999). There is also evidence that HR changes are correlated with presence (Wiederhold, Jang, Kaneda, Cabral, & Lurie, Reference Wiederhold, Jang, Kaneda, Cabral, Lurie, Riva and Galimberti2003). HR and HRV represent an alternative objective measurement of anxiety response, presence and craving.

Apparatus and virtual environments

The VR system includes a ruggedized Sensics ZSight HMD (1280 × 2024 stereoscopic OLED screen with 60° field of view), coupled with an embedded 3 degrees of freedom head tracker (angular resolution: 0.05°, latency 8 ms). The head tracker enables the participant to visually explore the environment by updating the 3D scene as a function of head orientation. Otherwise, navigation is triggered by mouse motion. The participant's locomotion direction is defined by his or her head orientation in the VEs. The participants must use a wireless controller with a directional pad for walking or swimming locomotion. The steering wheel exploited for driving VE was a Logitech G29 with vibration and force feedback capabilities. The VEs are generated and run on an ordinary graphics-orientated notebook with a four-core processor, 16Go DDR2 RAM, a graphics card with 3 Go RAM and a 1440 × 900 resolution screen. The required software is Microsoft Windows 10 (64-bit edition), Microsoft DirectX 9.0 or higher and the equipment's drivers.

The main software exploited to create and run the VEs was Sandbox. Sandbox was an inexpensive (30–60 Euro/USD) and commercially available game level editor (GLE) of the video game Crysis 1/2, exploiting the CryEngine graphic engine developed by Crytek GmbH. Prior to its full use for the trial, this GLE was tested and compared to seven other commercially available GLEs by considering several distinct criteria and requirements previously reported (Malbos, Rapee, & Kavakli, Reference Malbos, Rapee and Kavakli2011). To construct the VEs, the investigator exploited the aforementioned GLE to build six specific cue-graded VEs related to smoking. The VEs were selected to represent common situations of daily life involving high-risk situations in terms of smoking relapse (Garcia-Rodriguez, Ferrer-Garcia, Pericot-Valverde, Gutierrez-Maldonado, & Secades-Villa, Reference Garcia-Rodriguez, Ferrer-Garcia, Pericot-Valverde, Gutierrez-Maldonado and Secades-Villa2011; Garcia-Rodriguez, Pericot-Valverde, Gutierrez, Ferrer-Garcia, & Secades-Villa, Reference Garcia-Rodriguez, Pericot-Valverde, Gutierrez, Ferrer-Garcia and Secades-Villa2012). These VEs are validated smoking eliciting situations according to empirical works on tobacco consumption (Beck, Wright, Newman, & Liese, Reference Beck, Wright, Newman and Liese2001; Marlatt & Gordon, Reference Marlatt and Gordon1985), past studies (Girard et al., Reference Girard, Turcotte, Bouchard and Girard2009; Pericot-Valverde et al., Reference Pericot-Valverde, Secades-Villa, Gutiérez_Maldonado and Garcia-Rodriguez2014) and the criteria of the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). These six VEs offer distinct craving-inducing scenarios: having a drink with people smoking in a virtual beach bar at sunset; walking with avatars smoking on the terrace of a restaurant; being in a furnished living room or its balcony with a beer, an ashtray and a lighted cigarette; waiting at a bus stop with avatars smoking around; taking a break in a workplace with smoker colleagues and driving a virtual car on a road during a traffic jam.

During exposure, the investigator can trigger specific events within the VE (i.e. avatars talking about smoking or inviting the participants to smoke a cigarette or drink a cup of coffee). These options allow for progressive increases in the intensity of induced craving to modulate the degree of exposure at various times. Dynamic VEs also provide the participant with direct, realistic interactions (such as opening doors, virtual human interactions, grabbing objects and physical or mechanical reactions to the user's presence) (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1. Screenshots of four VEs constructed for the present study. Note the smoking-related cues (cigarettes and packets, ashtray, alcohol) and the avatars' smoking attitudes (beach bar, restaurant, bus stop and interior with balcony).

Procedure

Following the intake assessment and the diagnostic interview, the participants were randomly assigned to two therapeutic groups: one group receiving VRCE and one group receiving traditional CBT. The allocation to each group was determined using a stratified random sampling method. The protocol included 8 weekly sessions of 45 min each for both groups. All participants were taught addiction centred psychoeducation (addiction modelization), craving management, relaxation, positive self-statements, assertiveness (how to refuse a cigarette) and cognitive restructuring as outlined by several works of reference (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Wright, Newman and Liese2001; Marlatt & Gordon, Reference Marlatt and Gordon1985). The first two sessions were entirely dedicated to acquiring these methods while following sessions provided additional instruments before exposure. The only difference between the groups was the smoking related exposure procedure carried out from the 3rd to 8th session. While participants in the CBT group were asked to visualize such conditions, the participants in the VRCE group were immersed in a computerized world using virtual reality equipment. Both methods aim to reduce craving or cue reactivity by habituation or extinction. Moreover, throughout the exposure sessions, the participants were regularly encouraged to review and apply the methods acquired previously. Given individual differences in the relevance of craving-specific stimuli and to determine the order of exposure to virtual or imagined situations, a list was established with each participant independently, from the least craving inducing to the most craving inducing environment. In the VRCE group, VR exposure was presented in gradual context, since smoking-related cues can vary in intensity (i.e. number of avatars smoking around the patient, presence of cigarette boxes spread on tables). Throughout the exposure to VEs, the participants were invited to progress to the next VE when they had reached a level of emotion or craving considered as comfortable in the current environment (between 0 and 20 on the craving scale). Additionally, to optimize habituation or extinction, exposure to a particular event or situation could be repeated at the participant's discretion.

Results

The progression of participants through the protocol is summarized in the flowchart (Fig. 2). Twenty-nine participants dropped out at different stages of the protocol for different reasons (18 in the CBT group v. 11 in the VRCE group).

Fig. 2. Flowchart of the different stages of inclusion and progression through the protocol.

Regarding the cumulative choice of VEs over the course of the six sessions in the VRCE group, 32.3% of the participants selected the beach bar, 22.3% the workplace with a break, 17.1% the living room or balcony, 11.7% the restaurant, 9.2% the bus stop, 7.1% the traffic jam.

Effects of virtual reality exposure on craving, presence and cybersickness

In the VRCE group, the VEs were able to produce tobacco craving among the participants with a mean craving level at 21.26/100 (s.d. = 9.97) across all sessions and at 28.59/100 (s.d. = 16.01) at its peak during the fourth session and a maximum craving level 30.23/100 (s.d. = 12.36) at across all sessions and at 40.64/100 (s.d. = 20.84) at its peak during the fourth session (cf. Fig. 3). For the following sessions (S5 to S8), craving scores decreased progressively and significantly (cf. Table 2 and Fig. 3). Regarding presence and possible VR side effects, mean PQ high scores (PQ = 113.77, s.d. = 13.98) and mean SSQ low scores (SSQ = 6.30, s.d. = 5.07) indicated that the participants felt immersed in the VEs without exhibiting strong cybersickness. The PQ raised over time significantly while the SSQ was ebbing away as detailed in Table 2 and represented in Figs 3 and 4.

Fig. 3. Line representation of mean SSQ, mean and maximum craving, RMSSD and Pnn50 across all exposure sessions in the VRCE group (sessions 3–8). SSQ, Simulation Sickness Questionnaire; RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences (ms); pNN50, proportion of adjacent R waves more than 50 ms (%).

Fig. 4. Line representation of mean PQ, HR and HRmax across all exposure sessions in the VRCE group (sessions 3–8). PQ, Presence Questionnaire; HR, mean heart rate per min; HRmax maximum heart rate per min.

Table 2. Mean, standard deviations of the dependent variables, results of two-way ANOVA between the third session S3 (beginning of exposure session) and eighth session S8 (post-test) (time)

ANOVA, analysis of variance; VRCE, virtual reality cue exposure; PQ, Presence Questionnaire; SSQ, Simulation Sickness Questionnaire; Craving, mean craving level; HR; HRmax, maximum heart rate per min; RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences (ms); pNN50, proportion of adjacent R waves more than 50 ms (%).

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.025; *p < 0.05; ns: non-significant.

Effects of treatment on symptoms and physiology

Questionnaire scores and physiological measures are detailed in Tables 2 and 3 and represented in Figs 3–5. A two-way mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare the two groups over time (pretest v. post-test) and interaction (VRCE v. CBT). There is evidence of significant main time effects for all subjective measures related to smoking dependence and tobacco craving namely the CDS-12, FTCQ-12 and in session mean craving. Significant time effects were also found for the BDI but not for the two factors of the SF-12 nor the EES. Regarding group comparison, examination of group by time interactions indicated no significant interactions for any variable. However, according to the record of CO exhaled upon finishing the protocol with a cut-up value at 4 ppm, five participants (7.1%) of the VRCE group and nine (12.8%) in the CBT group were in relapse (cf. Table 4).

Fig. 5. Representation of mean and standard deviations of the dependent variables between pretest and post-test. VRCE, virtual reality cue exposure; CBT, cognitive behavioural therapy; CDS12, Cigarette Dependence Scale; FTCQ12: French tobacco Craving Questionnaire.

Table 3. Mean, standard deviations of the dependent variables, results of two-way ANOVA between pre- and post-test period (time), between the third session (beginning of exposure session) and eighth session (post-test), and ANOVA for time × group comparison (interaction)

ANOVA, analysis of variance; VRCE, virtual reality cue exposure; CBT, cognitive behavioural therapy; CDS, Cigarette Dependence Scale; FTCQ, French tobacco Craving Questionnaire; STAI, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; BDI, Beck Depressive Inventory. SF12ph/me: physical and mental factors of the SF12; EES: Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale.

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.025; *p < 0.05; ns: non- significant.

Table 4. Distribution of participants according to CO exhaled at post-test

CO, exhaled carbon monoxide; ppm, parts per million

With respect to the analysis of cardiophysiological recordings in the VRCE group as reported in Table 2 and Fig. 4, a slight decrease of the average HR and maximum HR between the first and the last VR exposure session can be observed, although it did not reach statistical significance. Concerning the HRV parameters and their elevation between the first and the last VR exposure session, a significant time effect for the pNN50 was found (S3 to S8: F = 7.60; p < 0.01).

Correlation

For systematic analysis, correlations were evaluated between demographic values using Pearson calculation. These results demonstrated significant negative correlations between video game experience and age (r = −0.246; p < 0.04), and between marital status and level of education (r = −0.28; p < 0.02).

Regarding presence, the influence of gaming experience on immersion was verified: there was minimal non-significant correlation between overall PQ and gaming experience (r = −0.12; p < 0.48). Interaction between presence and craving was also evaluated: small non-significant positive correlations were found between PQ and overall maximum craving (r = 0.21; p < 0.21) but not between PQ and overall mean craving (r = 0.09; p < 0.57). Moreover, to investigate any relationship between perceived presence and treatment efficacy, difference scores were calculated between pre-test and post-test for all dependent variables and correlations were computed between these scores, and mean PQ across all sessions. Large to minimal significant or non-significant positive correlations were found between PQ and pre-post difference for BDI (r = 0.61; p < 0.019), FTCQ-12 (r = 0.51; p < 0.38), SF-12 physical (r = 0.18; p < 0.53), CDS-12 (r = 0.13; p < 0.83) and STAI-A (r = 0.10; p < 0.95). Small to medium significant or insignificant negative correlations were found between PQ and pre-post difference for EES (r = −0.36; p < 0.19) and SF-12 mental (r = −0.23; p < 0.42).

With respect to the effect of presence on physiological parameters evolution, HR and HRV difference rates were calculated between the third and last sessions, small to medium insignificant positive correlations were noted between PQ and mean HR (r = 0.27; p < 0.11) and maximum HR (r = 0.30; p < 0.076). Small to medium insignificant negative correlations were noted between PQ and RMSSD (r = −0.21; p < 0.22) and pNN50 (r = −0.15; p < 0.36).

No correlations were computed through the six exposure sessions between PQ and SSQ scores on one hand and the craving/physiological scores on the other hand, as their distinct evolution over time indicated a different linear/non-linear pattern (cf. Figs 3 and 4).

We hypothesized that during initial VR sessions, it was common to observe slightly more discomfort when using the VR apparatus if the participants were not familiar with its use. An increase in HRV as well as some symptoms such as nausea can be both triggered by the parasympathetic system (Lanier, Minsky, Fisher, & Druin, Reference Lanier, Minsky, Fisher and Druin1989; Riva et al., Reference Riva, Botella, Baños, Mantovani, García-Palacios and Quero2015). Thus, correlations were searched between cybersickness and physiological measures. However, for the first VR session, no correlations were demonstrated between SSQ and RMSSD (r = 0.09; p < 0.58), pNN50 (r = 0.01; p > 0.97) and mean HR (r = −0.04; p < 0.79).

Discussion

The present clinical trial provides evidence that VRCE was effective in preventing smoking relapse with a greater effect than traditional CBT/CET: more participants in the VRCE group who completed the protocol did not experience smoking relapse as attested by post-test exhaled CO. Additionally, there were fewer dropouts in VRCE group. We posit that the participants in the VRCE group were more motivated considering that several past trials related to anxiety disorders treatment demonstrated a general preference for VR upon other traditional exposure methods [35].

Other outcomes revealed a significant reduction in tobacco craving and dependence in both groups, and aside from smoking-related variables, a small increase in mood was also observed. However, no significant differences were found regarding the other scales, even though an improvement in mental quality of life was observed but was not enough to reach statistical significance. Group comparison between all variables did not lead to any significant interactions, indicating that VRCE is at least as effective as traditional CBT/CET in terms of craving and dependence. When considering cardiophysiological measures in the VRCE group, after an initial increase of cardiac activity, the VR exposure combined with the use of cognitive and relaxation techniques lead to an overall (although non-significant) reduction of the HR and a significant increase of the HRV at the end of the protocol. As the HRV reflects emotional arousal, most notably anxiety, and is known to be a predictor of craving (Newman, Szkodny, Llera, & Przeworski, Reference Newman, Szkodny, Llera and Przeworski2011), the improvement might be due to the reduction of one or both of these factors.

On the technical and creative side, VEs constructed with an inexpensive off-the-shelf graphic engine/GLE were able to generate sufficient presence and craving among the sample of abstinent smokers. In the VRCE group, it is noteworthy that participants mainly selected the beach bar, the workplace and the living room with a balcony for their cue exposure sessions in VR.

Concerning the potential influence of past exposure to electronic entertainment devices, the correlations carried out on the demographic values have revealed that the younger the participants were, the more experience they had with video games, however this experience had almost no effect on perceived presence in VEs. Involvement inside VEs was sought to ensure that participants would behave in a VE as they would when exposed to similar craving cues in reality, the effect of presence is of significant importance. Consequently, correlations between presence scores and self-reported questionnaires or cardiophysiological parameters were calculated and lead to significant and non-significant positive correlations. They seemed to point towards a possible relationship between the degree to which participants felt immersed inside the VEs and the therapeutic outcomes on dependence, craving, mood and cardiophysiology scores as controlled by the autonomic nervous system.

Interestingly, after listing marginal accounts from some of the participants in the VRCE group, we discovered that nausea generated by cybersickness was accidentally associated with tobacco cues as they occurred simultaneously within the VE. Consequently, when exposed to real cigarettes or people smoking outside the clinical settings, these participants reported feeling the same nausea and even disgust. This unscheduled consequence due to punishing conditioning created in VR could be useful in the context of drug aversive therapy. Such aversive procedures are indeed, already exploited for the treatment of alcohol dependence by prescribing antabuse medications. Further investigation of this aspect can be achieved by generating cybersickness voluntarily during VRCE and is a feasible process (i.e. by suddenly lowering the frame rate or provoking external orientational shift of the user's view when seeing tobacco cues).

