Hostname: page-component-788cddb947-rnj55 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-08T19:34:08.831Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mental health literacy

Public knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

A. F. Jorm*
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Research, The Australian National University, Canberra 0200, Australia
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

Background

Although the benefits of public knowledge of physical diseases are widely accepted, knowledge about mental disorders (mental health literacy) has been comparatively neglected.

Aims

To introduce the concept of mental health literacy to a wider audience, to bring together diverse research relevant to the topic and to identify gaps in the area.

Method

A narrative review within a conceptual framework.

Results

Many members of the public cannot recognise specific disorders or different types of psychological distress. They differ from mental health experts in their beliefs about the causes of mental disorders and the most effective treatments. Attitudes which hinder recognition and appropriate help-seeking are common. Much of the mental health information most readily available to the public is misleading. However, there is some evidence that mental health literacy can be improved.

Conclusions

If the public's mental health literacy is not improved, this may hinder public acceptance of evidence-based mental health care. Also, many people with common mental disorders may be denied effective self-help and may not receive appropriate support from others in the community.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2000 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Health literacy has been defined as “the ability to gain access to, understand, and use information in ways which promote and maintain good health” (Reference Nutbeam, Wise and BaumanNutbeam et al, 1993). In the area of physical health, examples of health literacy would include knowledge and use of a healthy diet, taking actions to prevent skin cancer, performing breast self-examination, having first aid skills and knowing how to look up health information in a library or on the internet. While the importance of health literacy for physical health is widely acknowledged, the area of mental health literacy has been comparatively neglected. The purpose of this review is to introduce the concept to a wider audience, to bring together diverse research relevant to mental health literacy and to identify gaps in the area.

DEFINITION AND CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK

Jorm et al (Reference Jorm, Korten and Jacomb1997a ) introduced the term ‘mental health literacy’ and have defined it as “ knowledge and beliefs about mental disorders which aid their recognition, management or prevention”. Mental health literacy consists of several components, including: (a) the ability to recognise specific disorders or different types of psychological distress; (b) knowledge and beliefs about risk factors and causes; (c) knowledge and beliefs about self-help interventions; (d) knowledge and beliefs about professional help available; (e) attitudes which facilitate recognition and appropriate help-seeking; and (f) knowledge of how to seek mental health information.

If people experience disabling psychological symptoms or have close contact with others who have such problems, they will attempt to manage those symptoms. People's symptom-management activities will be influenced by their mental health literacy. If successful, these symptom-management activities may lead to a reduction in disabling symptoms and also a change in mental health literacy. In this framework, the person affected by the symptoms (either personally or through close contact) is seen as the primary agent in symptom management, with professional help being one of a range of strategies he or she might try. This perspective is important because it leads to a greater emphasis on increasing public (rather than professional) knowledge and skills about mental health and on empowering the person experiencing disabling symptoms. The need for the public to have greater mental health literacy is highlighted by the high lifetime prevalence of mental disorders (up to 50%, according to Reference Kessler, McGonagle and ZhaoKessler et al, 1994), which means that virtually everyone will either develop a mental disorder or have close contact with someone who does.

RECOGNITION OF MENTAL DISORDERS

Many members of the public cannot correctly recognise mental disorders and do not understand the meanings of psychiatric terms. For example, when a representative sample of the Australian public was shown vignettes of a person suffering from major depression or schizophrenia, most recognised that there was some sort of mental health problem but depression was correctly used as the label by only 39% and schizophrenia by 27% (Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997a ). For the depression vignette, 11% thought the person had a physical disorder. Similarly, European surveys have found lack of understanding of the terms schizophrenia and mania to be common (Reference Brändli, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusBrändli, 1999; Reference Hillert, Sandmann, Ehmig, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusHillert et al, 1999) and that ‘schizophrenia’ is commonly associated with a split personality (Reference Angermeyer, Matschinger, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1999). A US study found that the public are reasonably knowledgeable about the mood symptoms of depression, but less likely to know about somatic changes (Reference Regier, Hirschfeld and GoodwinRegier et al, 1988).

