Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-07T21:43:19.475Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Anti-stigma campaigns: Time to change

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Michael smith*
Affiliation:
William Street Clinic, 120–130 William Street, Glasgow G3 8UR, UK. Email: michael.smith2@ggc.scot.nhs.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

This evaluation of the Time to Change anti-stigma campaign in England represents a milestone in international stigma research. While showing some positive outcomes, the overall picture is mixed and falls short of the wholesale shift in attitudes that is needed. A new approach is proposed for the coming decades.

Type
Editorials
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2013

‘If you want truly to understand something, try to change it.’

Kurt Lewin

Elaine Cumming was a sociologist, and her husband John a psychiatrist. In 1951, they sought to change attitudes to mental illness by implementing a programme of community ‘mental health education’. Reference Cumming and Cumming1 Based in a small prairie town in Canada, their pioneering anti-stigma programme used information leaflets, radio broadcasts, school programmes and engagement with community groups to convey to the townspeople three key principles about mental health. The first of these was that ‘behaviour is caused, and therefore understandable and subject to change’; the second that ‘there is a continuum between normality and abnormality’; and the third that ‘there is a wider variety of normal behaviour than is generally realised’.

The project failed. The ‘six-month educational program, in its all-out attempt to improve attitudes toward mental illness, produced virtually no change in the general orientation of the population either toward the social problem of mental illness or toward the mentally ill themselves’. Reference Cumming and Cumming1 Twenty-five years later, a replication study confirmed no change in attitudes. Reference Stuart2

What went wrong? Contrary to expectations, townspeople accepted (or ‘normalised’) a broader range of behaviour than did the researchers. Lay people had their own explanations for human behaviour, which were rooted in their knowledge of peoples' life histories and which they were reluctant to reconsider. Most critically, the townspeople were convinced that there was a sharp dividing line between mental illness and normality and repelled efforts to change that view.

These findings have not dimmed the enthusiasm of professionals to ‘educate’ the public: quite the opposite. In 1996, the World Psychiatric Association launched its ‘Open the Doors’ campaign against the stigma of schizophrenia. Large-scale public campaigns followed in several countries: New Zealand (‘Like Minds, Like Mine’, 1997), Scotland (‘see me’, 2002); the USA (‘What a Difference a Friend Makes’, 2006), England (‘Time to Change’, 2007), Canada (‘Opening Minds’, 2009) and several others in Europe and elsewhere.

Change is difficult

The English Time to Change campaign consolidated much of this experience into a best practice model which targeted knowledge, attitudes and behaviour. The campaign was well funded (close to £21 million for the 4 years to 2011), led by major mental health charities, had clear objectives, and was subject to the rigorous evaluation published in this supplement. Separate papers describe the influence of the programme on employers, Reference Henderson, Williams, Little and Thornicroft3 newspapers, Reference Thornicroft, Goulden, Shefer, Rhydderch, Rose and Williams4 medical students Reference Friedrich, Evans-Lacko, London, Rhydderch, Henderson and Thornicroft5 and the general public. Reference Evans-Lacko, Henderson and Thornicroft6 This supplement also describes an economic evaluation, Reference Evans-Lacko, Henderson, Thornicroft and McCrone7 and the experience of discrimination by people using mental health services. Reference Corker, Hamilton, Henderson, Weeks, Pinfold and Rose8 The intervention and outcomes are summarised in the accompanying editorial by Henderson & Thornicroft. Reference Evans-Lacko, Henderson, Thornicroft and McCrone9

The results reveal a mixed picture. Some parameters showed a positive change, such as a small reduction in discrimination reported by service users and improved employer recognition of common mental health problems. Some parameters showed an initial improvement which subsequently receded - confirming, for example, that improvements in medical students' attitudes are typically short lived.

Other measures did not change. Disappointingly, there was no improvement in knowledge or behaviour among the general public, nor in user reports of discrimination by mental health professionals. A few indicators showed a negative change over the study period, ironically including increased discrimination reported by those with most exposure to the Time to Change programme (albeit from survey response rates of only 6-8%). It is not clear whether reports of increased discrimination represent a negative finding (due to adverse user experience) or a positive one (because users have greater awareness or confidence about speaking out).