Even though smoking-related VEs induced craving by sensory stimulations (visual, auditory and proprioceptive) and bodes well for therapeutic efficiency in a clinical setting, we exercise caution in our statements regarding its use in relapse prevention. Several key limitations need to be kept in mind when interpreting these results. First of all, abstinence being one of the inclusion criteria, some participants quit smoking without proper supervision and complained about strong craving and withdrawal symptoms exhibited at the early stage of the protocol. This may explain some of the drop out cases and prompt towards the inception of a more comprehensive care when facing future abstinence. Secondly, this comparative study being the first in smoking relapse prevention, it was decided to isolate the VRCE effect by excluding any other simultaneous form of therapy (most notably NRT or electronic cigarettes). We surmise that the combination of VRCE and other therapeutic instruments could optimize relapse prevention. Future studies should test this assumption for better results. Thirdly, the restricted field of view of the HMD utilized was relatively narrow by today's standard (60° while recent models can cover the complete field of view: 200°). Also, follow-up results are ongoing and will have to demonstrate the VRCE lasting effects. Lastly, the absence of olfactive stimuli such as smoke odours (computer-controlled odour banks can now diffuse specific scents when encountering a targeted event in VR) might have restricted the multisensory integration of the craving cues present in the VEs and limited the craving intensity. In fine, we encourage researchers, clinicians and users alike to make the most of the current ubiquitous access of VR, telepsychotherapy, gamification of therapeutic process, recent multi-sensorial equipment with olfactive cues, avatars with a strong artificial intelligence, presence of the therapist inside the VEs themselves and easy access to VE creation for the betterment of mental health in real worlds, metaverses and virtual universes.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Jean Dhorne, Frédérique Durieux, Lucie Archambault, the ARC monitors team of the DRS as well as the reviewers for their support, advice and help.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Bagot, K., Heishman, S., & Moolchan, E. (2007). Tobacco craving predicts lapse to smoking among adolescent smokers in cessation treatment. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 9(6), 647652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, T., Mermelstein, R., & Collins, L. (2011). New methods for tobacco dependence treatment research. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 41(2), 192207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, S., & Sayette, M. (2006). Smoking cues in a virtual world provoke craving in cigarette smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(4), 484489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, A., Steer, A., & Carbin, M. (1988). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(1), 77100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, A. T., Wright, F. D., Newman, C. F., & Liese, B. S. (2001). Cognitive therapy of substance abuse. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Bedi, G., Preston, K., Epstein, D., Heishman, S., Marrone, G., & Shaham, Y. (2011). Incubation of cue-induced cigarette craving during abstinence in human smokers. Biological Psychiatry, 69(7), 708711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berlin, I., Singleton, E. G., & Heishman, S. J. (2010). Validity of the 12-item French version of the Tobacco Craving Questionnaire in treatment-seeking smokers. Nicotine and Tobacco Research, 12(5), 500507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernston, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Heart variability: Stress and psychiatric conditions. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Brandon, T., Piasecki, T., Quinn, E., & Baker, T. (1995). Cue exposure treatment in nicotine dependence. In Drummond, D. C., Tiffany, S. T., Glautier, S., & Remington, B. (Eds.), Addictive behaviour: Cue exposure theory and practice (pp. 211227). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Carter, B. L., & Tiffany, S. T. (1999). Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction, 94(3), 327340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collet, L., & Cottraux, J. (1986). The shortened Beck Depression Inventory: Study of the concurrent validity with the Hamilton scale and Widlöcher's retardation scale. L'Encephale, 12(2), 7779.Google ScholarPubMed
Drummond, S., Tiffany, S. T., Glautier, S., & Remington, B. (1995). Cue exposure in understanding and treating addictive behaviors. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Etter, J. F., Houezec, J. L., Huguelet, P., & Etter, M. (2009). Testing the Cigarette Dependence Scale in 4 samples of daily smokers: Psychiatric clinics, smoking cessation clinics, a smoking cessation website and in the general population psychiatric clinics, smoking cessation clinics, a smoking cessation website and in the general population. Addictive Behaviors, 34(5), 446–445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
García-Rodríguez, O., Valverde, I. P., Maldonado, J. G., & García, M. F. (2009). La Realidad Virtual como estrategia para la mejora de los tratamientos del tabaquismo [Virtual Reality as a strategy to improve smoking treatment]. Health and Addictions, 9(1), 3955.Google Scholar
Garcia-Rodriguez, O., Ferrer-Garcia, M., Pericot-Valverde, I., Gutierrez-Maldonado, J., & Secades-Villa, R. (2011). Identifying specific cues and contexts related to smoking craving for the development of effective virtual environments. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 14(3), 9197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Rodriguez, O., Pericot-Valverde, I., Gutierrez, J., Ferrer-Garcia, M., & Secades-Villa, R. (2012). Validation of smoking-related virtual environments for cue exposure therapy. Addictive Behaviors, 37, 703708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauthier, J., & Bouchard, S. (1993). Adaptation canadienne-française de la forme révisée du State-Trait Anxiety Inventory de Spielberger. Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 25, 559578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giovancarli, C., Malbos, E., Baumstarck, K., Parola, N., Pélissier, M. F., Lançon, C., … Boyer, L. (2016). Virtual reality cue exposure for the relapse prevention of tobacco consumption: A study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials, 17(96), 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girard, B., Turcotte, V., Bouchard, S., & Girard, B. (2009). Crushing virtual cigarettes reduces tobacco addiction and treatment discontinuation. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 12(5), 477483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray-Little, B., Williams, V., & Hancock, T. (1997). An item response theory analysis of the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(5), 443451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guichenez, P., Clauzel, I., Cungi, C., Quantin, X., Godard, P., & Clauzel, A. M. (2007). Apport des thérapies cognitivo-comportementales dans le sevrage tabagique. Revue des maladies respiratoires, 24(2), 171182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hajek, P., Stead, L., West, R., Jarvis, M., Hartmann-Boyce, J., & Lancaster, T. (2013). Relapse prevention interventions for smoking cessation. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, (8), 14651858.Google ScholarPubMed
Hatsukami, D. K., Stead, L. F., & Gupta, P. C. (2008). Tobacco addiction. Lancet, 371(9629), 20272038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Javors, M., Hatch, J., & Lamb, R. (2005). Cut-off levels for breath carbon monoxide as a marker for cigarette smoking. Addiction, 100(2), 159167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkinson, C., & Layte, R. (1997). Development and testing of the UK SF-12 (short form health survey). Journal of Health Services Research & Policy, 2(1), 814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, R. S., Lane, N. E., Berbaum, K. S., & Lilienthal, M. G. (1993). Simulator sickness questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 3(3), 203220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancaster, T., Stead, L., & Cahill, K. (2008). An update on therapeutics for tobacco dependence. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 9(1), 1522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanier, J., Minsky, M., Fisher, S., & Druin, A. (1989). Virtual environments and interactivity: Windows to the future. ACM Siggraph Computer Graph, 23, 718.Google Scholar
Lee, J., Lim, Y., Graham, S. J., Kim, G., & Wiederhold, B. K. (2004). Nicotine craving and cue exposure therapy using virtual environments. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 7, 705713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lombard, M., & Ditton, T. (1997). At the heart of it all: The concept of presence. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 3(2), retrieved March 15, 2008, from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/loi/pres.Google Scholar
Malbos, E., Rapee, R. M., & Kavakli, M. (2011). The Use of Game Level Editors to Create Virtual Environments for the Treatment of Anxiety: Effects on Presence and Anxiety Levels. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 13th Virtual Reality International Conference (VRIC 2011), April 6–8 2011, Laval, France.Google Scholar
Malbos, E., Rapee, R. M., & Kavakli, M. (2013). A controlled study of agoraphobia and the independent effect of virtual reality exposure therapy. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 47(2), 160168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (1985). Relapse prevention : Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors. New York: Guilford press.Google Scholar
Mottillo, S., Filion, K., Bélisle, P., Joseph, L., Gervais, A., & O'Loughlin, J. (2009). Behavioural interventions for smoking cessation: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. European Heart Journal, 30(6), 718730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, M., Szkodny, L., Llera, S., & Przeworski, A. (2011). A review of technology-assisted self-help and minimal contact therapies for anxiety and depression: Is human contact necessary for therapeutic efficacy? Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 89103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pericot-Valverde, I., Secades-Villa, R., Gutiérez_Maldonado, J., & Garcia-Rodriguez, O. (2014). Effects of systematic cue exposure through virtual reality on cigarette craving. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 16(11), 14701477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, M., & Page, A. (2007). The role of presence in virtual reality exposure therapy. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 742751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quintana, D. S., Heathers, J. A., & Kemp, A. H. (2012). On the validity of using the Polar RS800 heart rate monitor for heart rate variability research. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 112(12), 41794180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riva, G., Botella, C., Baños, R., Mantovani, F., García-Palacios, A., & Quero, S. (2015). Presence-inducing media for mental health applications. Immersed in media (pp. 283–332). Berlin: SpringerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robillard, G., Bouchard, S., & Fournier, T. (2003). Anxiety and presence during VR immersion, a comparative study of the reactions of phobic and non phobic participants in therapeutic virtual environments derived from computer games. Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 6(5), 467476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and The adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sayette, M., Shiffman, S., Tiffany, S., Niaura, R., Martin, C., & Schadel, W. (2000). The measurement of drug craving. Addiction, 95(S2(8)), 189210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sayette, M. A., & Hufford, M. R. (1994). Effects of cue exposure and deprivation on cognitive resources in smokers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(4), 812818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiffman, S., Engberg, J., & Paty, J. (1997). A day at a time: Predicting smoking lapse from daily urge. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 104116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steer, R., Beck, A., Riskind, J., & Brown, G. (1986). Differentiation of depressive disorders from generalized anxiety by the Beck Depression Inventory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42(3), 475478.3.0.CO;2-D>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiffany, S. (1990). A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonautomatic processes. Psychological Review, 97(2), 147168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Traylor, A., Parrish, D., Copp, H., & Bordnick, P. (2011). Using virtual reality to investigate complex and contextual cue reactivity in nicotine dependent problem drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 36(11), 10681075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Underner, M., & Peiffer, G. (2010). Interprétation des valeurs du CO expiré en tabacologie [expired CO values interpretation in tabacology]. Revue des Maladies Respiratoires, 27(4), 293300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanderlei, L. C. M., Silva, R. A., Pastre, C. M., Azevedo, F. M., & Godoy, M. F. (2008). Comparison of the polar S810i monitor and the ECG for the analysis of heart rate variability in the time and frequency domains. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 41(10), 854859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ware, J. E., Kosinski, M., & Turner-Bowker, D. M. (2002). User's manual for the SF-12v2TM health survey with a supplement documenting SF-12® health survey. Lincoln, RI: Quality Metric Incorporated.Google Scholar
Wiederhold, B. K., Jang, D. P., Kaneda, M., Cabral, I., & Lurie, Y. (2003). An investigation into physiological responses in virtual environments: An objective measurement of presence. In Riva, G., & Galimberti, C. (Eds.), Towards cyberpsychology: Mind, cognitions and society in the internet age (pp. 176182). Amsterdam: Ios Press.Google Scholar
Witmer, B. G., Jerome, C. J., & Singer, M. J. (2005). The factor structure of the presence questionnaire. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 14(3), 298312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witmer, B. G., & Singer, M. J. (1998). Measuring presence in virtual environments: A presence questionnaire. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7(3), 225240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organisation (2009). WHO report on the global tobacco epidemic, 2009: Implementing smoke-free environments. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Social and demographics characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Screenshots of four VEs constructed for the present study. Note the smoking-related cues (cigarettes and packets, ashtray, alcohol) and the avatars' smoking attitudes (beach bar, restaurant, bus stop and interior with balcony).