Is the inability to use a correct psychiatric label and lack of knowledge of symptomatology of any significance? These failures of mental health literacy may cause problems of communication with health practitioners. It is well known that patients with mental disorders are often missed by general practitioners (GPs). Aspects of the GP interviewing style are known to be associated with rate of detection (Reference Goldberg and HuxleyGoldberg & Huxley, 1992), but the patient's mode of interacting with the GP is also important. Detection of a mental disorder is greater if the patient presents his or her symptoms as reflecting a psychological problem (Reference Herran, Vázquez-Barquero and DunnHerran et al, 1999; Reference Kessler, Lloyd and LewisKessler et al, 1999) and explicitly raises the problem with the GP (Reference Bowers, Jorm and HendersonBowers et al, 1990; Reference Jacob, Bhugra and LloydJacob et al, 1998). Although GP recognition may not be sufficient in itself to benefit the patient (Reference Goldberg, Privett and UstunGoldberg et al, 1998; Reference Simon, Goldberg and TiemensSimon et al, 1999), it is a first step towards effective action.

KNOWLEDGE AND BELIEFS ABOUT CAUSES

In Western countries depression and schizophrenia are most often seen by the public as caused by the social environment, particularly recent stressors (Reference McKeon and CarrickMcKeon & Carrick, 1991; Reference Matschinger and AngermeyerMatschinger & Angermeyer, 1996; Reference Priest, Vize and RobertsPriest et al, 1996; Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997b ; Reference Link, Phelan and BresnahanLink et al, 1999). While psychiatric epidemiologists would concur about the importance of stressful life events in depression, in schizophrenia life events are more of a trigger than a cause. Biological factors are seen by the public as less important than environmental ones (Reference McKeon and CarrickMcKeon & Carrick, 1991; Reference Matschinger and AngermeyerMatschinger & Angermeyer, 1996; Reference Wolff, Pathare and CraigWolff et al, 1996; Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997b ; Reference Link, Phelan and BresnahanLink et al, 1999), although relatives of people with schizophrenia are more likely to see biological factors as important (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996a ). Providing the label ‘schizophrenia’ to a vignette has also been found to increase the likelihood that biological rather than psychosocial causes are seen as responsible (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996b ). In some non-Western cultures, supernatural phenomena, such as witchcraft and possession by evil spirits, are seen as important causes of mental disorders (Reference Razali, Khan and HasanahRazali et al, 1996), although this is uncommon in the West (Reference Angermeyer, Matschinger, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1999; Reference Brändli, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusBrändli, 1999).

Beliefs about causes may alter patterns of help-seeking and response to treatment. For example, in Malaysia belief by psychiatric patients in supernatural causes was associated with greater use of traditional healers and poorer compliance with medication (Reference Razali, Khan and HasanahRazali et al, 1996). In a US controlled trial of psychotherapy for depression, belief in relationship causes was associated with a better outcome in behavioural therapy, while belief in existential causes was associated with a better outcome in cognitive therapy (Reference Addis and JacobsonAddis & Jacobson, 1996).

KNOWLEDGE AND BELIEFS ABOUT SELF-HELP

Given that only a minority or people who meet diagnostic criteria for a mental disorder seek professional help (Reference Regier, Narrow and RaeRegier et al, 1993; Reference Lin, Goering and OffordLin et al, 1996; Reference Andrews, Hall and TeessonAndrews et al, 1999), self-help skills are of great importance. When the public were asked to rate a range of interventions for likely helpfulness, self-help interventions were found to be at the top of the list in both Australia and the UK (Reference RippereRippere, 1979; Reference Parker and BrownParker & Brown, 1982; Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997a ). Among the most popular self-help interventions are seeking support from family and friends, engaging in pleasurable activities, taking up new activities and physical exercise. Unfortunately, there is much less evidence on the effectiveness of self-help interventions than on that of professional ones, making it difficult to say which are likely to work. However, for milder states of depression, there is evidence for the effectiveness of social support (Reference Goldberg and HuxleyGoldberg & Huxley, 1992), physical exercise (Reference MartinsenMartinsen, 1994), self-help books based on cognitive-behavioural therapy (Reference CuijpersCuijpers, 1997) and for the herb St John's wort (Reference Linde, Ramirez and MulrowLinde et al, 1996). Rauwolfia serpentina is a traditional herbal remedy known to have an antipsychotic effect (Reference Bhatara, Sharma and GuptaBhatara et al, 1997). There is a clear need for further evaluation of self-help interventions, so that the public can be given appropriate advice.

Knowledge of how to help others is a related component of mental health literacy. Very little research has been done on the topic, but a Swiss survey found that the public have difficulty in dealing with mental disorders, saying they do not know how to behave, are afraid of making mistakes and do not have sufficient knowledge (Reference Brändli, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusBrändli, 1999).

KNOWLEDGE AND BELIEFS ABOUT PROFESSIONAL HELP

A number of surveys have asked the public about various helping professions. General practitioners are rated very highly in many countries, particularly for depression (Reference Priest, Vize and RobertsPriest et al, 1996; Reference Wolff, Pathare and CraigWolff et al, 1996; Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997a ; Reference Brändli, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusBrädli, 1999). The strong endorsement of GPs is not, however, universal (Reference McKeon and CarrickMcKeon & Carrick, 1991; Reference Jorm, Angermeyer, Katschnig, Andrews and HendersonJorm et al, 2000a ). For depression, psychiatrists and psychologists are rated less highly than GPs, but are more likely to be seen as helpful for schizophrenia (Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997a ; Reference Angermeyer, Matschinger and Riedel-HellerAngermeyer et al, 1999). These results come from developed countries. Beliefs about professional help may be very different in developing countries. For example, in Ethiopia traditional sources of help, such as witchcraft, holy water and herbalists, were preferred over medical help for a range of mental health problems (Reference Alem, Jacobsson and ArayaAlem et al, 1999). By contrast, medical help was over-whelmingly preferred for physical health problems.

When the public are asked about various therapies, a strikingly consistent finding across many countries is very negative beliefs about medication for a range of mental disorders (Reference Regier, Hirschfeld and GoodwinRegier et al, 1988; Reference Angermeyer, Däumer and MatschingerAngermeyer et al, 1993; Reference Priest, Vize and RobertsPriest et al, 1996; Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997a ; Reference Fischer, Goerg, Zbinden, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusFischer et al, 1999; Reference Hillert, Sandmann, Ehmig, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusHillert et al, 1999; Reference Jorm, Angermeyer, Katschnig, Andrews and HendersonJorm et al, 2000a ). The public's belief about medication is in sharp contrast to both the evidence from randomised controlled trials and the views of mental health professionals that anti-depressant and antipsychotic medications are effective (Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997c ; Reference Caldwell and JormCaldwell & Jorm, 2000). The public's negative views about psychotropic medication also contrast with their own positive views about medication for common physical disorders (Reference Hillert, Sandmann, Ehmig, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusHillert et al, 1999). The reasons given by the public for their negative views of psychotropic medication are perceived side-effects, such as dependence, lethargy or brain damage, and the belief that the treatments deal only with the symptoms and not the causes (Reference Angermeyer, Däumer and MatschingerAngermeyer et al, 1993; Reference Priest, Vize and RobertsPriest et al, 1996; Reference Fischer, Goerg, Zbinden, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusFishcher et al, 1999). One interpretation of these findings is that the negative attributes of benzodiazepines have become generalised to all types of psychotropic medication (Reference Angermeyer, Däumer and MatschingerAngermeyer et al, 1993). Indeed, the public do not seem to discriminate between different types of psychotropic medication, in contrast to mental health professionals who are quite specific in their recommendations (Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997c ). An Australian survey found that other treatments specifically associated with psychiatrists, such as electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) and admission to a psychiatric ward, are also viewed very negatively by the public, with more people believing they are harmful than helpful (Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997a ). Anecdotal evidence would support the conclusion that such beliefs are widespread in other countries as well.

‘Natural’ remedies, such as vitamins and herbs, are viewed much more positively by the public (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996c ; Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997a ) and are not generally seen as sharing the negative attributes of psychotropics (Reference Fischer, Goerg, Zbinden, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusFischer et al, 1999).