Although economic analysis suggests a likely benefit from the programme, variation in the assumptions used in the model leaves a wide range of uncertainty: from a net cost to a benefit of £223 million.

Increase the dose or change the treatment?

The scale, coherence and rigour of this work is exemplary, and it represents a milestone in international stigma research. This evaluation comes at a time when campaigners are reflecting critically on the lessons learnt from similar campaigns in recent years. Reference Stuart, Arboleda-Florez and Sartorius10 The fantasy shared (though rarely voiced) by many campaigners might have run something like this:

‘if mental health workers truly have a model which is superior, and not just novel, this wonderful news will travel. When this happens, they will be invited into the community, as experts, to teach what they know, and they will find that the ranks have opened to welcome them.’ Reference Siegier and Osmond11

Sadly, that did not happen. We have made gains, not least in turning a ‘non-issue’ into an issue, but such change has been slow, patchy and vulnerable. This leaves anti-stigma campaigns with a critical strategic dilemma: do we increase the dose (more time, more advertising, more contact) or change the treatment? If the latter, what might such a change look like?

Old lessons, new models

Although defined in dictionaries as ‘a mark of disgrace’, stigma occurs in a complex mesh of relationships involving labelling, stereotyping, separation, status loss, discrimination and power imbalances. Reference Link and Phelan12 These processes occur at an individual level and in whole populations; affect some people with mental illness but not all; are poorly correlated with prejudicial attitudes; Reference Schütz and Six13 and seem to disempower some individuals while energising others. Reference Corrigan and Watson14 Considered in this way, stigma is not really a ‘mark’ at all: it is a dynamic, relational process.

If so, it might be helpful to distinguish between stigma arising within individual relationships and that which emerges from whole populations. There are similarities here with economics. ‘Macroeconomic’ factors (such as unemployment and inflation) must be based on the ‘microeconomic’ decisions made in shops and workplaces. Yet ‘macro’ is not the same as more ‘micro’: economists recognise that new, ‘emergent’ properties arise from the complexity of interactions between the system components. Reference Keen15 It might be useful to think about stigma and discrimination in a similar way.

At the ‘micro’ level, researchers need to better understand the nature of the events that are reported as stigmatising. It is common, for example, for people who have harmed themselves to report stigmatising attitudes expressed by emergency department staff. Reference Hawton, Taylor, Saunders and Mahadevan16 Current conceptualisation of that adverse experience considers the shame felt by the service user, and presumes a deficit or dysfunction in the knowledge and attitudes of staff. It does not acknowledge the anger, frustration and despair brought to that consultation from outside, and is therefore blind to the potential for transference and countertransference reactions. The presumption of prejudice by staff does not permit an exploration of their perspective, which might include the defensive function of emotional distance from patients, or a fear of the medico-legal consequences of a wrong decision.

If stigma arises within relationships, we need to hear from both sides. We should be aware of the risk of ‘stigmatising the stigmatiser’ for behaviours that may be perceived as discriminatory, but which have complex origins that we need to understand.

These are not new insights. As the Cummings wrote 60 years ago: ‘one of our errors […] lay in our latent assumption that we knew the content of […] people's attitudes toward human behaviour and its causes’. They recommended that future researchers should ‘give attention to the function which these [bad] attitudes had for those who held them’. At the ‘macro’ level, our understanding of the ebb and flow of population attitudes and opinions is very limited. Worse, a complex adaptive system of this kind will tend to nullify intervention, evade investigation and confound interpretation. Reference Plsek and Greenhalgh17 There is, therefore, much ‘dark matter’ influencing stigma at a population level that we still cannot see. The following analogy might apply:

‘Let's say that we are trying to measure tide height at the beach. We know that the sea is filled with fish, and so we exhaustively model fish behaviour, developing complex models of their movements and interactions […] The model is great. And the model is useless. The behaviour of the fish is irrelevant for the question we are interested in: how high will the seawater go up the beach? […] we are focusing on the fish when we should be studying the moon.’ (Cecchetti, quoted in Pettifor Reference Pettifor18 )

The work of Thornicroft and colleagues is impressive and reassuring, because they show that systematic, insightful and multifaceted campaigns can be implemented and tested at scale. But it also highlights a finding that we have to face up to: the wholesale shift in attitudes that we all want to see has yet to occur. We should resist the temptation to presume that ‘more of the same’ might work in future, since the Cummings remind us that we have been trying that now for 60 years. I hope that Time to Change can live up to its name and provoke a rethink not just for the public, but for anti-stigma campaigns as well.

