Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-19T03:29:33.668Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Infant Crying: Pattern of Weeping, Recognition of Emotion and Affective Reactions in Observers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2013

Mariano Chóliz*
Affiliation:
Universidad de Valencia (Spain)
Enrique G. Fernández-Abascal
Affiliation:
Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (Spain)
Francisco Martínez-Sánchez
Affiliation:
Universidad de Murcia (Spain)
*
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mariano Chóliz. Departamento de Psicología Básica, Universidad de Valencia. Blasco Ibáñez, 21. 46010-Valencia (Spain). Phone: +34-963864853. Fax: +34-963864822. E-mail: Mariano.Choliz@uv.es

Abstract

This study has three objectives: a) to describe the main differences in the crying patterns produced by the three affective states most closely related to crying: fear, anger and pain; b) to study the adults' accuracy in the recognition of the affective states related to the infant's crying, and c) to analyze the emotional reaction that infant crying elicits in the observers. Results reveal that the main differences appear in the ocular activity and in the pattern of weeping. The infants maintain their eyes open during the crying produced by fear and anger, but in the case of crying provoked by painful stimuli, the eyes remain closed almost all the time. In regard to the pattern of weeping, the crying gradually increase in the case of anger, but the weeping reaches its maximum intensity practically from the beginning in the case of pain and fear. In spite of these differences, it is not easy to know the cause that produces crying in infants, especially in the case of fear or anger. Although observers can't recognize the cause of crying, the emotional reaction is greater when the baby cries in pain than when the baby cries because of fear or anger.