Another consistent finding across a range of countries is very positive views about psychological treatments such as counselling. (Reference McKeon and CarrickMcKeon & Carrick, 1991; Reference Priest, Vize and RobertsPriest et al, 1996; Jorm et al, Reference Jorm, Korten and Jacomb1997a , Reference Jorm, Angermeyer, Katschnig, Andrews and Henderson2000a ) and psychotherapy (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996c ; Reference Hillert, Sandmann, Ehmig, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusHillert et al, 1999). Indeed, the public's views tend to be more positive than those of professionals (Reference Furnham, Wardley and LillieFurnham et al, 1992; Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997c ). What is most surprising is that psychological interventions are seen by the public as highly effective for psychotic disorders (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996c ; Jorm et al, Reference Jorm, Korten and Jacomb1997c , Reference Jorm, Angermeyer, Katschnig, Andrews and Henderson2000a ) and even, according to an Austrian survey, for dementia (Reference Jorm, Angermeyer, Katschnig, Andrews and HendersonJorm et al, 2000a ).

What are the consequences of the public's beliefs about treatment? The most obvious is that negative beliefs about medication may lead to failure to seek medical help and lack of compliance with any medication recommended (Reference Fischer, Goerg, Zbinden, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusFischer et al, 1999). It has been proposed that greater account should be taken of patients' views in negotiating the treatment approach. In this regard, the term ‘concordance’, which implies a two-way negotiation between doctor and patient, is more appropriate than ‘compliance’ (Reference MullenMullen, 1997). Public beliefs about professional help may also affect the help-seeking of others. It has been found that professional help for depression is more likely to occur when another person recommends that help be sought (Reference Dew, Bromet and SchulbergDew et al, 1991), so the views of significant others about treatment may also be influential.

ATTITUDES THAT FACILITATE RECOGNITION AND HELP-SEEKING

There is a stigma associated with mental disorders and this may hinder seeking help. For example, the German public report much greater reluctance to discuss mental disorders with relatives and friends than to discuss physical disorders (Reference Hillert, Sandmann, Ehmig, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusHillert et al, 1999), while in the USA many members of the public reported an unwillingness to seek treatment for depression because they feared a negative impact on their employment situation (Reference Regier, Hirschfeld and GoodwinRegier et al, 1988). Stigmatising attitudes also extend to approaching professionals. In the UK, a majority of the public reported that they would be embarrassed to consult a GP for depression, primarily because the GP would see them as unbalanced or neurotic (Reference Priest, Vize and RobertsPriest et al, 1996), and in India patients with stigmatising attitudes have been found to present their distress in somatic rather than psychological terms (Reference Raguram, Weiss and ChannabasavannaRaguram et al, 1996).

KNOWLEDGE OF HOW TO SEEK MENTAL HEALTH INFORMATION

We know very little about how people acquire knowledge and beliefs about mental health. It is likely that personal experiences and anecdotal evidence from family and friends are an important source. A UK study found that 33% of respondents cited personal experience of someone with a mental disorder as their main source of information, while a further 10% cited friends and relatives (Reference Wolff, Pathare and CraigWolff et al, 1996). Indeed, personal experience or contact has been found to be associated with beliefs about causes (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996b ), with more favourable attitudes (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996a ; Reference Wolff, Pathare and CraigWolff et al, 1996), with treatment preferences (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1996a ) and with greater understanding of the term schizophrenia (Reference Hillert, Sandmann, Ehmig, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusHillert et al, 1999).

Other important influences are journalists' reports and television and cinema dramas depicting mental disorders. In the UK, 32% cited the media as their main source of information (Reference Wolff, Pathare and CraigWolff et al, 1996). Unfortunately, these media often tend to report on the negative aspects. In a survey of the German public, 64% said that they had read about a person with a mental illness who had committed a violent crime and 50% about someone who became addicted to prescribed drugs, but only 17% had read about persons with mental illnesses who became able to lead a normal life by taking their medication (Reference Hillert, Sandmann, Ehmig, Guimon, Fischer and SartoriusHillert et al, 1999). It is clear that such negative reporting has an impact. Another German study was able to show that two attempts on the lives of prominent politicians by persons with a mental disorder led to a marked increase in negative attitudes (Reference Angermeyer and MatschingerAngermeyer & Matschinger, 1995). People with mental disorders are also frequently portrayed as violent or having other undesirable characteristics in fictional accounts in the cinema and on television (Reference Hyler, Gabbard and SchneiderHyler et al, 1991; Reference Wilson, Nairn and CoverdaleWilson et al, 1999). Although violence is a problem in a small proportion of people suffering from severe mental disorders (Reference TorreyTorrey, 1994), the public clearly overestimate this risk, on the basis of media reports and dramatic portrayals (Reference Wolff, Pathare and CraigWolff et al, 1996; Reference Link, Phelan and BresnahanLink et al, 1999).

