Hostname: page-component-76c49bb84f-lxspr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-07-06T01:45:51.251Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Two Models of Group Behavior in the Solution of Eureka-Type Problems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2025

Irving Lorge
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University
Herbert Solomon
Affiliation:
Teachers College, Columbia University

Abstract

A study by Shaw (7) some twenty years ago is frequently cited by social scientists to support the generalization that groups are superior to individuals in problem-solving. Shaw suggests that personal interaction within the group is responsible for the superior performance of groups. This article re-examines her data in the light of two models which propose that the difference in quality of solution between group and individual performance is solely a matter of ability. It is shown that Shaw's data may be considered to have been an outcome of behavior postulated by the models. Since Shaw's observations relate to a special population and to special kinds of problems, the proposed models may not be appropriate under differing experimental conditions. In fact, Lorge et al. (4) have indicated that experimental demonstration of the superiority of groups over individuals in problem-solving depends not only on the kind of group but also on the kind of problem to be solved. In addition, the diversity of transfer of training for groups and for individuals is considered.

Information

Type
Original Paper
Copyright
Copyright © 1955 The Psychometric Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Article purchase

Temporarily unavailable

Footnotes

*

Supported in part by the Office of Naval Research under Contract N6 onr 266 (21) and the Air Force Personnel and Training Research Center under Contract AF 18(600)-341.

References

Cook, T. W. Amount of material and difficulty of problem solving. II. The disc transfer problem. J. exp. Psychol., 1937, 20, 288296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenhart, C. Inverse sine transformation of proportions. In Eisenhart, , Hastay, , Wallis, (Eds.), Techniques of statistical analysis, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1947Google Scholar
Kasner, E. and Newman, J.. Mathematics and the imagination, New York: Simon and Schuster, 1940Google Scholar
Lorge, I., Fox, D., Davitz, J., Weltz, P., Herrold, K. Products of individual and of group: Review of literature. Unpublished manuscript developed in connection with Contract AF 33 (038) 28792, Human Resources Research Institute, Maxwell Air Force Base, Alabama.Google Scholar
National Bureau of Standards. Tables of the binomial probability distribution, Applied Mathematics Series 6, January 1950.Google Scholar
Paulson, E. and Wallis, A. Planning and analyzing experiments for comparing two percentages. In Eisenhart, , Hastay, , and Wallis, , Techniques of statistical analysis, McGraw-Hill, 1947.Google Scholar
Shaw, M. E. Comparison of individuals and small groups in the rational solution of complex problems. Amer. J. Psychol., 1932, 44, 491504CrossRefGoogle Scholar