References

1 Cumming, E, Cumming, J. Closed Ranks: An Experiment in Mental Health Education. Harvard University Press, 1957.Google Scholar
2 Stuart, H. Fighting stigma and discrimination is fighting for mental health. Can Publ Policy 2005; 31: s218.Google Scholar
3 Henderson, C, Williams, P, Little, K, Thornicroft, G. Mental health problems in the workplace: changes in employers' knowledge, attitudes and practices in England 2006-2010. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202 (suppl 55): s706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
4 Thornicroft, A, Goulden, R, Shefer, G, Rhydderch, D, Rose, D, Williams, P, et al Newspaper coverage of mental illness in England 2008-2011. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202 (suppl 55): s649.Google Scholar
5 Friedrich, B, Evans-Lacko, S, London, J, Rhydderch, D, Henderson, C, Thornicroft, G. Anti-stigma training for medical students: the Education Not Discrimination project. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202 (suppl 55): s8994.Google Scholar
6 Evans-Lacko, S, Henderson, C, Thornicroft, G. Public knowledge, attitudes and behaviour regarding people with mental illness in England 2009-2012. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202 (suppl 55): s517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7 Evans-Lacko, S, Henderson, C, Thornicroft, G, McCrone, P. Economic evaluation of the Time to Change anti-stigma social marketing campaign in England 2009-2011. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202 (suppl 55): s95101.Google Scholar
8 Corker, E, Hamilton, S, Henderson, C, Weeks, C, Pinfold, V, Rose, D, et al Experiences of discrimination among people using mental health services in England 2008-2011. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202 (suppl 55): s5863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9 Evans-Lacko, S, Henderson, C, Thornicroft, G, McCrone, P. Economic evaluation of the Time to Change anti-stigma social marketing campaign in England 2009-2011. Br J Psychiatry 2013; 202 (suppl 55): s95101.Google Scholar
10 Stuart, H, Arboleda-Florez, J, Sartorius, N. Paradigms Lost: Fighting Stigma and the Lessons Learned: 304. Oxford University Press, 2012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11 Siegier, M, Osmond, H. ‘Closed Ranks’ twenty years later. Orthomolecular Psychiatry 1973; 2: 150–63.Google Scholar
12 Link, BG, Phelan, JC. Conceptualizing stigma. Ann Rev Sociology 2001; 27: 363–85.Google Scholar
13 Schütz, H, Six, B. How strong is the relationship between prejudice and discrimination? A meta-analytic answer. Int J Intercult Relations 1996; 20: 441–62.Google Scholar
14 Corrigan, PW, Watson, AC. The paradox of self-stigma and mental illness. Clin Psychol Sci Practice 2002; 9: 3553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15 Keen, S. Debunking Economics (Revised and Expanded Edition): 472. Zed Books, 2011.Google Scholar
16 Hawton, K, Taylor, T, Saunders, K, Mahadevan, S. clinical care of deliberate self-harm patients: an evidence-based approach. In International Handbook of Suicide Prevention Research, Policy and Practice: 329–52. John Wiley & Sons, 2011.Google Scholar
17 Plsek, PE, Greenhalgh, T. Complexity science: the challenge of complexity in health care. BMJ 2001; 323: 625–8.Google Scholar
18 Pettifor, A. The power to ‘create money out of thin air’. Open Democracy 2013; 18 January (http://www.opendemocracy.net/ourkingdom/ann-pettifor/power-to-create-money-out-of-thin-air).Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.