Finally, there are sources of knowledge, such as books, libraries, the internet and courses of study, available to those with better education and resources. While it is known that some self-help books are best-sellers and that some mental health websites receive a large number of hits, the overall impact of such sources on mental health literacy is unknown. There is a need for greater quality control of such sources to ensure that the public gets accurate information. For example, a recent study of the top 20 depression websites found the overall quality of the information was poor when evaluated against clinical practice guidelines (K. Griffiths, personal communication, 2000; further details available from the author upon request).

COGNITIVE ORGANISATION OF MENTAL HEALTH LITERACY

There is a clear gulf between public and professional beliefs about mental disorders (Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997c ). One interpretation of this finding is that there is a continuum of mental health literacy running from lay beliefs to professional knowledge. The professionals have expert knowledge which is to a large extent based on scientific evidence and expert consensus, while the public have a range of beliefs based on personal experience, anecdotes, media reports and more formal sources of knowledge. However, factor analysis of public beliefs reveals not a general factor corresponding to mental health literacy, but a number of factors representing general belief systems that illness is best handled by medical, psychological or lifestyle interventions (Reference Jorm, Korten and JacombJorm et al, 1997d ). It may be that when people are confronted by a health problem they know little about, they fall back on their general belief systems about health (Reference Jorm, Christensen and MedwayJorm et al, 2000b ). For example, if a person has no specific knowledge about depression, they might fall back on a general belief system that health problems are caused by lifestyle and that the solution is to be found in natural remedies and lifestyle changes. These general belief systems then become a scaffold onto which specific knowledge (mental health literacy) is grafted.

IMPROVING MENTAL HEALTH LITERACY

Efforts to improve public knowledge of mental disorders have been much less common than for cancer and heart disease. Nevertheless, a number of approaches have been tried. One is an information campaign targeted at the general population. In the late 1980s, the Americans instituted the Depression Awareness, Recognition and Treatment Program, which aimed to inform both the public and health professionals that depressive disorders are common, serious and treatable (Reference Regier, Hirschfeld and GoodwinRegier et al, 1988). This campaign involved a broad range of educational materials, including television, radio and print advertisements, bookmarks and brochures. This national campaign was coordinated with action in local communities. Its effects are unknown. Another US campaign, begun in the early 1990s, is the National Depression Screening Day (Reference JacobsJacobs, 1995). The aims of this day are to call public attention to depression, to educate the public about symptoms and treatments and to identify individuals who may be unaware they are clinically depressed. This day has resulted in wide-spread media publicity and screening of a large number of people.

In the UK there was the Defeat Depression Campaign run by the Royal College of Psychiatrists and the Royal College of General Practitioners from 1992 to 1996 (Reference Paykel, Hart and PriestPaykel et al, 1998). This campaign aimed to educate the public about depression and its treatment, to encourage earlier treatment-seeking and to reduce the stigma of depression. It included use of radio, television and print media. National surveys carried out at the beginning, middle and end of the campaign showed small but significant changes in the percentage of the public who believe that antidepressants are effective and who would be willing to seek professional help. It is impossible to say whether these changes were solely due to the campaign, but the results are certainly encouraging.

In Norway, there has recently been a campaign in one county aimed at reducing the duration of untreated psychosis by encouraging early help-seeking (Reference JohannessenJohannessen, 1998). As well as targeting the public, this campaign was aimed at health care providers, educators and treatment centres. It involved radio, newspaper, cinema and television advertisements. Public surveys carried out before and after the campaign showed a large increase in knowledge of the terms psychosis and schizophrenia. There is also early evidence that help-seeking behaviour has changed and that the duration of untreated psychosis has decreased.

Another approach is to target specific subgroups of the public. This approach is exemplified by the work of Wolff et al (Reference Wolff, Pathare, Craig, Guimon, Fischer and Sartorius1999), who educated the public in a neighbourhood where a group house for those with mental illnesses was being established. In this study, one such neighbourhood received an education campaign, while another acted as a control. The campaign consisted of an educational package with information sheets and a video, social events to establish contact with the group house, a formal reception and informal discussion sessions. Pre- and post-surveys in the experimental and control neighbourhoods showed only a small effect on public knowledge, but revealed less fear and more social contact with the group house residents in the experimental neighbourhood. Another targeted population subgroup is high-school students. Fairly brief classroom instruction has been found to improve willingness to seek professional help (Reference Battaglia, Coverdale and BushongBattaglia et al, 1990; Reference Esters, Cooker and IttenbachEsters et al, 1998).

Finally, there are attempts to improve the quality of information presented in the media through expert input. Although mental health experts frequently make themselves available for media comment, there is virtually no research on the effects of doing so. However, in an analysis of four media items derived from lay sources and three from psychiatrists, Nairn (Reference Nairn1999) found that the psychiatrists presented mental disorders in a less negative manner, but the journalists tended to undermine their message to produce a more newsworthy story.

IMPLICATIONS FOR MENTAL HEALTH CARE

The evidence reviewed here makes it clear that the public in many countries have poor mental health literacy. There are a number of important consequences of this poor knowledge. First, it may place a limit on the implementation of evidence-based mental health care. Attempts to make clinical practice more evidence-based, such as the Cochrane Collaboration (Reference AdamsAdams, 1995), are founded on the assumption that meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials and the dissemination of clinical practice guidelines are sufficient to improve clinical practice. This approach is very much a top-down one in which it is assumed that the benefits of research will be realised if clinicians can be won over to use evidence-based treatments. However, this approach fails to take account of the views of the public, who are the potential consumers of services. If evidence-based treatments do not accord with public views, people who develop mental disorders may be unwilling to seek those treatments or to adhere to advice given by clinicians (Reference Jorm, Angermeyer, Katschnig, Andrews and HendersonJorm et al, 2000a ). Furthermore, they may burden the health care system by seeking inappropriate services and unnecessary investigations.

A second consequence of poor mental health literacy is that the task of preventing and helping mental disorders is largely confined to professionals. However, the prevalence of mental disorders is so high that the mental health workforce cannot help everyone affected and tends to focus on those with more severe and chronic problems. If there are to be greater gains in prevention, early intervention, self-help and support of others in the community, then we need a ‘mental health literate’ society in which basic knowledge and skills are more widely distributed.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  • The public does not share many of the core beliefs of clinicians with regard to treatment and aetiology of mental disorders.

  • Clinicians may have difficulty in implementing evidence-based mental health care if patients do not believe in the interventions offered.

  • An increase in mental health literacy in the population may assist prevention, early intervention, effective self-help and support of others in the community.

LIMITATIONS

  • The concept of mental health literacy assumes the superiority of expert psychiatric knowledge over lay beliefs.

  • The concept could be criticised for seeing the sufferer's interpretation of his or her condition as less valid.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Suggestions for improving the paper were provided by Betty Kitchener, Kathy Griffiths, Scott Henderson, Jo Medway and Bryan Rodgers.

References

Adams, C. E. (1995) A systematic approach to evaluation of care: the Cochrane Collaboration. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 39, 927931.Google Scholar
Addis, M. E. & Jacobson, N. S. (1996) Reasons for depression and the process and outcome of cognitive–behavioural psychotherapies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 14171424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alem, A., Jacobsson, L., Araya, M., et al (1999) How are mental disorders seen and where is help sought in a rural Ethiopian community? A key informant study in Butajira, Ethiopia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 100, 4047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, G., Hall, W., Teesson, M., et al (1999) The Mental Health of Australians. Canberra: Commonwealth Department of Health and Aged Care.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (1995) Violent attacks on public figures by persons suffering from psychiatric disorders: their effect on the social distance towards the mentally ill. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 245, 159164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (1996a) Relatives' beliefs about the causes of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 93, 199204.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (1996b) The effect of diagnostic labelling on the lay theory regarding schizophrenic disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31, 316320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (1996c) Public attitude towards psychiatric treatment. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 94, 326336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (1996d) The effect of personal experience with mental illness on the attitude towards individuals suffering from mental disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31, 321326.Google Scholar
Angermeyer, M. C. & Matschinger, H. (1999) Social representations of mental illness among the public. In The Image of Madness: The Public Facing Mental Illness and Psychiatric Treatment (ed. Guimon, J., Fischer, W. & Sartorius, N.), pp. 2028. Basel: Karger.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angermeyer, M. C., Däumer, R. & Matschinger, H. (1993) Benefits and risks of psychotropic medication in the eyes of the general public: results of a survey in the Federal Republic of Germany. Pharmacopsychiatry, 26, 114120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angermeyer, M. C., Matschinger, H. & Riedel-Heller, S. G. (1999) Whom to ask for help in case of a mental disorder? Preferences of the lay public. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 202210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Battaglia, J., Coverdale, J. H. & Bushong, C. P. (1990) Evaluation of a mental illness awareness week program in public schools. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 324329.Google ScholarPubMed
Bhatara, V. S., Sharma, J. N., Gupta, S., et al (1997) Rauwolfia serpentina: the first herbal antipsychotic. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 894.Google Scholar
Bowers, J., Jorm, A. F., Henderson, S., et al (1990) General practitioners' detection of depression and dementia in elderly patients. Medical Journal of Australia, 153, 192196.Google Scholar
Brändli, H. (1999) The image of mental illness in Switzerland. In The Image of Madness: The Public Facing Mental Illness and Psychiatric Treatment (eds. Guimon, J., Fischer, W. & Sartorius, N.), pp. 2937. Basel: Karger.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, T. M. & Jorm, A. F. (2000) Mental health nurses' beliefs about interventions for schizophrenia and depression: a comparison with psychiatrists and the public. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuijpers, P. (1997) Bibliotherapy for unipolar depression: a meta-analysis. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 28, 139147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dew, M. A., Bromet, E. J., Schulberg, H. C., et al (1991) Factors affecting service utilization for depression in a white collar population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 26, 230237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esters, L. G., Cooker, P. G. & Ittenbach, R. F. (1998) Effects of a unit of instruction in mental health on rural adolescents' conceptions of mental illness and attitudes about seeking help. Adolescence, 33, 469476.Google ScholarPubMed
Fischer, W., Goerg, D., Zbinden, E., et al (1999) Determining factors and the effects of attitudes towards psychotropic medication. In The Image of Madness: The Public Facing Mental Illness and Psychiatric Treatment (ed. Guimon, J., Fischer, W. & Sartorius, N.), pp. 162186. Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Wardley, Z. & Lillie, F. (1992) Lay theories of psychotherapy, III: Comparing the ratings of lay persons and clinical psychologists. Human Relations, 45, 839858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, D. & Huxley, P. (1992) Common Mental Disorders: A Bio-Social Model. London & New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goldberg, D., Privett, M., Ustun, B., et al (1998) The effects of detection and treatment on the outcome of major depression in primary care: a naturalistic study in 15 cities. British Journal of General Practice, 48, 18401844.Google ScholarPubMed
Herran, A., Vázquez-Barquero, J. L. & Dunn, G. (1999) Patients' attributional style is important factor. British Medical Journal, 318, 1558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillert, A., Sandmann, J., Ehmig, S. C., et al (1999) The general public's cognitive and emotional perception of mental illnesses: an alternative to attitude-research. In The Image of Madness: The Public Facing Mental Illness and Psychiatric Treatment (ed. Guimon, J., Fischer, W. & Sartorius, N.), pp. 5671. Basel: Karger.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyler, S. E., Gabbard, G. O. & Schneider, L. (1991) Homicidal maniacs and narcissistic parasites: stigmatization of mentally ill persons in the movies. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 42, 10441048.Google Scholar
Jacob, K. S., Bhugra, D., Lloyd, K. R., et al (1998) Common mental disorders, explanatory models and consultation behaviour among Indian women living in the UK. Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine, 91, 6671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, D. G. (1995) National Depression Screening Day: educating the public, reaching those in need of treatment, and broadening professional understanding. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 3, 156159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johannessen, J. O. (1998) Early intervention and prevention in schizophrenia: experiences from a study in Stavanger, Norway. Seishin Shinkeigaku Zasshi. Psychiatria et Neurologia Japonica, 100, 511522.Google Scholar
Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., et al (1997a) ‘Mental health literacy’: a survey of the public's ability to recognise mental disorders and their beliefs about the effectiveness of treatment. Medical Journal of Australia, 166, 182186.Google Scholar
Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., et al (1997b) Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for depression and schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 82, 143148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., et al (1997c) Helpfulness of interventions for mental disorders: beliefs of health professionals compared with the general public. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 233237 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., et al (1997d) Belief systems of the general public concerning the appropriate treatments for mental disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32, 468473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorm, A. F., Angermeyer, M. & Katschnig, H. (2000a) Public knowledge of and attitudes to mental disorders: a limiting factor in the optimal use of treatment services. In Unmet Need in Psychiatry (ed. Andrews, G. & Henderson, S.), pp. 399413. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Medway, J., et al (2000b) Public belief systems about the helpfulness of interventions for depression: associations with history of depression and professional help-seeking. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessler, D., Lloyd, K., Lewis, G., et al (1999) Cross sectional study of symptom attribution and recognition of depression and anxiety in primary care. British Medical Journal, 318, 436440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., et al (1994) Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM–III–R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, E., Goering, P., Offord, D. R., et al (1996) The use of mental health services in Ontario: epidemiologic findings. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 41, 572577.Google Scholar
Linde, K., Ramirez, G., Mulrow, C. D., et al (1996) St John's wort for depression – an overview and metaanalysis of randomised controlled trials. British Medical Journal, 313, 253258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Link, B. G., Phelan, J. C., Bresnahan, M., et al (1999) Public conceptions of mental illness: labels, causes, dangerousness, and social distance. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 13281333.Google Scholar
Martinsen, E. W. (1994) Physical activity and depression: clinical experience. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 377 (suppl.), 2327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matschinger, H. & Angermeyer, M. (1996) Lay beliefs about the causes of mental disorders: a new methodological approach. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 31, 309315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKeon, P. & Carrick, S. (1991) Public attitudes to depression: a national survey. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 8, 116121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullen, P. D. (1997) Compliance becomes concordance. British Medical Journal, 314, 691692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nairn, R. (1999) Does the use of psychiatrists as sources of information improve media depictions of mental illness? A pilot study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 33, 583589.Google Scholar
Nutbeam, D., Wise, M., Bauman, A., et al (1993) Goals and Targets for Australia's Health in the Year 2000 and Beyond. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service.Google Scholar
Parker, G. B. & Brown, L. B. (1982) Coping behaviors that mediate between life events and depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 39, 13861391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paykel, E. S., Hart, D. & Priest, R. G. (1998) Changes in public attitudes to depression during the Defeat Depression Campaign. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 519522.Google Scholar
Priest, R. G., Vize, C., Roberts, A., et al (1996) Lay people's attitudes to treatment of depression: results of opinion poll for Defeat Depression Campaign just before its launch. British Medical Journal, 313, 858859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raguram, R., Weiss, M. G., Channabasavanna, S. M., et al (1996) Stigma, depression, and somatization in south India. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 10431049.Google Scholar
Razali, S. M., Khan, U. A. & Hasanah, C. I. (1996) Belief in supernatural causes of mental illness among Malay patients: impact on treatment. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 94, 229233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regier, D. A., Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Goodwin, F. K., et al (1988) The NIMH depression awareness, recognition, and treatment program: structure, aims, and scientific basis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 13511357.Google ScholarPubMed
Regier, D. A., Narrow, W. E., Rae, D. S., et al (1993) The de facto US mental and addictive disorders service system. Epidemiologic catchment area prospective 1-year prevalence rates of disorders and services. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 8594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rippere, V. (1979) Scaling the helpfulness of antidepressive activities. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 17, 439449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, G. E., Goldberg, D., Tiemens, B. G., et al (1999) Outcomes of recognized and unrecognized depression in an international primary care study. General Hospital Psychiatry, 21, 97105.Google Scholar
Torrey, E. F. (1994) Violent behavior by individuals with serious mental illness. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 45, 653662.Google Scholar
Wilson, C., Nairn, R., Coverdale, J., et al (1999) Mental illness depictions in prime-time drama: identifying the discursive resources. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 33, 232239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, G., Pathare, S., Craig, T., et al (1996) Community knowledge of mental illness and reaction to mentally ill people. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 191198.Google Scholar
Wolff, G., Pathare, S., Craig, T., et al (1999) Public education for community care: a new approach. In The Image of Madness: The Public Facing Mental Illness and Psychiatric Treatment (ed. Guimon, J., Fischer, W. & Sartorius, N.), pp. 105117. Basel: Karger.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.