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Flowchart of the different stages of inclusion and progression through the protocol.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Line representation of mean SSQ, mean and maximum craving, RMSSD and Pnn50 across all exposure sessions in the VRCE group (sessions 3–8). SSQ, Simulation Sickness Questionnaire; RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences (ms); pNN50, proportion of adjacent R waves more than 50 ms (%).

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Line representation of mean PQ, HR and HRmax across all exposure sessions in the VRCE group (sessions 3–8). PQ, Presence Questionnaire; HR, mean heart rate per min; HRmax maximum heart rate per min.

Figure 5

Table 2. Mean, standard deviations of the dependent variables, results of two-way ANOVA between the third session S3 (beginning of exposure session) and eighth session S8 (post-test) (time)

Figure 6

Fig. 5. Representation of mean and standard deviations of the dependent variables between pretest and post-test. VRCE, virtual reality cue exposure; CBT, cognitive behavioural therapy; CDS12, Cigarette Dependence Scale; FTCQ12: French tobacco Craving Questionnaire.

Figure 7

Table 3. Mean, standard deviations of the dependent variables, results of two-way ANOVA between pre- and post-test period (time), between the third session (beginning of exposure session) and eighth session (post-test), and ANOVA for time × group comparison (interaction)

Figure 8

Table 4. Distribution of participants according to CO exhaled at post-test