Este trabajo tiene tres objetivos: a) describir las diferencias en el patrón de llanto provocado por tres emociones características del llanto: miedo, enfado y dolor; b) estudiar el grado de precisión que tienen los adultos para reconocer la emoción que ha provocado el llanto y c) analizar la reacción afectiva de los observadores ante el llanto de los bebés. Los resultados obtenidos ponen de manifiesto que las principales diferencias se presentan en la actividad ocular y en la dinámica del llanto. Así, cuando los bebés lloran a causa del enfado, o por miedo, permanecen con los ojos abiertos, mientras que los mantienen cerrados durante todo el tiempo en el caso del dolor. En lo que se refiere a la dinámica del llanto, la intensidad se va incrementando gradualmente en el caso del enfado, mientras que aparece en su máxima intensidad desde el primer momento en el caso del dolor y del miedo. Pese a ello, no es fácil reconocer qué emoción es la que está provocando el llanto, especialmente con el enfado y miedo. Sin embargo, aunque no sepan reconocer la causa del llanto, el dolor provoca una reacción afectiva más intensa en los adultos que el enfado o miedo.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G. (2006). Crying behavior and its importance for psychosocial development in children. In Tremblay, R. E. Barr, R. G., & Peters, R. D. (Eds.), Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. Montreal, Quebec: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development. Retrieved from http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/documents/BarrANGxp.pdfGoogle Scholar
Bellieni, C. V., Sisto, R., Cordelli, D. M., & Buonocore, G. (2004). Cry features reflect pain intensity in term newborns: An alarm threshold. Pediatric Research, 55, 142146. http://dx.doi.org/10.1203/01.PDR.0000099793.99608.CBCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. (1994). Measuring emotion: The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 4959. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. (2000). Affective reactions to acoustic stimuli. Psychophysiology, 37, 204215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3720204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cecchini, M., Lai, C., & Langher, V. (2007). Communication and cry in newborns. Infant, Behavior and Development, 30, 655665. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cecchini, M., Lai, C., & Langher, V. (2010). Disphonic newborn cries allow prediction of their perceived meaning. Infant, Behavior and Development, 33, 314320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, K. D., Prkachin, K. M., & Grunau, R. V. E. (2001). The facial expression of pain. In Turk, D. C. & Melzack, R. (Eds.), Handbook of pain assessment (2nd Ed., pp. 153169). New York, NY: Guilford.Google Scholar
Del Vecchio, T., Walter, A. L., & O'Leary, S. G. (2009). Affective and physiological factors predicting maternal response to infant crying. Infant Behavior and Development, 32, 117122. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delevati, N. M., & Bergamasco, N. H. P. (1999). Pain in the neonate: An analysis of facial movements and cry in response to nociceptive stimuli. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 137143. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(99)80011-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C. (2002) Facial Action Coding System (FACS). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Fernández-Abascal, E. G., Martínez-Sánchez, F., & Chóliz, M. (2009). Guía de la expresión del bebé [Guide of the Expression of Babies]. Madrid, Spain: Roche.Google Scholar
Fox, N., & Davidson, R. J. (1988). Patterns of brain electrical activity during facial signs of emotion in 10 month old infants. Developmental Psychology, 24, 230236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.24.2.230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galati, D., & Lavelli, M. (1997). Neonate and infant emotion expression perceived by adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 5783. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024999818682CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goberman, A., & Robb, M. (1999). Acoustic examination of pre-term and full-term infant cries: The long-time average spectrum. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 850861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, J. A., Gustafson, G. E., & McGuie, A. C. (1998). Changes in infants' cries as a function of time in a cry bout. Child Development, 69, 271279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06187.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, J. A., Jones, L. E., & Gustafson, G. E. (1987). Perception of cries by parents and nonparents: Relation to cry acoustics. Developmental Psychology, 23, 370382. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.3.370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, M. K., Cook, E. W., & Lang, P. J. (1989). Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli. Journal of Psychophysiology, 3, 5164.Google Scholar
Irwin, J. R. (2003). Parent and nonparent perception of the multimodal infant cry. Infancy 4, 503516. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0404_06CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: Computer applications. In Sidowski, J. B. Johnson, J. H., & Williams, T. A. (Eds.), Technology in mental health care delivery systems (pp. 119137). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Leeds, L., & Thompson, J. (2005). Tears as tools: How to learn from crying. Early Years: Journal of the Texas Association for the Education of Young Children, 27, 56.Google Scholar
Long, T., & Johnson, M. (2001). Living and coping with excessive infantile crying. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 34, 155162. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2648.2001.01740.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lummaa, V., Vuorisalo, T., Bard, R. G., & Lehtonen, L. (1998). Why cry?: Adaptive significance of intensive crying in human infants. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 193202. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00014-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, C. R., Hull, J. G., Edwards, W. H., Cramer, C. P., & Dewey, W. L. (1993). Neonatal pain: A comprehensive survey of practices. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 8, 716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0885-3924(93)90114-BCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeil, D. W., & Brunetti, D. G. (1992). Pain and fear: A bioinformational perspective on responsivity to imagery. Behavior Research Therapy, 30, 513520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(92)90035-FCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messinger, D. S. (2002). Positive and negative: Infant facial expressions and emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A., Levin, D. N., Kozak, M. J., Cook, E. W. III, McLean, A., & Lang, P. J. (1987). Individual differences in imagery and the psychophysiology of emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 367390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699938708408058CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. D. (2007). Neural circuits underlying crying and cry responding in mammals. Behavioural Brain Research, 182, 155165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2007.02.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oster, H., Hegley, D., & Nagel, L. (1992). Adult judgments and fine-grained analysis of infant facial expressions: Testing the validity of a priori coding formulas. Developmental Psychology, 28, 11151131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.6.1115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prkachin, K. M. (1992). The consistency of facial expressions of pain: A comparison across modalities. Pain, 51, 297306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(92)90213-UCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Protopapas, A., & Lieberman, P. (1997). Fundamental frequency of phonation and perceived emotional stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 101, 22672277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1121/1.418247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rautava, L., Lempinen, A., Ojala, S., Parkkola, R., Rikalainen, H., Lapinleimu, H., … Lehtonen, L. (2007). Acoustic quality of cry in very-low-birth-weight infants at the age of 1 1/2 years. Early Human Development, 83, 512. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.03.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robb, M. P., Crowell, D. H., Dunn-Rankin, P., & Tinsley, C. (2007). Cry features in siblings of SIDS. Acta Paediatrica, 96, 14041408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00486.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runefors, P., & Arnbjörnsson, E. (2005). A sound spectrogram analysis of children's crying after painful stimuli during the first year of life. Pholia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica, 57, 9095. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000083570CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soltis, J. (2004). The developmental mechanisms and the signal functions of early infant crying. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 443458. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0400010XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, A. C. (2002). Facial expression of pain: An evolutionary account. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 439455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X02000080Google ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. H. (1969). The natural history of crying and other vocalizations in early infancy. In Foss, B. (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 81109). London, England: Mathuen.Google Scholar
Zeifman, D. M. (2001). An ethological analysis of human infant crying: Answering Tinbergen's four questions. Developmental Psychobiology 39, 265285. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.1005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeifman, D. M. (2003). Predicting adult responses to infant Distress: Adult characteristics associated with perceptions, emotional reactions, and timing of intervention. Infant Mental Health Journal, 24, 597612. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10077CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeskind, P. S., Marshall, T. R., & Goff, D. M. (1996). Cry threshold predicts regulatory disorder in newborn infants. Pediatric Psychology, 21, 803819. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/21.6.803CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed